Está en la página 1de 5

C O M M E N T A R Y

DYNAMIC-MATURATIONAL MODEL OF ATTACHMENT AND ADAPTATION VERSUS


ABC+D ASSESSMENTS OF ATTACHMENT IN CHILD PROTECTION AND TREATMENT:
REPLY TO VAN IJZENDOORN, BAKERMANS, STEELE, & GRANQVIST (2018)

PATRICIA M. CRITTENDEN
Family Relations Institute, Miami, Florida

SUSAN J. SPIEKER
University of Washington

ABSTRACT: In this brief response, we commend the commentary authors for joining a dialogue about the future of individual differences in attachment
both around person-specific forensic and clinical issues also around working together to develop theory and coding practices. We point to several areas
of explicit and implicit agreement and discuss several misunderstandings. We close with a proposal for future work together, possibly using the only
set of video-recorded Strange Situations classified by Mary Ainsworth as a starting point from which we can explore alternative means of extending
and expanding her work.

RESUMEN: En esta breve respuesta, aplaudimos a los autores del comentario por haberse unido al diálogo acerca del futuro de las diferencias
individuales en la afectividad, tanto en términos de los asuntos forenses y clı́nicos de una persona especı́fica, como en términos de trabajar en conjunto
para desarrollar prácticas teóricas y de codificación. Señalamos varias áreas en las que se dan acuerdos implı́citos y explı́citos y discutimos varios
malentendidos. Terminamos con una propuesta para el trabajo en conjunto en el futuro, posiblemente usando el único grupo de videograbaciones de la
Situación Extraña clasificada por Mary Ainsworth como un punto a partir del cual podemos explorar maneras alternativas de extender y expandir el
trabajo de Ainsworth.

RÉSUMÉ: Dans cette courte réponse, nous saluons la contribution des auteurs de commentaires pour s’être joints à un dialogue sur le futur des
différences individuelles dans l’attachement à la fois pour ce qui concerne l’expertise médico-légale et les problèmes cliniques ainsi que le travail de
concertation pour développer des théories et des pratiques de codage. Nous mettons en évidence certains domaines d’accord explicite et implicite et
discutons plusieurs malentendus. Nous concluons avec un projet de travail commun, qui pourrait utiliser le seul ensemble de Situations Etranges filmées
en vidéo par Mary Ainsworth comme point de départ à partir duquel nous pouvons explorer des moyens alternatifs d’agrandir et d’étendre le travail de
Ainsworth.

ZUSAMMENFASSUNG: In dieser kurzen Stellungnahme empfehlen wir den Kommentatoren, sich an einem Dialog über die Zukunft individueller Bindung-
sunterschiede sowohl hinsichtlich personenbezogener forensischer und klinischer Fragestellungen als auch in Bezug auf die Zusammenarbeit bei der
Entwicklung von Theorie und Kodierungspraktiken zu beteiligen. Wir weisen auf mehrere Themenfelder mit expliziter und impliziter Übereinstimmung
hin und diskutieren einige Missverständnisse. Abschließend schlagen wir eine zukünftige Zusammenarbeit vor, bei der möglicherweise die per Video
aufgezeichneten Fremden Situationen, die von Mary Ainsworth klassifiziert wurden als Ausgangspunkt verwendet werden, von dem aus wir alternative
Wege zur Erweiterung und zum Ausbau der Arbeit von Ainsworth erforschen können.

We thank Mark Baumann, J.D., for verifying Family Court standards.


Direct correspondence to: Patricia M. Crittenden, Family Relations Institute, Inc., 9481 SW 147 Street, Miami, FL 33176; e-mail: crittenden@patcrittenden.com.

INFANT MENTAL HEALTH JOURNAL, Vol. 39(6), 647–651 (2018)


C 2018 The Authors. Infant Mental Health Journal Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. on behalf of Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health

DOI: 10.1002/imhj.21750
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is
properly cited.
647
648 • P.M. Crittenden and S.J. Spieker

dd:ddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
ddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddddd
dddddd–ddddddddddddddddd

dd: ddddddddd, dddddddd, dddddddddddddddddddddddd, dddddddddddd


ddddddd dddddddddddddddd, dddddddd ddddd, dddddddddddddd, dddMary
AinsworthddddddddddStrange Situationsdddd, dddddddddAinsworthdddddddd

* * *

We are pleased to have Drs. van IJzendoorn, Bakermans, history. To clarify, the assessments should be classified blindly, but
Steele, and Granqvist join a dialogue with us and other Dynamic- interpreted in the context of other family attachment relationships
Maturational Model of Attachment and Adaptation (DMM) re- and the family’s history of exposure to danger and, where relevant,
searchers about the relative contributions of the DMM and ABC+D other professionals’ reports and experience of working with family
attachment to case-specific assessment of attachment for young members.
children and their families in forensic and clinical settings.
Granqvist et al. (2017) provided an opening to begin a conver- “Disorganization” is not suitable for case-specific reporting. We
sation about advancing attachment theory and applications. We also agree that the construct of disorganization does not have suf-
recognize that years of hard work on a model can result in strong ficient construct or empirical validity for use in person-specific
feelings when the model displays limitations and, especially, when cases, including forensic and clinical applications.
others point these out. Nevertheless, we think that the topics under
discussion are too important to be framed by researchers’ feelings
and commend our ABC+D colleagues for responding with their Implicit Agreement
commentary. Ambivalent-passive (C2) has been overlooked in ABC+D clas-
In this response, we will address three points: areas of ex- sifications. In addition, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans, Steele, and
plicit and tacit agreement, DMM issues that might have been mis- Granqvist (2018) tacitly accepted that the ABC+D classifica-
understood, and steps that can be taken to address important core tory method has overlooked and likely misclassified as B4 the
differences. ambivalent-passive (C2) category. Further, ABC+D researchers
have often mislabeled Type C as resistant rather than using
Ainsworth’s, Blehar, Waters, and Wall’s (1978) selected term am-
AREAS OF AGREEMENT bivalent, with resistant (C1) and passive (C2) subtypes.
Explicit Agreement
The need to see attachment figures in the Strange Situation Pro-
Attachment classifications are not evidence of maltreatment. First cedure (SSP) video. Similarly, our point that infant/child behavior
and possibly most important, researchers using both the DMM cannot be understood when maternal behavior is not visible points
and ABC+D models of attachment agree that classifications of to a problem in many video-recorded Strange Situations: They do
attachment of parents and children do not constitute evidence of not include the mother, thus depending heavily on the morphol-
maltreatment and should never be used to support or refute charges ogy of infant behavior without having the possibility to understand
of abuse, neglect, or sexual abuse. the interpersonal function of the behavior—for each dyad. The
unfortunate result is that many researchers and clinicians think
Attachment classifications are not stand-alone information. Sec- of attachment as a characteristic of the infant/child rather than
ond, both groups agree that classifications of attachment are not as a description of the protective aspect of the child–attachment
stand-alone information and should always be presented to others figure relationship. The solution, of course, is to always include
in the context of both assessments of other family members’ attach- the attachment figure in the filming, especially during departures,
ment and also history of the family, often including the parents’ life reunions, and contact or refusal of contact.

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
DMM vs. ABC+D Reply • 649

Three, not four, patterns of attachment. Finally, the commentary equivalence of classifications from different laboratories. The
did not address the issue of whether there are three or four patterns DMM uses standardized tests and, for the SSP, video-recordings
of attachment. Because the empirical data are clear, we take this classified by Mary Ainsworth from her own laboratory form the
as tacit agreement that Ainsworth’s three patterns of attachment basis of both our teaching set and the test; these ensure that DMM
are the patterns of attachment. That does not, however, resolve the coders retain the meanings given by Ainsworth. We are not aware
discussion of how to conceptualize the many cases that do not fit of any other set of SSPs classified by Ainsworth. We have offered
Ainsworth’s et al. (1978) model for 11-month-olds. Are there other these SSPs to the 42 signatories to the Granqvist et al. (2017) ar-
forms of A and C? Are there variations in degree of organization? ticle, but have received no response. We think that these videos
Do psychological traumas and pervasive hyper- or hypo-arousal could be a good starting point for a discussion of the cases that do
affect the three patterns? The exploration of these ideas will be not fit Ainsworth’s original method.
exciting and, hopefully, can be carried out with insights from both As far as person-specific clinical and forensic reports are con-
approaches to attachment theory. cerned, DMM coders with Level I forensic reliability (the highest
of four levels) are required; these coders have passed the reliability
Security has been overestimated in ABC+D distributions. We test twice at 85% or better and have completed three approved
made the point that epidemiologically, 25 to 30% of Western pop- family functional formulations (of the sort that would be in a
ulations warrant psychiatric diagnosis and that this supports the court report or a clinical plan). Of course, 85% is not perfect and
DMM finding of about one third of a normative distribution being carries an average of 15% errors. Unfortunately, all information,
secure and two thirds on a gradient from mildly to dangerously including the reading of X-rays and neuroimaging, carries risk of
insecure. Both classifying C2 as B4 and omitting the attachment error. We consider our standard stringent and appropriate to the
figure would tend to inflate the proportion of B classifications. gravity of the application, particularly because DMM coders are
The ABC+D finding of 50% secure seems untenable in light of trained to provide evidence from the assessment of attachment
population-level risks of psychiatric diagnosis, child maltreatment, (e.g., screenshots or quoted speech) and not simply their statement
and criminality. of the classification. Moreover, the full Strange Situation or Adult
Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan & Main, 1984–1996)
MISUNDERSTANDINGS is made available to court authorities, including other experts. The
availability of evidence of attachment classification further reduces
Reliability Versus Agreement the probability of error, makes the entire process transparent, and
We think that the commentary authors might have confused coder enables others to examine the evidence.
reliability (achieved on a standardized test prior to coding) with
coder agreement on a set of research data. Their lengthy discussion Seven Billion Humans
of agreement in the NICHD comparison study (Spieker & Critten- In their commentary, van IJzendoorn et al. (2018) state that the
den, 2010) is accurate regarding its reported research agreement, DMM has too many categories; they counted 24, but we think
59%, which we discussed as a major limitation of the findings. In there are more, even to the point of a unique DMM-AAI classifi-
our article (this issue), we stated that the Family Relations Institute cation for each individual person. Because we think that the DMM
(FRI) standard for research reliability is 80% (a Level II certifi- assessments are relevant to mental health, we compare the four
cate); the practice of the FRI when it undertakes blind coding of ABC+D categories with the increasing array of DSM-5 (Amer-
datasets is to select reliable coders, ask them to establish intercoder ican Psychiatric Association, 2013) or International Classifica-
agreement on each dataset before coding the data, and for success- tion of Diseases (World Health Organization, 1993) categories of
ful coders, to have unpredictable agreement checks throughout the psychiatric disorder and wonder if four categories can possibly
coding process so that if agreement drops, coding can stop until differentiate the types of relationship-based psychological prob-
agreement is reestablished. Maintaining agreement in this way is lems faced by infants, young children, and their parents (much
a time-consuming process, but it produces better data than having less all humans of all ages and cultures). What if we constrained
reliable coders classify an entire set of SSPs, only checking agree- doctors to four readily understood categories of physical illness,
ment at the conclusion of the study. The NICHD Study of Early some with high rates of misclassification? Our point is that only
Child Care coders in the 1990s, when the assessments reported in a theoretically structured model with meaningful dimensions and
Spieker and Crittenden (2010) were classified, did not use the FRI memorable clusters can be helpful to troubled children and their
procedures (These had not yet been developed.) This is an example parents. We seek a model where every peg has a place and can be
of learning from past experience and improving procedures for the described in ways that can guide compassionate treatment. We do
future. not think the four-category ABC+D model can do that.

Standardized Reliability Tests A Celebration With Colorful Confetti


We are, of course, aware of the discussion among ABC+D re- An important point is what van IJzendoorn et al. (2018) dis-
searchers regarding establishing standardized tests to ensure the miss as “a shower of colorful confetti” (p. 2) We wish they had

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
650 • P.M. Crittenden and S.J. Spieker

attended more thoughtfully to the structure of the DMM. The DMM classifications, the association of DMM classifications with mal-
and D share the goal of describing behavior that falls outside of treatment, and court decision-making (for residency and treatment
Ainsworth’s original ABC model. The DMM adds the notion of planning) might yield productive ideas.
seeking evidence of the function of this behavior, specifically with
regard to receiving protection and comfort from an attachment
IMPORTANT ISSUES FOR THE FUTURE
figure when a vulnerable person is exposed to danger. As to con-
fetti, our model is highly organized along two dimensions, both of The central purpose of our article was to explain how attachment,
which were introduced by Bowlby (1980, chap. 4), albeit in a less as conceptualized and assessed with the DMM, could be used in
complete form than now appears in the DMM. One dimension is forensic and clinical settings where the consequences of an attach-
source of information; that is, an individual’s relative reliance on ment report could affect individual lives. As stated earlier, we agree
cognition (temporal contingencies) and affect (negative feelings); with Granqvist and his 42 eminent coauthors (2017) that the D cat-
this dimension ranges from A strategies (mostly cognitive) through egory is not suitable for this application. On the other hand, when
integrative B strategies to C strategies (mostly affective). The other DMM assessments and classifications are used within the guide-
dimension is the degree of transformation of cognitive and affective lines of the IASA Family Attachment Court Protocol (Crittenden,
information from truly predictive through omitted, erroneous, dis- Farnfield, Landini, & Grey, 2013), we believe that attachment can
torted, falsified, and denied to delusionally self-generated. These contribute helpful information for solving the puzzle of family
dimensions free the DMM from the constraint of predefined cat- problems. Crucial to following IASA guidelines are coder relia-
egories while still permitting clustering of recognizable patterns. bility and report transparency. The standards for forensic coder
We think this is an elegant model that is approximated in DMM reliability are high (85% or more, together with approved work
assessments by strategies, psychological traumas and losses, and on writing family functional court reports). Even so, mistakes will
modified states of arousal. Indeed, we are pleased enough with this always be made. For this reason, the reports must be transparent to
achievement that we would happily celebrate with brightly colored the point of offering both evidence (photos, texts) from the assess-
confetti. ments and the full assessments to other court-appointed experts for
their scrutiny; this meets the evidentiary requirement of Daubert
v. Merrill (1993). In addition, interpretation of the assessment re-
Science Is Ever-Emergent
sults must be embedded in an array of assessments of attachment
Tied to the incomplete understanding of the DMM is a complaint among family members as well as professionals’ reports on the his-
that the model is “ever-emergent.” This, we think, is its strength. tory and functioning of family members. Finally, as stated in our
We cannot think of any body of scientific thought that has been original article, the purpose of DMM assessments of attachment
constant for almost four decades—except ABC+D attachment. is to contribute to placement and treatment decisions regarding
The hallmark of all sciences is constant change, constant updat- vulnerable children, and not to be the sole source of information
ing of what is known and how it is discovered. Yes, our coders upon which these decisions are made, nor the basis of decisions
have time-limited certificates of reliability and are required to keep regarding parental maltreatment.
up-to-date in the DMM. Psychotherapists and physicians are sim-
ilarly required to update their work. The Granqvist et al. (2017)
Evidentiary Standards
article could be considered recognition of the gap between what
ABC+D theory offers and delivers. Our article could be consid- van IJzendoorn et al. (2018) hold the DMM Preschool Assessment
ered, in part, an invitation to ABC+D researchers to update their of Attachment (PAA; Crittenden, 1992–2017) and other DMM as-
work. Put another way, we are proud of the ever-emergent qual- sessments accountable to the wrong evidentiary standard. (They
ity of the DMM because it keeps the theory and applications in are unclear as to whether they would hold the ABC+D Strange
tune with a wide range of scientific advances in related fields from Situation assessment to the same standard.) Proof beyond a rea-
genetics and neurobiology to family and community systems. sonable doubt and innocent until proven guilty are standards only
applicable to criminal cases. Dependency and custody cases have
different, and lower, standards. Further, the standard for expert ev-
Assessment and Absolutes
idence, as set out in Federal Rules of Evidence 702 (FRE, 2018),
A small point is that van IJzendoorn et al. (2018) repeatedly refer for any type of case, is that the fact and opinion evidence must be
to attachment “measures.” Attachment cannot be measured quanti- helpful, based on sufficient facts or data, be the product of reliable
tively; it is assessed qualitatively. That was an important distinction principles and methods, and be reliably applied to the facts of the
Mary Ainsworth discussed with her students, and we agree with case. The evidence must be nonspeculative, and the facts or data
her use of the word “assessment.” Another small point is that van and methods must be transparent (FRE 702; committee also notes
IJzendoorn et al. (2018) took several of our qualified and nuanced citing case law). Forensic DMM assessments, using highly reliable
comments and turned them into absolutes. Absolutes are straw coders with clear standards and transparent methods, meet the re-
men that are easy to destroy, but the field is not advanced by that. quirements of FRE 702. The five factors identified in the Daubert
Looking more carefully at what we actually said about anomalous case have been supplemented and expanded by FRE 702, yet DMM

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.
DMM vs. ABC+D Reply • 651

assessments also meet all five factors from the Daubert v. Merrill Austin, W.G., Kirkpatrick, H.D., & Flens, J.R. (2011). The emerging
(1993) case. DMM assessments and the IASA Court Protocol also forensic role for work product review and case analysis in child
are consistent with forensic guidelines identified by Austin, Kirk- access and parenting plan disputes. Family Court Review, 49(4),
patrick, and Flens (2011) for evaluator testimony using conceptual 737–749.
frameworks such as attachment. Bowlby, J. (1980). An information processing approach to defense. In
J. Bowlby (Ed.), Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness and
depression (pp. 44–74). New York: Basic Books.
Collegial Cooperation Around Shared Goals
Cassidy, J., & Marvin, R.S. (1992). Attachment organization in preschool
van IJzendoorn et al. (2018) call for unbiased joint research to com- children: Coding guidelines (4th ed.). Charlottesville, VA: University
pare the ABC+D and DMM classificatory methods. We heartily of Virginia.
agree with this idea. Because bias can be hard to discern, we sug- Crittenden, P.M. (1992–2017). Preschool assessment of attachment. Un-
gest a joint committee of ABC+D and DMM researchers (in equal published manual, Family Relations Institute, Miami.
numbers) and an existing dataset that meets both methods’ require- Crittenden, P.M., Farnfield, S., Landini, A., & Grey, B. (2013). Assessing
ments. We suggest videos that include the attachment figures, SSPs attachment for family court decision-making. Journal of Forensic
at 11 to 12 months (because Ainsworth’s et al. (1978) guidelines Practice, 15, 237–248.
were written for 11-month-old infants), and follow-up SSPs at 30 Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals (92-102), 509 U.S. 579 (1993).
to 36 months (because this age is centered within the preschool Federal Rules of Evidence 702, U.S.C.S. (2018)
years and is suitable for both the PAA and the preschool assess-
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1984–1996). Adult attach-
ment classification system (PACS); Cassidy and Marvin, with the ment interview protocol. Berkeley, CA: University of California at
MacArthur Attachment Working Group, 1992). Other validating Berkeley.
data should be in the dataset. Once a suitable dataset is found, all the
Granqvist, P., Sroufe, L.A., Dozier, M., Hesse, E., Steele, M., van Ijzen-
videos should be reclassified with the leaders in each camp choos-
doorn, M., . . . Duschinsky, R. (2017). Disorganized attachment in
ing and training the coders. The researchers should register the
infancy: A review of the phenomenon and its implications for clini-
study and agree to publish the results, regardless of the outcomes. cians and policy-makers. Attachment & Human Development, 19(6),
In closing, we want to emphasize the importance of a dialogue 534–558. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2017.1354040
and cross-classified datasets. This could be an opportunity to attract
Spieker, S., & Crittenden, P.M. (2010). Comparing two attach-
the next generation of attachment scholars to build upon a half- ment classification methods applied to preschool strange situa-
century of lessons learned. We appreciate Drs. van IJzendoorn, tions. Clinical Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 15(1), 97–120.
Bakermans, Steele, and Granqvist considering and responding to https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104509345878
our article and look forward to collaborative work addressing the
Spieker, S.J., & Crittenden, P.M. (2018). Can attachment inform decision-
needs of vulnerable children and their families.
making in child protection and forensic settings? Infant Mental
Health Journal, 39(6), 625–641.
REFERENCES van IJzendoorn, M.H., Bakermans, J.J.W., Steele, M., & Granqvist, P.
(2018). Diagnostic use of Crittenden’s attachment measures in Fam-
Ainsworth, D.S.M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of ily Court is not beyond a reasonable doubt. Infant Mental Health
attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, Journal, 39(6), 642–646.
NJ: Erlbaum. World Health Organization (1993). ICD-10, the ICD-10 classification of
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical man- mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research.
ual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Geneva: Author.

Infant Mental Health Journal DOI 10.1002/imhj. Published on behalf of the Michigan Association for Infant Mental Health.

También podría gustarte