Está en la página 1de 10

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 55:8 (2014), pp 914–923 doi:10.1111/jcpp.

12212

Genetics of preparation and response control in


ADHD: the role of DRD4 and DAT1
€ rn Albrecht,1,2,a Daniel Brandeis,2,3,4,5,a Henrik Uebel- von Sandersleben,1
Bjo
Lilian Valko,3 Hartmut Heinrich,6,7 Xiaohui Xu,8 Renate Drechsler,3 Alexander Heise,1
Jonna Kuntsi,8 Ueli C. Mu € ller,3,9 Philip Asherson,8 Hans-Christoph Steinhausen,3,10,11
Aribert Rothenberger,1 and Tobias Banaschewski2
1
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Medical Center G€ ottingen, Germany; 2Department of Child
ottingen, G€
and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim/
Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany; 3Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Z€ urich,
urich, Switzerland; 4Center for Integrative Human Physiology, University of Z€
Z€ urich, Z€
urich, Switzerland;
5
Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Z€ urich, Switzerland; 6Department of Child and
Adolescent Mental Health, University of Erlangen, Erlangen, Germany; 7Heckscher-Klinik, M€ unchen, Germany;
8
King’s College London, MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, London,
UK; 9Interkantonale Hochschule f€ ur Heilp€
adagogik, Z€urich, Switzerland; 10Research Unit of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, Aalborg University Hospital, Aalborg, Denmark; 11Clinical Psychology and Epidemiology, University of
Basel, Basel, Switzerland

Background: Difficulties with performance and brain activity related to attentional orienting (Cue-P3), cognitive or
response preparation (Cue-CNV) and inhibitory response control (Nogo-P3) during tasks tapping executive functions
are familial in ADHD and may represent endophenotypes. The aim of this study was to clarify the impact of dopamine
receptor D4 (DRD4) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) gene polymorphisms on these processes in ADHD and control
children. Methods: Behavioural and electrophysiological parameters from cued continuous performance tests with
low and high attentional load were assessed in boys with ADHD combined type (N = 94) and controls without family
history of ADHD (N = 31). Both groups were split for the presence of at least one DRD4 7-repeat allele and the DAT1
10-6 haplotype. Results: Children with ADHD showed diminished performance and lower Cue-P3, CNV and Nogo-P3
amplitudes. Children with DRD4 7R showed similar performance problems and lower Cue-P3 and CNV, but Nogo-P3
was not reduced. Children with the DAT1 10-6 haplotype had no difficulties with performance or Cue-P3 and CNV,
but contrary to expectations increased Nogo-P3. There were no Genotype by ADHD interactions. Conclusions: This
study detected specific effects of DRD4 7R on performance and brain activity related to attentional orienting and
response preparation, while DAT1 10-6 was associated with elevated brain activity related to inhibitory response
control, which potentially compensates increased impulsivity. As these genotype effects were additive to the impact of
ADHD, the current results indicate that DRD4 and DAT1 polymorphisms are functionally relevant risk factors for
ADHD and presumably other disorders sharing these endophenotypes. Keywords: Attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder, ERP, endophenotypes, CPT, CNV, Nogo, P3.

notypes, which are characterized by more fundamen-


Introduction
tal quantitative biological properties in between
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders (ADHD),
genetic or environmental risk factors and the pheno-
characterized by high and age-inappropriate levels
type, may help to clarify these associations through
of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity, is asso-
potentially larger and/or more specific genetic effects
ciated with dysfunctions in neuronal networks
than found for the ADHD diagnosis. Endophenotypes
responsible for attention and response control
may thus offer advantages for molecular genetic
(Castellanos, Sonuga-Barke, Milham, & Tannock,
studies (Doyle et al., 2005) and explain the hetero-
2006; Durston, van Belle, & de Zeeuw, 2011). Her-
geneity of the ADHD phenotype (Buitelaar, 2005).
itability estimates of more than 70% favour a model of
Recently, we found that several performance and
strong genetic impact on the expression of the
electrophysiological parameters of response prepa-
disorder (Faraone & Mick, 2010). However, there is
ration and response control are driven by familial
a gap between the large heritability estimates from
factors shared with childhood ADHD (Albrecht et al.,
twin and adoption studies and the rather small
2013). Children with ADHD and to a lesser extent
effects found between ADHD and individual risk
also their nonaffected siblings showed impairments
alleles. So far, developmental pathways from genetics
when compared to controls without a family history
and environmental factors are not well understood
of the disorder. Thus, deficits on these parameters
(Banaschewski et al., 2005). Intermediate endophe-
may be a consequence of genes or environmental
factors shared within families.
a
These authors contributed equally to this work. Preparation and response control was assessed
Conflict of interest statement: See disclosures in during visual Continuous Performance Tests [CPT
Acknowledgements (Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarason, Bransome, & Beck,

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
doi:10.1111/jcpp.12212 Genetics of preparation and response control in ADHD 915

1956)] combining vigilance and cued Go-Nogo tasks Two genes related to dopaminergic functioning and
where rare cues require preparation for a Go-Nogo associated with ADHD and related difficulties may
task: if followed by a target, a simple response have an impact on preparation and response control.
should be executed, but if followed by a distractor, The dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene modulates a
the prepared response must be withheld through receptor, which is mostly present in the frontal lobe,
inhibitory response control. orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex (Gizer &
Processing cues requires mainly attentional orient- Waldman, 2012; Noain et al., 2006). DRD4 has a
ing and resource allocation reflected in the Cue-P3, a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) between 2
parietally broad positive deflection in the event-re- and 10, with the 7-repeat (7R) polymorphism thought
lated potential (ERP) starting about 300 ms after cue to be less sensitive to dopaminergic stimulation
onset. Source localizations revealed predominantly (Asghari et al., 1995). Several studies indicate that
posterior sources; moreover, children with ADHD the DRD4 7R polymorphism is related to sustained
consistently show impairments (Albrecht et al., attention and cognitive control performance (Gizer &
2013; Brandeis et al., 2002; van Leeuwen et al., Waldman, 2012; Johnson et al., 2008; Kieling,
1998). In a recent longitudinal study, the Cue-P3 Roman, Doyle, Hutz, & Rohde, 2006), probably in
showed a persistent deficit in children with ADHD epistatic interaction with a gene coding cate-
that was particularly prominent at a mean age of chol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) (Heinzel et al.,
12 years (Doehnert, Brandeis, Imhof, Drechsler, & 2012). Recent metaanalyses suggest that DRD4 7R
Steinhausen, 2010), and deficits were also present in is also moderately associated with ADHD (Faraone &
adults with ADHD (McLoughlin et al., 2010). Mick, 2010; Li, Sham, Owen, & He, 2006).
Following the Cue-P3, cues (but not irrelevant The second gene to be tested here is the dopamine
distractors) evoke a centrally negative slow cortical transporter gene (DAT1, SLC6A3) which codes for the
potential terminated by the next stimulus (contingent carrier protein that accomplishes the reuptake of
negative variation, CNV), which reflects preparation dopamine from the synaptic cleft. It is present espe-
(Walter, Cooper, Aldridge, McCallum, & Winter, 1964) cially in the striatum and nucleus accumbens, with
or time estimation (Macar & Vidal, 2003). Neurophys- projections to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
iological and fMRI studies demonstrated dopaminer- (DLPF) (Ciliax et al., 1999). Methylphenidate, the
gic control (Kratz et al., 2012; Linssen et al., 2011) most frequently prescribed stimulant used for ADHD
and activity of an ensemble of thalamo-cortical treatment, blocks the dopamine transporter which
structures including the dorsal anterior cingulate, results in higher levels of available dopamine in the
frontal cortex, thalamus and midbrain dopaminergic synaptic cleft (Spencer, Biederman, & Wilens, 2000).
nuclei involved in CNV generation (Lutcke, Gevensle- The DAT1 gene has two commonly studied VNTR
ben, Albrecht, & Frahm, 2009). Moreover, recent polymorphisms of 9 or 10 repeats of a 40-basepair
studies showed an impact of dopaminergic genes on sequence in the 3′ untranslated region (UTR). The
visual and motor postprocessing during CNV gener- 10R polymorphism may be related to some kind of
ation (Bender et al., 2012a,b). The CNV amplitude is response control problem, as recent studies reported
consistently reduced in ADHD (Banaschewski et al., performance problems in a verbal inhibition task
2003; Perchet, Revol, Fourneret, Mauguiere, & Garcia- (Cornish et al., 2005) and higher number of impulsive
Larrea, 2001) and may also represent a persistent errors in a CPT paradigm (Gizer & Waldman, 2012),
deficit throughout life (Doehnert, Brandeis, Schneider, as well as diminished brain activity related to perfor-
Drechsler, & Steinhausen, 2013; Doehnert et al., mance monitoring (Althaus et al., 2010) and reduced
2010; McLoughlin et al., 2010; Valko et al., 2009). anteriorization of the Nogo-P3 (Dresler et al., 2010).
Following Cues, response control is required, and However, the effect of these polymorphisms on the
the difference in task demand between Go and Nogo dopamine transporter functioning may change with
trials – executing versus suppressing a prepared development. The 10-repeat allele might be a risk
response – is paralleled in the ERP. In Nogo trials, factor for childhood ADHD, while the 9-repeat poly-
enhanced N2 amplitudes indexing conflict monitor- morphism was associated with ADHD in adults
ing (Albrecht et al., 2008; Banaschewski et al., 2003) (Franke et al., 2010). Moreover, the 10-repeat allele
are followed by the Nogo-P3, which is maximal at may lead to functional deficits only if other func-
fronto-central sites (Fallgatter & Strik, 1999). The tional sites within DAT1 are present that form a
Nogo-P3 may be generated by several processes of specific haplotype of the 10-repeat allele in the 3′UTR
inhibitory response control in medial or anterior in conjunction with the 6-repeat allele of a 30-base
cingulate cortex (ACC) and premotor and frontal pair VNTR located within intron 8 (the 10-6 haplo-
areas (Verleger, Paehge, Kolev, Yordanova, & type) (Asherson et al., 2007).
Jaskowski, 2006). Clinical studies have shown that Taken together, cognitive behavioural performance
the Nogo-P3 amplitude or anteriorization is altered in in the CPT and the brain activity parameters related
patients with ADHD and seems to represent a persis- to attentional orienting (Cue-P3), preparation (CNV)
tent neurophysiological deficit in children and adults and inhibitory response control (Nogo-P3) show
with ADHD (Doehnert et al., 2010; Fallgatter et al., functional impairments in ADHD that are driven by
2004; McLoughlin et al., 2010; Valko et al., 2009). familial factors and probably relate to dopaminergic

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
916 Bj€
orn Albrecht et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2014; 55(8): 914–23

dysfunctions. We aim to clarify the role of DRD4 and


DAT1 genes and their impact on preparation and
response control in addition to the diagnostic phe-
notype further in this study.

Method
Participants
Children or adolescents with ADHD were recruited from
specialist clinics or child and adolescent psychiatrists in
private practice in Z€ urich, Switzerland and G€ ottingen, Ger-
many. Controls were recruited from local schools or sports
clubs. All children participated on the basis of informed
consent from child and parents. Ethical approval for this Figure 1 Task description. Standard- and Flanker-CPT in compar-
study was obtained from the local ethical review boards. ison. Both tasks require responding if the central Cue letter ‘O’ is
One hundred and twenty-five boys aged 8–16 years with an followed by the Target ‘X’, the additional flankers augment
estimated IQ above 80, genotyped and with good EEG quality additional attentional load
fulfilled either a research diagnosis of ADHD combined type
(N = 94) or were healthy controls without a known family the time course and source localizations of Cue-P3, -CNV and
history of ADHD (N = 31). The sample overlap with a previous Nogo-P3 (see Figure 2). Topographic exploration by genotype
familiality study (Albrecht et al., 2013) was 90% (disregarding and group is given in the supplementary Figures S2 and S3.
unaffected siblings). More details of the sample, diagnostics,
comorbidities and the genotyping are given in the supplemen-
tary Appendix S1. The ADHD and Control groups were further
subdivided according to DRD4 7R (presence of at least one
Results
7-repeat allele, 10/31 controls & 24/94 ADHD) and DAT1 10-6
haplotype (homozygotic for the 10-repeat allele of the VNTR in Group description
the 3UTR and homozygotic for the 6-repeat allele of the intron
Children with ADHD and controls did not differ in
8 VNTR, 17/31 controls & 38/94 ADHD) polymorphisms.
Children taking stimulant treatment were off medication for at age in both genotype splits (Diagnosis: both
least 48 hr before testing. F(1,121)<1.4, p > .24, part. g2<.02). Frequencies of
DRD4 7R polymorphisms and the DAT1 10-6 hap-
lotype were similar in both diagnostic groups
Stimuli and task (v2(1) = .5, p = .47 and v2(1) = 2.0, p = .16 respec-
To assess attentional alerting, orienting and preparation and tively). In the DRD4 split, nonsignificant trends for
response control, a Standard- and a Flanker-CPT (augmenting higher prorated IQ in Controls (F(1,121) = 3.7, p = .06,
attentional and response control load) was used (Doehnert
part. g2 = .03) and for age a marginal trend Diagno-
et al., 2010, 2013; Valko et al., 2009). In both CPTs, the
children had to respond with the index finger of their dominant sis*DRD4 (F(1,121) = 2.8, p = .10, part. g2 = .02) were
hand only if a central Cue ‘O’ (presented 80 times, see Figure 1) detected. No effects were found for DAT1 (all
was followed by a central Target ‘X’ (N = 40), but to withhold F(1,121)<1.9, p > .17). The SDQ teacher ratings (Fig-
responding if the cue was followed by a nontarget (Nogo trials, ure 3, missing for 5 controls and 1 boy with ADHD)
N = 40) and on all other trials. The total of 400 stimuli
showed consistently higher behavioural problems in
presented pseudorandomized every 1650 ms.
Both CPTs included 25 practice trials, lasted 11 min each, ADHD compared to controls (Diagnosis: all
and were run counterbalanced after 5 min of resting EEG and F(1,115)>9.9, p < .01, part. g2>.08) without main
with a 13 min action monitoring task in between. effects for either DRD4 nor DAT1 (all F(1,115)<1.6,
p > .22, part. g2<.02) nor interactions between Diag-
nosis and Genetics (all F(1,115)<1.9, p > .16, part.
Data analysis
g2<.02) except for Hyperactivity (Diagnosis*DRD4:
The recording and processing of performance and electrophys- F(1,115) = 3.5, p = .06, part. g2 = .03).
iological data is similar to the methods used in a previous
publication (Albrecht et al., 2013); details are given in the
Appendix S1. Performance data
Performance and electrophysiological parameters were
tested parametrically with a repeated measure analysis of Mean reaction time (MRT). Responses were faster
variance (ANOVA) with within-subject factor ‘Task’ (Flanker- in older participants (Age: both genotype splits
vs. Standard-CPT) and between-subject factors ‘Diagnosis’
F(1,120)>25.2, p < .01, part. g2>.17) and slower in
(ADHD vs. Controls) and genotype status of either ‘DRD4’
(presence of 7R) or ‘DAT1’ (presence of the 10-6 haplotype). Age the more difficult Flanker-CPT (Task: both
was entered as a covariate in analyses of performance and ERP F(1,120)>6.0, p < .02, part. g2>.05).
data to control for small differences in the genotype splits. The split by DRD4 status revealed slower MRT on
Entering IQ as an additional covariate retained all effects the one hand in ADHD (Diagnosis: F(1,120) = 4.6,
except for the Nogo-P3 (see below) and revealed no further
p = .04, part. g2 = .04) and in children with DRD4
significant main effects or interactions.
ERP waveforms, topographic mapping and sLORETA analy- 7R polymorphism (DRD4: F(1,120) = 5.9, p = .02,
ses (Fuchs, Kastner, Wagner, Hawes, & Ebersole, 2002; Pasc- part. g2 = .05) without a significant interaction
ual-Marqui, 2002) of the control group’s grand average explored Diagnosis*DRD4 (F(1,120)<1, p = .81, part. g2<.01).

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
doi:10.1111/jcpp.12212 Genetics of preparation and response control in ADHD 917

Figure 2 Event-related Potentials during Preparation and Response Control. This figure gives Control subjects ERP waveforms from Cues
(above) and Cued Distractors (below) of the Standard-CPT

This was more pronounced in the Flanker-CPT cortex (Figures 2 and S2a). Controls showed higher
(Diagnosis*Task: F(1,120) = 5.6, p = .02, part. amplitude than children with ADHD (Diagnosis:
g2 = .04 and DRD4*Task: F(1,120) = 3.3, p = .07, both F(1,120)>10.0, p < .01, part. g2>.08). Moreover,
part. g2 = .03, see Figure 4). children with DRD4 7R had a diminished Cue-P3
The split by DAT1 10-6 haplotype revealed non- amplitude (DRD4: F(1,120) = 5.1, p = .03, part.
significant trends for slower RT in ADHD particularly g2 = .04; DRD4*Task: F(1,120) = 2.7, p = .10, part.
in the Flanker-CPT (Diagnosis: F(1,120) = 3.5, p = .07, g2 = .02), independently of ADHD (Diagnosis*DRD4:
part. g2 = .03 and Diagnosis*Task: F(1,120) = 3.4, F(1,120)<1, p = .93, part. g2<.01, see Figure S2b). The
p = .07, part. g2 = .03). No main effects or interac- DAT1 10-6 haplotype yielded no effect on Cue-P3 (all
tions including DAT1 10-6 haplotype were found (all F(1,120)<1.6, p > .21, part. g2<.02, see Figure S2c).
F(1,120)<1.0, p > .33, part. g2<.01).
Contingent negative variation (CNV). The CNV
Reaction time variability (RT-SD). RT-SD was mean amplitude has a centro-parietal maximum
lower in older participants (Age: both F(1,120)>11.1, with sources in medial and orbitofrontal cortex, as
p < .01, part. g2>.85) and in controls (Diagnosis: well as dorsal ACC (see Figures 2 and S2a for further
both F(1,120)>10.6, p < .01, part. g2>.08). It did not details). It was generally lower in ADHD than
differ between tasks (Task, both F(1,120)<2.3, p > .13, controls (Diagnosis: both genotype splits
part. g2<.02 and Task*Diagnosis: both F(1,120)<1, F(1,120)>17.2, p < .01, part. g2>.13). The Flanker-CPT
p > .54, part. g2<.01)). evoked a smaller CNV in children with ADHD, but
RT-SD was nonsignificantly increased in DRD4 7R not in controls (Task: both F(1,120)>7.0, p < .01, part.
(DRD4: F(1,120) = 3.7, p = .06, part. g2 = .03), and g2>.05 and Task*Group: both F(1,120)>5.0, p < .03,
clearly unaffected from DAT1 10-6 haplotype part. g2>.04). In addition, this CNV reduction under
(F(1,120) = .7, p = .40, part. g2<.01), without any increased attentional load was smaller in older
higher order interactions including Diagnosis or children (Age: both F(1,120)<1, p > .90, part. g2<.01,
DRD4 nor DAT1 status (all F(1,120)<2.0, p > .15, part. but Task*Age: both F(1,120)>5.6, p < .02, part.
g2<.02). g2>.04). Children with DRD4 7R showed a lower
CNV (DRD4: F(1,120) = 4.3, p = .04, part. g2 = .04,
see Figure S2d). Moreover, the CNV reduction in the
ERP data
Flanker-CPT in DRD4 7R carriers was particularly
Cue-P3. The mean Cue-P3 was maximal over pari- present in ADHD, but not in control children
etal areas with sources in the superior parietal (Task*Group*DRD4: F(1,120) = 5.1, p = .03, part.

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
918 Bj€
orn Albrecht et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2014; 55(8): 914–23

Figure 3 Psychopathology. SDQ teacher ratings by ADHD and genotype; error bars indicate confidence intervals with p = .05. More
problems in ADHD, but neither DRD4 (top) nor DAT1 (bottom) main effects are evident

g2 = .04). However, we detected homogeneity of part. g2<.02, and Task*Age: both F(1,120)>3.7,
variance in the Standard-CPT’s CNV (driven by lower p < .06, part. g2>.03).
variance within the Controls samples), as well as one It was smaller in ADHD (Diagnosis: both
potential outlier in the control sample carrying F(1,120)>3.8, p < .05, part. g2>.03). No effects of
DRD4 7R (with CNV amplitude 2.2 SD below the DRD4 were found (all F(1,120)<1.9, p > .17, part.
group’s mean, but not detected as an outlier by g2<.02, see Figure S3b for further details), but the
Grubbs’ procedure (Grubbs, 1969), p > .05), which Nogo-P3 amplitude was contrary to the hypothesis
may compromise the statistics. Limiting the DRD4 larger in children with DAT1 10-6 haplotype, following
analysis to the ADHD sample confirmed a significant an additive model with ADHD (DAT1: F(1,120) = 6.9,
main effect of DRD4 (F(1,91) = 5.0, p = .03, part. p = .01, part. g2 = .05, and Diagnosis*DAT1:
g2 = .05 and a tendency towards an interaction F(1,120)<1, p > .39, part. g2<.01; see Figure S3c).
Task*DRD4 (F(1,91) = 3.1, p = .08, part. g2 = .03), To clarify whether the elevated Nogo-P3 in children
indicating for ADHD that the diminished CNV ampli- with DAT1 10-6 may index reduced neuronal pro-
tude in children with DRD4 7R was present in both cessing efficiency, partial correlations with RT con-
CPTs, but as a tendency more pronounced in the trolling for age and phenotype effects were computed.
more difficult Flanker-CPT. If the Nogo-P3 reflects an intact response inhibitory
No such effects were found for the DAT1 10-6 process, higher Nogo-P3 amplitudes are expected in
haplotype (DAT1: F(1,120)<1.0, p > .48, part. g2<.01; children responding faster, resulting in negative
Group*DAT1: F(1,120)<1, p = .48, part. g2<.01; Task*- correlations. The interindividual correlations in
Group*DAT1: F(1,120) = 1.6, p = .21, part. g2 = .01, children with (r(51) = .46, p < .01) and without
Figure S2e). (r(66) = .24, p = .05) the 10-6 haplotype were both
significantly negative, but as a tendency more
Nogo-P3. The amplitude of the Nogo-P3 had a strongly in children with the 10-6 haplotype
more anterior maximum than the Cue-P3 and (p = .09, one-tailed).
sources in superior and medial frontal cortex (Fig- Controlling for IQ in the DAT1 comparison reduced
ures 2 and S3a). It was lower in the Flanker-CPT the main ‘Task’ effect on Nogo-P3 to a trend; no other
(Task: both genotype splits F(1,120)>4.6, p < .04, changes in the results were found. Please see
part. g2 ≥ .04), but this difference tended to Figure 5 for a synopsis of results regarding brain
decrease with age (Age: both F(1,120)<1.6, p > .20, electrical activity.

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
doi:10.1111/jcpp.12212 Genetics of preparation and response control in ADHD 919

Figure 4 Performance Data. Mean RT and RT-SD with confidence intervals (adjusted to the mean age of 138.6 months, p = .05) for the
main effects from both Standard- and Flanker-CPT (Mean: (CPTS+ CPTF)/2, left side in each plot) as well as the interaction difference [D:
(CPTF-CPTS], right side) of Controls and children with ADHD and DRD4 and DAT1 comparisons. Mean was slower (left column) and more
variable (right column) in both CPTs for children with ADHD and for those with DRD4 7R (above), but not with the DAT1 10-6 haplotype
(below)

Discussion given the small risks associated with polymorphisms


The first research question addressed in the study in previous studies of ADHD. Thus, the functional
was whether functional polymorphisms of the DRD4 relationship between these polymorphisms and brain
and DAT1 genes have an impact on symptom sever- activity parameters in ADHD discussed below may not
ity in ADHD. As groups were matched for age, we be the consequence of increased symptom severity
would not expect that the findings were confounded per se.
with effects of typical development. Moreover, gen- In a previous report with an overlapping sample,
eral cognitive ability was similar in ADHD and we found that the performance parameters response
controls, and did not differ between DRD4 and speed and variability, as well as the electrophysio-
DAT1 polymorphisms. logical brain activity parameters Cue-P3, CNV and
As expected, children with ADHD showed increased Nogo-P3 were driven by familial factors in ADHD,
hyperactivity, but also less social behaviour and and may thus be linked to shared genetic or
increased emotional, conduct and peer problems. As environmental factors (Albrecht et al., 2013). This
outlined in the supplement, children with ADHD also leads to the second question addressed here,
frequently showed comorbidities, particularly whether the DRD4 and DAT1 polymorphisms have
oppositional defiant and conduct disorders, as well an impact on performance and electrophysiological
as mood disorders, which are common, particularly in brain activity parameters associated with prepara-
boys (Biederman, 2005). Taken together, these tion and response control.
results suggest that the patient sample investigated Here, we observed that performance (mean
shows very typical behavioural problems of children response speed) decrements were also present in
with ADHD (Becker et al., 2006). There were no children with the DRD4 7R polymorphism. As DRD4
relations either of DRD4 or DAT1 polymorphisms 7R is also associated with ADHD, such an effect
with symptom severity, although this was expected might be expected if the DRD4 7R polymorphism

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
920 Bj€
orn Albrecht et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2014; 55(8): 914–23

Figure 5 Electrophysiology of preparation and response control. Brain electrical mean amplitudes and confidence intervals (adjusted to
the mean age of 138.6 months, p = .05) of brain electrical activity. The Cue-P3 (left) and CNV (middle) showed an additive effect of ADHD
phenotype and DRD4 7R genotype. The Nogo-P3 (right) was lower in ADHD, but contrary to expectations increased in children with the
DAT1 10-6 haplotype

play a functional role in ADHD. But furthermore, the Specifically, the DRD4 polymorphism had a gen-
effect of the DRD4 7R was present in both children eral impact on brain activity during cue processing:
with ADHD and Controls, i.e. the effect appears to be the Cue-P3 associated with attentional orienting
additive and of similar small to medium effect size as appears to be diminished in children with the 7R
the response speed problems of children with ADHD. variant. In line with other studies, we found source
However, the DAT1 polymorphisms revealed no such localizations of Cue P3 in superior parietal brain
impact on CPT performance. areas. However, as the posterior attention system is
The analysis of performance errors was limited particularly associated with Acetylcholine and Nor-
by the generally high accuracy reached, and the epinephrine (Posner & Rothbart, 2007), the influ-
expected ADHD and genotype effects may be hid- ence of DRD4 may be due to processes that trigger
den by ceiling effects. This is in contrast to a the Cue-P3. The preparatory Cue-CNV shows a very
recent study by Gizer and Waldman with a similar similar pattern of results, as it is diminished in
CPT that found an association of DRD4 with ADHD and children with the DRD4 7R allele. In
inattention errors (commission errors to uncued previous work, we found sources of the CNV
targets) and of DAT1 with impulsivity (commission located in dorsal anterior cingulate and frontal
errors to cued nontargets) (Gizer & Waldman, cortex (Albrecht et al., 2013; Lutcke et al., 2009).
2012). It remains open whether this discrepancy These brain regions have strong dopaminergic
could be explained by differences in speed accu- innervation potentially following input from dopa-
racy trade-off; but response speed was not minergic midbrain nuclei, and correspond well to
assessed in there. The slower mean response speed the DRD4 expression in frontal areas as the
associated with DRD4 7R in this study may thus orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate. Impor-
similarly be driven by inattention, but the role of tantly, these DRD4 genotype and ADHD phenotype
DAT1 on impulsivity cannot be confirmed at the effects on Cue-P3 and CNV follow and additive
performance level. model, so the DRD4 7R polymorphism led to

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
doi:10.1111/jcpp.12212 Genetics of preparation and response control in ADHD 921

similar impairments in children with ADHD and further explain preparation and response control
healthy Controls. deficits in ADHD.
No effects of DRD4 were found for the Nogo-P3, an
electrophysiological parameter of executive attention
during inhibitory response control. As it is generated Conclusion
in prefrontal brain regions where the D4 receptor is In this study, we found evidence that preparation and
most abundant (Meador-Woodruff et al., 1996), an response control, both impaired in ADHD, are related
impact would be expected. However, this specificity to polymorphisms of genes related to the dopaminer-
of DRD4 for attention and preparation rather than gic system. The DRD4 7R effect on performance and
response inhibitory control is also reported in per- brain activity in the superior parietal and medial
formance data studies (Gizer & Waldman, 2012) and frontal cortex and the dorsal anterior cingulate is
is thus paralleled by brain activity parameters. related to attentional orienting and response prepa-
Effects of the dopamine transporter (DAT1) 10-6 ration. The impact of the DAT1 10-6 haplotype,
haplotype polymorphism were almost a reverse potentially leading to increased impulsivity, may be
image of the DRD4 7R findings. Performance data compensated by increased inhibitory response con-
and brain activity during Cue processing were not trol generated in the superior and medial frontal
affected, but children carrying the 10-6 haplotype cortex and the anterior cingulate. The medium to
(moderately associated with ADHD) showed large effects of the diagnostic phenotype and the
increased instead of reduced Nogo-P3 amplitude, small to medium effects of the genetic DRD4 and
linked to neuronal sources in cingulate cortex and DAT1 polymorphisms were additive, indicating sim-
premotor and frontal areas (Verleger et al., 2006). As ilar genetic mechanisms in patients with ADHD and
the Nogo-P3 analysed here was recorded in trials healthy controls, supporting a dimensional view of
with successful inhibition or termination of the ADHD. If replicated, these results highlight the func-
prepared response, one may speculate that this tional role of dopaminergic gene variants among the
larger brain activity does in fact reflect lower neuro- risk factors for ADHD and probably other disorders
nal efficiency (as the same performance could be with attentional orienting, preparation and response
achieved with higher ‘effort’ in children with the control as endophenotypes.
DAT1 10-6). However, this may be not the case, as
interindividually faster responding was associated
with enhanced Nogo-P3 which may reflect higher Supporting information
need for response inhibition and consequently more Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
resource allocation in individuals responding faster. online version of this article:
Similarly, a recent study using transcranial mag- Figure S1. Supplement to Response preparation follow-
netic stimulation in healthy adults shows that ing Cues.
increased Nogo-P3 may compensate deficient inhib- Figure S2. Supplement to Inhibitory Response Control
itory processes within the motor system (Hoegl et al., following Cued Nontargets.
2011). Albeit this study gives no evidence for Appendix S1. Supplementary Methods and Materials.
increased impulsivity from performance data or
psychopathology, the increased Nogo-P3 in children
with the DAT1 10-6 haplotype may reflect a com- Acknowledgements
pensatory neuronal response inhibition process. In this study, recruitment of ADHD sibling pairs was
There are a number of limitations that need to be supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant
R01MH062873 to Steve Faraone and Swiss National
considered in the evaluation of the study. The 24
Science Foundation grant 32-109591 to H.-C. S.
scalp electrodes available for this study are suffi-
H.U.S received conference attendance support or was
cient to capture the basic scalp topography of the paid for public speaking by Eli Lilly and Company,
evoked potentials, but the precision of the brain Janssen-Cilag, Medice, Novartis and Shire. P.A. has
electrical neuroimaging is limited (Michel & Bran- received funding for educational and research activities
deis, 2009), and source localization is used here as from Janssen-Cilag and Shire, and consultancy and
an exploratory tool that may help to integrate the speaker fees from Eli Lilly and Company, Flynn
findings into theories of ADHD. As simple genotype Pharma, and Shire that have been used for educational
effects are expected to be rather small, this study and research activities. In the last 36 months, H.-C. S.
is limited by a relatively small sample, particularly has been a member of the advisory board and/or
of Controls. Albeit the data suggest that the received payment for lectures including service on
speakers’ bureaus by Shire and Medice (pharmaceuti-
genotype effects of DRD4 and DAT1 polymor-
cal companies). A. R. is on the advisory board and
phisms may be additive to the impact of the ADHD
speakers’ bureau of Eli Lilly and Company, Medice,
phenotype and occur similarly in patients and Novartis and Shire; has received educational grants
controls, we think that the results need to be from Medice and Shire; and has received research
replicated. It remains open whether DRD4 and support from Schwabe and Shire. T. B. served in an
DAT1 polymorphisms are specifically associated advisory or consultancy role for Hexal Pharma, Lilly,
with comorbid disorders, and whether these may Medice, Novartis, PCM scientific, Shire and Viforphar-

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
922 Bj€
orn Albrecht et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2014; 55(8): 914–23

ma. He received conference attendance support and and Nicola W€ostmann performed IQ- and further neu-
speaker’s fee by Lilly, Janssen McNeil, Medice, Novartis ropsychological testings. The authors also thank all
and Shire. He is/has been involved in clinical trials children and their families for participation.
conducted by Lilly, Shire & Viforpharma. The authors
declare that the present work is unrelated to the above
grants and relationships. Rest of the authors declare Correspondence
they have no potential or competing conflicts of interest. Bj€
orn Albrecht, University Medical Center of G€
ottingen,
The authors acknowledge: Christa Dahlmann, Re- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, von Siebold-Str. 5,
nate Kolle and Antonia Bak conducted ERP-recordings; 37075 G€ ottingen, Germany; Email:balbrec@gwdg.de
Anke Fillmer-Otte, Anne Reiners, Gudrun Schneider

Key points
• Attention, Preparation and Response Control problems that are familially driven in ADHD may reflect the
impact of genetic or environmental factors shared with the disorder.
• In this study, we tested the impact of ADHD and candidate gene polymorphisms regulating the Dopamine D4
receptor (DRD4) and Dopamine Transporter (DAT1).
• Children with the DRD4 7R polymorphism showed reduced attentional orienting and response preparation,
while the DAT1 10-6 haplotype led to increased need for response preparation.
• Genetic effects were similar in children with ADHD and healthy controls.
• Polymorphisms of genes regulating dopaminergic brain networks may increase the risk for ADHD by
diminishing attention and preparation or requiring compensatory response control.

Strengths and difficulties questionnaire in a pan-european


References study. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15 (Suppl
Albrecht, B., Brandeis, D., Uebel, H., Heinrich, H., Mueller, 1), I56–I62.
U.C., Hasselhorn, M., . . . & Banaschewski, T. (2008). Action Bender, S., Rellum, T., Freitag, C., Resch, F., Rietschel, M.,
monitoring in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity Treutlein, J., . . . & Laucht, M. (2012a). Dopamine inactivation
disorder, their nonaffected siblings, and normal control efficacy related to functional dat1 and comt variants
subjects: Evidence for an endophenotype. Biological influences motor response evaluation. PLoS ONE, 7, e37814.
Psychiatry, 64, 615–625. Bender, S., Rellum, T., Freitag, C., Resch, F., Rietschel, M.,
Albrecht, B., Brandeis, D., Uebel, H., Valko, L., Heinrich, H., Treutlein, J., . . . & Laucht, M. (2012b). Time-resolved
Drechsler, R., . . . & Banaschewski, T. (2013). Familiality of influences of functional dat1 and comt variants on visual
neural preparation and response control in childhood perception and post-processing. PLoS ONE, 7, e41552.
attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Biederman, J. (2005). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
Medicine, 43, 1997–2011. A selective overview. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1215–1220.
Althaus, M., Groen, Y., Wijers, A.A., Minderaa, R.B., Kema, Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., Baving, L., Georgiewa, P.,
I.P., Dijck, J.D., & Hoekstra, P.J. (2010). Variants of the Blanz, B., Warnke, A., . . . & Scheuerpflug, P. (2002).
slc6a3 (dat1) polymorphism affect performance Multicenter p300 brain mapping of impaired attention to
monitoring-related cortical evoked potentials that are cues in hyperkinetic children. Journal of the American
associated with adhd. Biological Psychology, 85, 19–32. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 990–998.
Asghari, V., Sanyal, S., Buchwaldt, S., Paterson, A., Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). ADHD: Strategies to unravel its genetic
Jovanovic, V., & Van Tol, H.H. (1995). Modulation of architecture. Journal of Neural Transmission. Supplementum,
intracellular cyclic amp levels by different human 69, 1–17.
dopamine d4 receptor variants. Journal of Neurochemistry, Castellanos, F.X., Sonuga-Barke, E.J., Milham, M.P., &
65, 1157–1165. Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in adhd:
Asherson, P., Brookes, K., Franke, B., Chen, W., Gill, M., Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci, 10, 117–123.
Ebstein, R.P., . . . & Faraone, S.V. (2007). Confirmation that a Ciliax, B. J., Drash, G. W., Staley, J. K., Haber, S., Mobley, C. J.,
specific haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene is Miller, G. W., . . . & Levey, A. I. (1999). Immunocytochemical
associated with combined-type adhd. American Journal of localization of the dopamine transporter in human brain.
Psychiatry, 164, 674–677. J Comp Neurol, 409, 38–56.
Banaschewski, T., Brandeis, D., Heinrich, H., Albrecht, B., Cornish, K.M., Manly, T., Savage, R., Swanson, J., Morisano,
Brunner, E., & Rothenberger, A. (2003). Association of adhd D., Butler, N., . . . & Hollis, C. (2005). Association of the
and conduct disorder–brain electrical evidence for the dopamine transporter (dat1) 10/10-repeat genotype with
existence of a distinct subtype. Journal of Child Psychology adhd symptoms and response inhibition in a general
and Psychiatry, 44, 356–376. population sample. Mol Psychiatry, 10, 686–698.
Banaschewski, T., Hollis, C., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Doehnert, M., Brandeis, D., Imhof, K., Drechsler, R., &
Rubia, K., Willcutt, E., & Taylor, E. (2005). Towards an Steinhausen, H.C. (2010). Mapping attention-deficit/
understanding of unique and shared pathways in the hyperactivity disorder from childhood to adolescence–no
psychopathophysiology of adhd. Dev Sci, 8, 132–140. neurophysiologic evidence for a developmental lag of attention
Becker, A., Steinhausen, H.C., Baldursson, G., Dalsgaard, S., but some for inhibition. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 608–616.
Lorenzo, M.J., Ralston, S.J., . . . & Rothenberger, A. (2006). Doehnert, M., Brandeis, D., Schneider, G., Drechsler, R., &
Psychopathological screening of children with adhd: Steinhausen, H.C. (2013). A neurophysiological marker of

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
doi:10.1111/jcpp.12212 Genetics of preparation and response control in ADHD 923

impaired preparation in an 11-year follow-up study of . . . & Brandeis, D. (1998). The continuous performance test
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd). Journal of revisited with neuroelectric mapping: Impaired orienting in
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 260–270. children with attention deficits. Behavioural Brain
Doyle, A.E., Willcutt, E.G., Seidman, L.J., Biederman, J., Research, 94, 97–110.
Chouinard, V.A., Silva, J., Faraone, S.V. (2005). Li, D., Sham, P.C., Owen, M.J., & He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder endophenotypes. shows significant association between dopamine system
Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1324–1335. genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd).
Dresler, T., Ehlis, A. C., Heinzel, S., Renner, T.J., Reif, A., Baehne, Human Molecular Genetics, 15, 2276–2284.
C. G., . . . & Fallgatter, A.J. (2010). Dopamine transporter Linssen, A.M., Vuurman, E. F., Sambeth, A., Nave, S., Spooren,
(slc6a3) genotype impacts neurophysiological correlates of W., Vargas, G., . . . & Riedel, W.J. (2011). Contingent negative
cognitive response control in an adult sample of patients with variation as a dopaminergic biomarker: Evidence from
adhd. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 2193–2202. dose-related effects of methylphenidate. Psychopharma-
Durston, S., van Belle, J., & de Zeeuw, P. (2011). cology (Berl), 218, 533–542.
Differentiating frontostriatal and fronto-cerebellar circuits Lutcke, H., Gevensleben, H., Albrecht, B., & Frahm, J. (2009).
in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Brain networks involved in early versus late response
Psychiatry, 69, 1178–1184. anticipation and their relation to conflict processing. J
Fallgatter, A.J., Ehlis, A. C., Seifert, J., Strik, W.K., Cogn Neurosci, 21, 2172–2184.
Scheuerpflug, P., Zillessen, K.E., . . . & Warnke, A. (2004). Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2003). The cnv peak: An index of
Altered response control and anterior cingulate function in decision making and temporal memory. Psychophysiology,
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder boys. Clinical 40, 950–954.
Neurophysiology, 115, 973–981. McLoughlin, G., Albrecht, B., Banaschewski, T., Rothenberger,
Fallgatter, A.J., & Strik, W.K. (1999). The nogo-anteriorization A., Brandeis, D., Asherson, P., & Kuntsi, J. (2010).
as a neurophysiological standard-index for cognitive response Electrophysiological evidence for abnormal preparatory
control. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 32, 233– states and inhibitory processing in adult adhd. Behav
238. Brain Funct, 6, 66.
Faraone, S.V., & Mick, E. (2010). Molecular genetics of Meador-Woodruff, J.H., Damask, S.P., Wang, J., Haroutunian,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics V., Davis, K.L., & Watson, S.J. (1996). Dopamine receptor
of North America, 33, 159–180. mrna expression in human striatum and neocortex.
Franke, B., Vasquez, A.A., Johansson, S., Hoogman, M., Neuropsychopharmacology, 15, 17–29.
Romanos, J., Boreatti-Hummer, A., . . . & Reif, A. (2010). Michel, C.M., & Brandeis, D. (2009). Data acquisition
Multicenter analysis of the slc6a3/dat1 vntr haplotype in standards for electrical neuroimaging. In C.M. Michel, T.
persistent adhd suggests differential involvement of the gene in Koenig, D. Brandeis, L.R.R. Gianotti & J. Wackermann
childhood and persistent adhd. Neuropsychopharmacology, (Eds.), Electrical neuroimaging (pp. 1–24). Cambridge:
35, 656–664. Cambridge University Press.
Fuchs, M., Kastner, J., Wagner, M., Hawes, S., & Ebersole, J.S. Noain, D., Avale, M.E., Wedemeyer, C., Calvo, D., Peper, M., &
(2002). A standardized boundary element method volume Rubinstein, M. (2006). Identification of brain neurons expressing
conductor model. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 702–712. the dopamine d4 receptor gene using bac transgenic mice.
Gizer, I.R., & Waldman, I.D. (2012). Double dissociation European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2429–2438.
between lab measures of inattention and impulsivity and Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2002). Standardized low-resolution
the dopamine transporter gene (dat1) and dopamine d4 brain electromagnetic tomography (sloreta): Technical
receptor gene (drd4). Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, details. Methods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical
1011–1023. Pharmacology, 24(Suppl D), 5–12.
Grubbs, F. (1969). Procedures for detecting outlying Perchet, C., Revol, O., Fourneret, P., Mauguiere, F., &
observations in samples. Technometrics, 11, 1–21. Garcia-Larrea, L. (2001). Attention shifts and anticipatory
Heinzel, S., Dresler, T., Baehne, C.G., Heine, M., mechanisms in hyperactive children: An erp study using the
Boreatti-Hummer, A., Jacob, C.P., . . . & Ehlis, A.C. (2012). posner paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 44–57.
Comt x drd4 epistasis impacts prefrontal cortex function Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (2007). Research on attention
underlying response control. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1453–1462. networks as a model for the integration of psychological
Hoegl, T., Heinrich, H., Albrecht, B., Diruf, M., Moll, G.H., & science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23.
Kratz, O. (2011). Interplay of neuronal processes during Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A., Sarason, I., Bransome, E.D., &
response inhibition: Results from a combined event-related Beck, L.H. (1956). A continuous performance test of brain
potentials (erps)/transcranial magnetic stimulation (tms) damage. J Consult Psychol, 20, 343–350.
study on methylphenidate. International Journal of Spencer, T., Biederman, J., & Wilens, T. (2000).
Psychophysiology, 81, 99–106. Pharmacotherapy of attention deficit hyperactivity
Johnson, K.A., Kelly, S.P., Robertson, I.H., Barry, E., Mulligan, disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am, 9, 77–97.
A., Daly, M., . . . & Bellgrove, M.A. (2008). Absence of the Valko, L., Doehnert, M., M€ uller, U.C., Schneider, G., Albrecht,
7-repeat variant of the drd4 vntr is associated with drifting B., Drechsler, R., . . . & Brandeis, D. (2009). Differences in
sustained attention in children with adhd but not in controls. neurophysiological markers of inhibitory and temporal
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet, 147B, 927–937. processing deficits in children and adults with adhd.
Kieling, C., Roman, T., Doyle, A.E., Hutz, M.H., & Rohde, L.A. Journal of Psychophysiology, 23, 235–246.
(2006). Association between drd4 gene and performance of Valko, L., Doehnert, M., M€ uller, U.C., Schneider, G., Albrecht,
children with adhd in a test of sustained attention. Biological B., Drechsler, R., . . . & Brandeis, D. (2006). On the relation
Psychiatry, 60, 1163–1165. of movement-related potentials to the go/no-go effect on p3.
Kratz, O., Studer, P., Baack, J., Malcherek, S., Erbe, K., Moll, Biological Psychology, 73, 298–313.
G.H., & Heinrich, H. (2012). Differential effects of Walter, W.G., Cooper, R., Aldridge, V.J., McCallum, W.C., &
methylphenidate and atomoxetine on attentional processes Winter, A.L. (1964). Contingent negative variation: An
in children with adhd: An event-related potential study using electric sign of sensorimotor association and expectancy in
the attention network test. Progress in Neuro- the human brain. Nature, 203, 380–384.
Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 37, 81–89.
van Leeuwen, T.H., Steinhausen, H. C., Overtoom, C.C., Accepted for publication: 28 December 2013
Pascual-Marqui, R.D., van’t Klooster, B., Rothenberger, A., Published online: 13 February 2014

© 2014 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. © 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

También podría gustarte