Está en la página 1de 10

Redalyc

Sistema de Información Científica


Red de Revistas Científicas de América Latina, el Caribe, España y Portugal

López-Jáuregui, Alicia; Oliden, Paula Elosua

Adaptation of the ESPA29 Parental Socialization Styles Scale to the Basque Language:
Evidence of Validity
The Spanish Journal of Psychology, Vol. 12, Núm. 2, noviembre-sin mes, 2009, pp. 737
-745
Universidad Complutense de Madrid
España

Disponible en: http://redalyc.uaemex.mx/src/inicio/ArtPdfRed.jsp?iCve=17213008033

The Spanish Journal of Psychology


ISSN (Versión impresa): 1138-7416
psyjour@sis.ucm.es
Universidad Complutense de Madrid
España

¿Cómo citar? Número completo Más información del artículo Página de la revista

www.redalyc.org
Proyecto académico sin fines de lucro, desarrollado bajo la iniciativa de acceso abierto
The Spanish Journal of Psychology Copyright 2009 by The Spanish Journal of Psychology
2009, Vol. 12, No. 2, 737-745 ISSN 1138-7416

Adaptation of the ESPA29 Parental Socialization Styles


Scale to the Basque Language: Evidence of Validity

Alicia López-Jáuregui and Paula Elosua Oliden


Universidad del País Vasco (Spain)

The aim of this study is to adapt the ESPA29 scale of parental socialization styles in
adolescence to the Basque language. The study of its psychometric properties is based on
the search for evidence of internal and external validity. The first focuses on the assessment
of the dimensionality of the scale by means of exploratory factor analysis. The relationship
between the dimensions of parental socialization styles and gender and age guarantee the
external validity of the scale. The study of the equivalence of the adapted and original
versions is based on the comparisons of the reliability coefficients and on factor congruence.
The results allow us to conclude the equivalence of the two scales.
Keywords: parental socialization styles, adolescence, test adaptation.

El objetivo de este trabajo es adaptar al euskera la escala de estilos de socialización


parental ESPA29. El estudio de sus propiedades psicométricas descansa en la búsqueda
de evidencias internas y externas de validez. Las primeras se centran en la evaluación
de la dimensionalidad de la escala a través de un análisis factorial exploratorio. La relación
entre las distintas dimensiones de la socialización y las variables sexo y edad garantizan
la validez externa de la escala. El estudio de equivalencia entre las versiones original y
adaptada se apoya en la comparación entre los coeficientes de fiabilidad y de la congruencia
factorial. Los resultados permiten concluir la equivalencia entre las dos escalas.
Palabras clave: estilos de socialización parental, adolescencia, adaptación de tests.

This work was developed within the framework of research projects: SEJ2005-01694 and PSI2008-00856, financed by the Ministry
of Education and Science and the GIU 08/17, subsidized by the University of the Basque Country.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alicia López Jáuregui. Departamento de Psicología Social y Metodología
de las Ciencias del Comportamiento. Facultad de Psicología, Universidad del País Vasco, Avenida de Tolosa, 70, 20018 San Sebastián-
Guipúzcoa (Spain). Phone: 34-943- 018340. E-mail: alicia.lopez@ehu.es

737
738 LÓPEZ AND ELOSUA

The role of the family as the transmitter of values, Paulson & Sputa, 1996; Shucksmith, Hendry, & Glendinning,
attitudes, and behaviors is unquestionable. Hence, the interest 1995; Smetana, 1995; Steinberg et al., 1994; Villalobos,
of sociologists, psychologists, and pedagogues has focused Cruz, & Sánchez, 2004.) Involvement refers the support,
on analyzing the relationships within the family. Processes acceptance, and warmth shown by parents to their children,
of parental socialization and their consequences on children and supervision refers to their degree of control, demands,
have been the object of research in the cross-cultural (Arnett, and discipline. The combination of these two independent
1995; Scarr, 1993), psychological and educational dimensions dimensions leads to the definition of four types of parental
(Musitu & Allat, 1994). socialization styles: (a) authoritative, with high involvement
Adolescence is a critical stage in the lives of individuals, and high supervision; (b) permissive or indulgent, with
in which they consolidate their values and identity and they high involvement and low supervision; (c) authoritarian,
conquer autonomy (Allen, Hauser, Bell, & O´Connor, 1993; with low involvement and high supervision; and (d)
Collins, 1990), achievements that frequently lead to negligent, with low involvement and low supervision
psychological and relational tensions and the risk of (Baumrind, 1991; Lamborn et al., 1991; Musitu & García,
problematic or maladaptive behaviors (Moore & Rosenthal, 2004; Martínez & García, 2007; Steinberg et al., 1994;
1993). Villalobos et al., 2004)
Many studies coincide in stating that the quality of The methods used to assess parental practices have
relationships in adolescence determines children’s adjustment evolved since the first observational studies and parents’
and psychological well-being. Parents’ warmth, proximity, reports (interviews and questionnaires), moving towards
and involvement, combined with vigilance and control, the preferential use of the children’s report (retrospective
contribute to good psychosocial, academic, and behavioral or otherwise) of their parents’ behavior (Locke & Prinz,
adjustment (Martínez & García, 2007; Oliva, Parra, & 2002). In this sense, a large number of instruments (Holden
Sánchez-Quejía, 2002; Steinberg, Darling, & Fletcher, 1995; & Edwards, 1989) have been designed to assess the
Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992), higher social children’s perception of their parents’ behavior, among
competence and autonomy (Lamborn, Mounts, Steinberg, which the following are noteworthy: the Parental Attitude
& Dornbusch, 1991), positive attitudes towards school and Research Instrument and the Child’s Report of Parent
work, academic achievement, and self-esteem (Linver & Behavior Inventory (CRPBI; Droppleman & Schaefer,
Silverberg, 1997; Maccoby & Martin, 1983; Steinberg, 1963), the Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostran (EMBU;
Lamborn, Darling, Mounts, & Dornbusch, 1994; Steinberg, Perris, Jacobsson, Lindstrom, Von Knorring, & Perris, 1980),
Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992), as well as a lower the Parental Bond Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tupling, &
incidence of depression, school problems, delinquency, and Brown, 1979), the Measure of Parenting Style (MOPS;
drug abuse (Baumrind, 1971; Darling & Steinberg, 1993; Parker, Roussos, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Mitchell, Wilhelm &
Jacobson & Crockett, 2000; Parish & McCluskey, 1992). Austin, 1997) and the “Escala de socialización parental”
In contrast, hostility and the use of punishments and coercion (ESPA29; translated: the Parental Socialization Scale;
combined with scarce vigilance and control contribute to Musitu & García, 2001).
the emergence of problematic and antisocial behaviors in The Parental Socialization Scale (ESPA29) analyzed herein
adolescence (Conger, Patterson, & Ge, 1995; Dishion, assesses the socialization style of the father and the mother
Patterson, Stoolmiller, & Skinner, 1991). separately, and is substantially different from the rest of the
However, the impact of parental practices on the children above-mentioned instruments in that this assessment is carried
is not independent of the social and ethnic-cultural context out with reference to specific daily life scenarios from the
in which the family system is inset (Lin & Fu, 1990; western culture. This scale has been standardized and validated
Martínez & García, 2007; Zern, 1984). For example, whereas in the population of Spanish adolescents and used in various
in Asian cultures, high levels of discipline and imposition works (Cerezo, 2006; Martínez & García, 2007, 2008;
have a positive influence on the children (Chao, 2001), in Martínez, García, & Yubero, 2007). It is based on the
western cultures, optimum adjustment is obtained with high previously described model, and the two socialization axes
levels of affection, acceptance, and involvement. The are named involvement/acceptance and coercion/imposition.
explanation of these cross-cultural divergences lies in the The acceptance/involvement dimension is positively related
diverse meanings of socialization practices, depending on to parents’ reactions of approval and affection in the case of
the cultural context (Chao, 1994). rule-abiding behaviors, or of dialogue in the case of
In order to systematize parental socialization practices adolescents’ inadequate or objectionable behaviors. In contrast,
and explain their effect on children, traditionally, two the involvement/acceptance dimension is negatively related
independent constructs have been postulated, which, in to parents’ indifference to adequate behaviors and to their
general terms, could be called involvement and supervision displeasure when rules are broken. The coercion/imposition
(Barber, Chadwick, & Oerter, 1992; Barnes & Farrell, 1992; dimension is positively related to parents’ actions of verbal
Chao,2001; Foxcroft & Lowe, 1991; Lamborn et al., 1991; coercion, physical coercion, or deprivation when faced with
Martínez & García, 2007; Musitu & García, 2001, 2004; adolescents’ inadequate behaviors.
ADAPTATION OF THE ESPA29 TO THE BASQUE LANGUAGE 739

The construct of socialization style, besides its defining initial sample of 1384 participants, we only retained those
dimensions, has other distinctive characteristics that are the who responded to at least 50% of the items in each one of
object of analysis in the adaptation of the ESPA29. Among the scales (referring both to the father and the mother), so
them is its developmental and differential nature. Socialization the final sample comprised 1184 participants. Of them, 598
patterns develop over time and differ as a function of sex are boys and 586 are girls. Table 1 shows the distribution
(Musitu & García, 2001). In general, as children grow up of the Basque-speaking sample as a function of sex and
and increase their autonomy, the parents’ influence decreases age, under the column Adapted Sample
and, as a consequence, their socialization techniques change. Selection of the adapted sample was carried out in two
Regarding sex differences, the more frequent use of the stages: in the first one, the population was stratified as a
acceptance/involvement style with daughters and the more function of two criteria: public/private/concerted center and
frequent use of punishment and deprivation with sons are rural/urban center. Once the population was defined, in the
congruent with the different roles our society assigns to second stage of sample selection, the selection criteria were
men and women (Calhoun, Light, & Keller, 2000). incidental. The scales were administered by psychology
Within this theoretical framework that defines parental students who were informed about the goals of the investigation
socialization styles from a bidimensional and dynamic model, and trained in the method of administration of the test.
the goal of this work is to analyze the ESPA29 version adapted
to the Basque language. This goal is upheld by two pillars; Instrument
on the one hand, the standards of the elaboration and use of
tests provided by the American Psychological Association The Parental Socialization Styles Scale (ESPA29) presents
(APA), American Educational Research Association (AERA), 29 relevant daily life situations (i.e., “If I fight with a friend
and the National Council on Measurement in Education or with one of my neighbors”), about which the adolescent
(NCME) (1999; Elosua, 2003a) and, on the other, the guidelines assesses the frequency of parents’ different reactions (i.e.,
of the International Test Commission (ITC) on the adaptation “—- hits me,” “—-talks to me,” “—-doesn’t care,” “—-
of tests (ITC, 2001; Elosua, 2003b). Within this methodological scolds me”), referring both to the father and the mother.
context, the goal of the work is to provide evidence that justifies In total, there are 106 items with four response categories
the use of the adapted version of the ESPA29. (never, sometimes, frequently, always), belonging to seven
subscales: affection, dialogue, displeasure, indifference,
physical coercion, coercion verbal, and deprivation, referring
Method both to the father and the mother, the first four are
components of the acceptance/involvement dimension and
Participants the last three of the coercion/imposition dimension.

The sample used in the adaptation comprises students Adaptation


aged between 10 and 17 years, attending school in the three
provinces of the Autonomous Basque Community at the The linguistic adaptation of the ESPA29 to the Basque
educational levels of Primary and Secondary Education language followed the guidelines set out for inverse translation.
and High School, and who are studying the linguistic model This is a judgment technique to maintain the linguistic quality
D (in this model, the Basque language is the vehicular of adaptations (Elosua, 2003a), that has basically three stages:
language for all the subjects except for Spanish). Of the in the first stage, the test is adapted to the target language;

Table 1
Distribution of the samples by sex and age
Adapted sample Original sample
Age (years)
Sons Daughters Total Sons Daughters Total
10 70 66 136 68 69 137
11 84 74 158 109 134 243
12 62 57 119 96 128 224
13 78 71 149 196 245 441
14 70 96 166 141 186 327
15 87 69 156 99 175 274
16 79 73 152 45 66 111
17 68 80 148 3 40 43
Total 598 586 1184 757 1043 1800
740 LÓPEZ AND ELOSUA

in the second one, a group of independent translators one (χ2[76, N = 1184] = 5853.22, RMSEA = .25, GFI =
retranslates the adapted version back to the original language; .64), we focused the analysis on the study of factor
and lastly, the possible divergences between the two versions congruence, which is operationalized by the Tucker and
in the source language are studied and refined. Each one of Levine indexes (Levine, 1977; Tucker, 1951).
the stages was carried out by a professional translator. The significance of the differences observed in the
subscales with regard to sex and age was assessed within
Analysis the general linear model using multivariate analysis of factor
variance and univariate analysis for each dependent variable
The validity of the results of the scale was analyzed using separately. Before carrying out these analyses, we analyzed
two sources of evidence: internal and external. As an internal the differences among the age groups with a one-factor
source of evidence, we examined the dimensionality of the design. As no significant differences were found between
instrument. In order to examine in depth the relationships the pairs of 10-11 years, 12-13 years, 14-15 years, and 16-
of the scale with external variables, we analyzed the relations 17 years, the 8 age levels of this study were recoded.
among the dimensions of the ESPA29 (adapted version),
sex, and age.
The analysis of the internal structure was performed Results
with exploratory factor analysis to collect evidence in support
of the substantive model (Elosua, 2003b). The factors Descriptive Statistics and Reliability
were extracted using the SPSS 11.5 program with the
principal axes method and the varimax with Kaiser The classic descriptive indexes for each subscale,
normalization rotation method. arithmetical mean, standard deviation, and corrected
The equivalence of the original and the adapted versions discrimination index, estimated with Pearson’s product-
of the ESPA29 was based on the equivalence of the moment correlation coefficient, are shown in Table 2. The
reliability coefficients and the factor congruence. A more global reliability of the scale was estimated with Cronbach’s
rigorous and complete assessment of the metric equivalence internal consistency coefficient, which reached the value
would require the progressive analysis of the factor of .97. Table 3 shows the reliability coefficients of the seven
invariance of the scales corresponding to the two test subscales, both for the father and for the mother. The values
versions (Elosua, 2005b; Elosua & López, 2004); however, are very high, ranging from the minimum value of .835
because of the poor fit obtained in the confirmatory factor for the subscale physical coercion (mother) to the maximum
analysis, both in the original test (χ2 [76, N = 1800 ] = of .940 for the subscale dialogue (father). The similar values
6602.04 root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA] of the same subscales referred to the father and to the mother
= .22, goodness of fit index [GFI] = .71) and in the adapted can be observed.

Table 2
Descriptive indexes and discrimination indexes of the subscales (adapted sample)
Arithmetical mean Standard deviation Discrimination
Subscale
M SD min max M SD min max M SD min max
Father
Dialogue 2.76 .33 2.18 3.34 .99 .07 .90 .99 .61 .04 .52 .68
Affection 2.97 .30 2.55 3.62 .96 .12 .73 .96 .71 .08 .54 .81
Displeasure 1.31 .13 1.15 1.61 .65 .11 .50 .65 .47 .07 .24 .54
Indifference 1.45 .16 1.14 1.70 .79 .15 .46 .79 .61 .11 .40 .74
Physical coercion 1.07 .03 1.04 1.14 .33 .07 .25 .33 .52 .11 .34 .73
Deprivation 1.74 .24 1.20 2.08 .84 .12 .57 .84 .60 .08 .40 .71
Verbal coercion 2.26 .32 1.29 2.62 .92 .10 .59 .92 .55 .09 .29 .66
Mother
Dialogue 2.98 .29 2.43 3.47 .95 .07 .80 1.07 .66 .04 .58 .72
Affection 3.18 .27 2.78 3.72 .92 .14 .63 1.14 .72 .08 .52 .80
Displeasure 1.24 .12 1.10 1.48 .59 .13 .41 .86 .48 .08 .34 .58
Indifference 1.35 .14 1.11 1.60 .72 .15 .43 .95 .58 .10 .34 .72
Physical coercion 1.06 .02 1.02 1.11 .30 .08 .18 .51 .48 .14 .23 .72
Deprivation 1.76 .22 1.25 2.12 .86 .11 .61 1.04 .67 .07 .48 .76
Verbal coercion 2.34 .32 1.41 2.68 .95 .08 .71 1.05 .60 .09 .35 .69
ADAPTATION OF THE ESPA29 TO THE BASQUE LANGUAGE 741

Dimensionality of the Scale

The theoretical model postulates two socialization


dimensions: Acceptance/Involvement and Coercion/Imposition,
referring both to the mother and the father. The former is
positively related to the subscales of affection and dialogue
and negatively to displeasure and indifference. The second
dimension is positively related to the subscales verbal coercion,
deprivation, and physical coercion. This bidimensional
structure was tested by means of exploratory factor analysis.
The loadings for each subscale on the two components are
shown in Table 3. To facilitate interpretation, the highest Figure 1. Scree plot.
loadings on each factor are represented in boldface.
In Figure 1, the sample scree plot shows the inflection the process of creation of the ESPA29 in its original version
point for the Eigenvalue corresponding to the third factor, (Musitu & García, 2001). Their description by sex and age
thus ratifying the plausibility of the bifactorial model. Internal can be seen in Table 1. This sample is made up of 1800
consistency was very high for the two dimensions (.967 for students, of whom 42.1% are boys and 57.9% are girls.
Acceptance/involvement and .963 for Coercion /imposition).
Equivalence of the Internal Consistency Coefficients
Equivalence with the Original Scale
The Cronbach´s alpha coefficient of the original sample
To analyze the equivalence between the original version was .968. The equivalence of the internal consistency
of the ESPA29 and its adaptation to the Basque language, coefficients was assessed with the statistic proposed by
we had to analyze the metric equivalence of both scales. Feldt (1969). The value obtained indicates that we can accept
In order to perform the equivalence analyses, we used as a the hypothesis of the equivalence of the coefficients (w =
reference sample part of the normative group employed in 1.032, p = .276).

Table 3
Factor loadings of the subscales and internal consistency (adapted test)
Scale Factor 1 Factor 2 α coefficient
Father
Affection .728 .036 .939
Indifference –.776 .142 .902
Displeasure –.612 –.013 .846
Dialogue .538 .265 .917
Verbal coercion .129 .759 .895
Deprivation .128 .767 .912
Physical coercion –.126 .396 .878

Mother
Affection .702 .029 .940
Indifference –.753 .120 .889
Displeasure –.558 .006 .846
Dialogue .473 .245 .933
Verbal coercion .112 .737 .913
Deprivation .127 .781 .936
Physical coercion –.081 .398 .835

Eigenvalue 4.179 3.095


Percentage of variance 24.818 2.006
α coefficient .967 .963 .969
742 LÓPEZ AND ELOSUA

Factor Congruence Relation with External Variables

The analysis of the similarity of the factors was In Table 5 are the descriptive statistics of each subscale
performed by means of two indexes; the congruence as a function of age and sex.
coefficient (Tucker, 1951) and the root mean square deviation The 2  4 multivariate analysis of factor variance (Table
(Levine, 1977). Both of them simultaneously assess similarity 6) shows that the main effects were significant, although
of form and magnitude between the diverse factor loadings. there was no significant interaction between them, Λ = 0,964,
The results can be seen in Table 4. F(42, 3450.78) = 1.009, p = .454.
The two indexes provide satisfactory values, indicating In order to analyze the dimensions and the scales that
that we can accept the congruence of the factor configurations contribute to the differences detected in the multivariate
of the adapted and the original test. analysis, we applied a 2  4 factor design (additive model)

Table 4
Factor congruence indexes (adapted test/original test)
Tucker Adequate value Levine Adequate value
Acceptance/involvement .996 .043
≥ .80 ≤ .20
Coercion/imposition .961 .100

Table 5
Means and standard deviations (in brackets) by age group and sex (adapted sample)
10-11 years 12-13 years 14-15years 16-17 years Boy Girl
Acceptance/involvement 3.42 (.38) 3.37 (.36) 3.16 (.45) 3.09 (.44) 3.39 (.96) 3.47 (.37)
Mother
Dialogue 3.04 (.74) 2.98 (.66) 2.95 (.67) 2.93 (.61) 2.89 (.64) 3.06 (.70)
Affection 3.47 (.66) 3.35 (.60) 3.03 (.72) 2.91 (.69) 3.11 (.71) 3.25 (.70)
Displeasure 1.22 (.38) 1.20 (.29) 1.25 (.32) 1.27 (.30) 1.29 (.37) 1.18 (.26)
Indifference 1.23 (.35) 1.25 (.38) 1.36 (.47) 1.44 (.48) 1.37 (.47) 1.27 (.39)
Acceptance/involvement 3.42 (.38) 3.37 (.36) 3.16 (.45) 3.09 (.44) 3.22 (.41) 3.30 (.45)
Father
Dialogue 2.91 (.67) 2.85 (.65) 2.69 (.67) 2.71 (.65) 2.75 (.64) 2.82 (.69)
Affection 3.33 (.68) 3.18 (.63) 2.78 (.73) 2.65 (.71) 2.91 (.74) 3.04 (.74)
Displeasure 1.27 (.40) 1.23 (.26) 1.33 (.37) 1.40 (.38) 1.34 (.38) 1.28 (.35)
Indifference 1.27 (.38) 1.33 (.42) 1.47 (.54) 1.59 (.55) 1.45 (.52) 1.38 (.47)
Coercion/imposition
Mother
Physical coercion 1.14 (.30) 1.05 (.15) 1.04 (.10) 1.01 (.06) 1.06 (.18) 1.05 (.19)
Deprivation 1.98 (.69) 1.96 (.63) 1.66 (.57) 1.48 (.46) 1.81 (.65) 1.71 (.60)
Verbal coercion 2.36 (.68) 2.43 (.63) 2.31 (.62) 2.30 (.57) 2.36 (.62) 2.33 (.64)
Coercion/imposition 1.80 (.40) 1.78 (.35) 1.64 (.33) 1.55 (.27) 1.71 (.36) 1.66 (.35)
Father
Physical coercion 1.15 (.29) 1.06 (.15) 1.06 (.17) 1.02 (.08) 1.08 (.20) 1.07 (.18)
Deprivation 1.95 (.60) 1.91 (.57) 1.65 (.48) 1.47 (.41) 1.78 (.57) 1.70 (.54)
Verbal coercion 2.31 (.62) 2.37 (.58) 2.22 (.58) 2.16 (.50) 2.29 (.58) 2.22 (.57)

Table 6
2  4 factor MANOVA of sex and age group with the two dimensions and subscales (adapted sample)
Source of variation Λ F df between df error p
Age .668 11.947 42 3450.78 < .001
Sex .940 5.248 14 1163 < .001
Age  Sex .964 1.009 42 3450.78 .45
ADAPTATION OF THE ESPA29 TO THE BASQUE LANGUAGE 743

Table 7
2  4 Factor ANOVAs (additive model) of groups of age and sex in the two main dimensions and subscales (adapted sample)
AGE SEX

MOTHER FATHER MOTHER FATHER

F (3, 1179) p F (3, 857) p F (1, 1179) p F (1, 857) p


Acceptance/involvement 25.920 < .001 44.840 < .001 37.970 < .001 14.284 < .001
Dialogue 1.957 .119 7.937 < .001 19.684 < .001 3.828 .050
Affection 47.267 < .001 66.017 < .001 15.507 < .001 13.183 < .001
Displeasure 2.720 .043 11.960 < .001 31.869 < .001 7.776 .005
Indifference 17.195 < .001 26.629 < .001 2.277 < .001 8.424 .004
Coercion/imposition 24.860 < .001 35.627 < .001 3.650 .050 4.959 .026
Physical coercion 28.664 < .001 25.365 < .001 .640 .424 .325 .569
Deprivation 47.826 < .001 55.445 < .001 7.192 .007 4.620 .032
Verbal coercion 2.379 .068 7.499 < .001 .492 .483 3.408 .065

separately for each dependent variable. These ANOVAs mean than the daughters, both in the case of the mother
are summarized in Table 7. (1.81 versus 1.71) and of the father (1.78 versus 1.70).
As shown in the table, there are differences among the
age groups in all the subscales referring to the father, as
well as in the two main dimensions, Acceptance/involvement Conclusions
and Coercion /imposition. Only in the verbal coercion and
dialogue subscales were no significant differences observed, The goal of this work was to construct and analyze the
F(3, 1179) = 2.379, p = .068 and F(3, 1179) = 1.957, p = version adapted to the Basque language of the parental
.119, respectively, both of them referring to the mother. socialization styles scale, ESPA29. The results obtained
Table 5 shows that in the scores in dialogue and affection show that these goals were fulfilled. The version of the
decrease with age, whereas the scores of displeasure and ESPA29 adapted to the Basque language has adequate
indifference increase, and this occurs both in the subscales internal consistency; both the external and the internal
referring to the father and to the mother. evidence analyzed support the validity of the version
Physical coercion and deprivation decrease across the analyzed; lastly, the new scale is equivalent to the original
age levels, whereas verbal coercion increases at first, and test. Within the sources of internal evidence, we analyzed
subsequently drops as of the age of 13 years, and continues dimensionality, the equivalence of the reliability coefficients,
to decrease until the age of 17 years. The variation patterns and factor equivalence. The assessment of the internal
in the three subscales that make up the dimension structure supports the model of parental socialization styles
Coercion/imposition are completely consistent for both parents. postulated by the authors; the new ESPA29 presents a
There were differences between daughters and sons in structure with two dimensions: Acceptance/involvement
the main dimensions and in all the subscales except for and Coercion/imposition.
physical and verbal coercion referred to both parents. As As this is an adaptation, it is essential to study its
seen in Table 5, these differences indicate that the daughters’ equivalence to the original test (Elosua & López, 1999). In
scores in dialogue (3.06) and affection (3.25) that they this sense, the present work shows that the factor congruence
perceive in the mother are higher than those observed by between the scales is high. The factor structure is similar
the sons (2.89 and 3.11, respectively). The differences also in the two samples and in the two tests. The differential
favor the daughters with regard to the degree of dialogue analyses are in accordance with the results of the latest
(2.82) and affection (3.04) observed in the father (versus investigations: parental socialization is different in sons
2.75 and 2.91 in the boys). The opposite is observed with and daughters, and it changes during adolescence (Cross &
regard to the other two components of the dimension; the Madson, 1997; Garside & Klimes-Dougan, 2002; Klimes-
sons scored higher than the daughters in parents’ displeasure Dougan, Brand, Zahn-Waxler, Usher, Hastings, Kendziora
and indifference, both in the case of the father and of the & Garside, 2007). The dimensions of parental socialization
mother. The greatest difference was in the subscale of the behave differently as a function of sex and age.
mother’s displeasure, where the sons obtained a mean of To sum up, the differential analyses performed allow
1.29 versus the daughters’ mean of 1.18. The only subscale us to state that parental style in the dimensions
of the Coercion/imposition dimension with significant Acceptance/involvement and Coercion/imposition follows
differences was deprivation, where the sons obtained a higher the same developmental pattern in both parents: an
744 LÓPEZ AND ELOSUA

uninterrupted decrease associated with the increasing age and cognition. In R. Montemayor, G.R. Adams, & T.P. Gullotta
of the children. In contrast, both the father and the mother (Eds.), From childhood to adolescence: A transitional period?
used Acceptance/involvement more frequently with their (Advances in adolescent development). Vol. 2 (pp. 86-106).
daughters than with their sons during the interval studied, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
but their degree of Coercion/imposition was similar for Conger, R. D., Patterson, G. R., & Ge, X. (1995). It takes two to
sons and daughters; however, they resorted more frequently replicate: A mediational model for the impact of parents’ stress
to deprivation with their sons than with their daughters. on adolescent adjustment, Child Development, 66, 80-97.
Ultimately, the version of ESPA29 adapted to the Cross, S., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals
Basque language meets the psychometric conditions and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37.
demanded of a test (Elosua, 2005a); it is consistent, it Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting. Psychological
presents an internal structure in accordance with the Bulletin, 113, 487-496.
substantive model, it is sensitive to external variables, and, Dishion, T.J., Patterson G.R., Stoolmiller M., & Skinner, M.L.
moreover, it is equivalent to the original version (1991). Family, school, and behavioural antecedents to early
adolescent involvement with antisocial peers, Developmental
Psychology, 27, 172-180.
References Droppleman, L. F., & Schaefer, E.S. (1963). Boys’ and girls’ reports
of maternal and paternal behavior, Journal of Abnormal and
Allen, J. P., Hauser, S.T., Bell K. L., & O’Connor T. G. (1993). Social Psychology, 67, 648-654.
Longitudinal assessment of autonomy and relatedness in family Elosua, P. (2003a). Testen euskaratzea. Balizko alborapenaren
interactions as predictors of adolescent ego development and iturriak. Tantak, 30, 17-38.
self-esteem, Child Development, 65, 179-194. Elosua, P. (2003b). Sobre la validez de los tests. Psicothema, 15,
American Psychological Association, American Educational 315-321.
Research Association, & National Council on Measurement Elosua, P. (2005a). Psikometria. Testen eraketa eta erabilpena.
in Education (1999). Standards for educational and Bilbao (Spain): Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea.
psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Elosua, P. (2005b). Evaluación progresiva de la invarianza factorial
Association. entre las versiones original y adaptada de una escala de
Arnett, J.J. (1995). Broad and narrow socialization: The family autoconcepto). Psicothema, 17, 356-362.
in the context of a cultural theory. Journal of Marriage and Elosua, P., & López, A. (1999). Funcionamiento diferencial de
the Family, 57, 617-628. los ítems y sesgo en la adaptación de dos pruebas verbales.
Barber, B. K., Chadwick, B. A., & Oerter, R. (1992). Parental Psicológica, 20, 23-40.
behaviors and adolescent self-esteem in the United States and Elosua, P., & López, A. (2004, September). Análisis factorial
Germany. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 128-141. multigupo. Paper presented at the III Congreso de Metodología
Barnes, G.M., & Farrell, M. (1992). Parental support and control de Encuestas. Granada (Spain).
as predictor of adolescent drinking, delinquency, and related Feldt, L.S. (1969). A test of the hypothesis that Cronbach’s alpha
problem behaviors. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, or Kuder-Richardson coefficient twenty is the same for two
763-776. tests. Psychometrika, 34, 363-373.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Harmonious parents and their preschool Foxcroft, D.R., & Lowe, G. (1991). Adolescents’ drinking behaviour
children. Developmental Psychology, 41, 92-102. and family socialization factors: A meta-analysis. Journal of
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent Adolescents 14, 255-273.
competence and substance use. The Journal of Early Garside, R.B., & Klimes-Dougan, B. (2002). Socialization of
Adolescence, 11, 56-95. discrete negative emotions: Gender differences and links with
Calhoun, G., Light, D., & Keller, S. (2000). Sociología. Madrid: psychological distress. Sex Roles, 47, 115-128.
McGraw-Hill. Holden, G., & Edwards, L. (1989). Parental attitudes toward child
Cerezo, F. (2006). Comparative analysis of differential rearing: Instruments, issues, and implications. Psychological
socioemotional variables among those involved in bullying: Bulletin, 106, 29-58.
Study of a bully-victim case. Annuary of Clinical and Health International Test Commission (2001). International guidelines
Psychology, 2, 27-34. for test use. International Journal of Testing, 1, 93-114.
Chao, R.K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian Jacobson, K.C., & Crockett, L.J. (2000). Parental monitoring and
parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the adolescent adjustment: An ecological perspective. Journal of
cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111-1119. Research on Adolescence, 10, 65-97.
Chao, R.K. (2001). Extending research on the consequences of Klimes-Dougan, B., Brand, A.E., Zahn-Waxler, C., Usher, B.,
parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Hastings, P.D., Kendziora, K., & Garside, R.B. (2007).
Child Development, 72, 1832-1843. Parental emotion socialization in adolescence: Differences
Collins, W.A. (1990). Parent-child relationships and the transition in sex, age and problem status. Social Development, 16, 326-
to adolescence: Continuity and change in interaction, affect 342.
ADAPTATION OF THE ESPA29 TO THE BASQUE LANGUAGE 745

Lamborn, S.D., Mounts, N.S., Steinberg, L., & Dornbusch, S.M. measure of dysfunctional parenting and assessment of its
(1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment from relevance in patients with affective disorders. Psychological
authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent and neglectful families. Medicine 27, 1193-1203.
Child Development, 62, 1049-1065. Paulson, S.E., & Sputa, C.L. (1996). Patterns of parenting during
Levine, M.S. (1977). Canonical correlation analysis and factor adolescence: Perceptions of adolescents and parents.
comparison techniques. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Adolescence, 31, 369-381.
Lin, C.C., & Fu, V.R. (1990). A comparison of child-rearing Perris, C., Jacobsson, L., Lindstromm H., Von Knorring, L., &
practices among Chinese, immigrant Chinese, and Caucasian Perris, H. (1980). Development of a new inventory for assessing
American parents. Child Development, 61, 429-433. memories of parental rearing behaviour. Acta Psychiatrica
Linver; M.S., & Silverberg, S.B. (1997). Maternal predictors of Scandinavica, 61, 265-274.
early adolescent achievement-related outcomes: Adolescent gender Scarr, S. (1993). Biological and cultural diversity: The legacy of
as moderator. Journal of Early Adolescence, 17, 294-318. Darwin for development. Child Development, 64, 1333-1353.
Locke L.M., & Prinz, R.J. (2002). Measurement of parental Shucksmith, J., Hendry, L.B., & Glendinning, A. (1995). Models
discipline and nurturance, Clinical Psychology Review, 22, of parenting: Implications for adolescent well-being within
895-930. different types of family contexts. Journal of Adolescence,
Maccoby, E.E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context 18, 253-270.
of the family: Parent-child interactions. In E.M. Heterington Smetana, J.G. (1995). Parenting styles and conceptions of parental
& P.H. Mussen (Eds.), Handbook of child Psychology (Vol. authority during adolescence. Child Development, 66, 299-
4, pp. 1-102). New York: Wiley. 316.
Martínez, I., & García, J.F. (2007). Impact of parenting styles Steinberg, L., Darling, N., & Fletcher, A.C. (1995). Authoritative
on adolescents’ self-esteem and internalization of values in parenting and adolescent adjustment: An ecological journey.
Spain. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10, 338-348. In P. Moen, G.H. Elder, Jr., & K. Luscher (Eds.), Examining
Martínez, I., García, J.F., & Yubero, S. (2007). Parenting styles lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human
and adolescents’ self-esteem in Brazil. Psychological Reports, development (pp. 423-466). Washington, DC: American
100, 731-745. Psychological Association.
Martínez I., & García, J.F. (2008). Internalization of values and Steinberg, L., Dornbusch, S.D., & Brown, B.B. (1992). Ethnic
self-esteem among Brazilian teenagers from authoritative, differences in adolescent achievement. An ecological
indulgent, authoritarian, and neglectful homes. Adolescence, perspective. American Psychologist, 47, 723-729.
43, 13-29. Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S.D., Darling, N. Mounts, N.S., &
Moore, S.M., & Rosenthal, D.A. (1993). Venturesomeness, Dornbusch, S.M. (1994). Over-time changes in adjustment
impulsiveness, and risky behaviour among older adolescents, and competence among adolescents from authoritative,
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 98. authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child
Musitu, G., & Allat, P. (1994). Psicosociología de la familia. Development, 65, 754-770.
Valencia: Albatros. Steinberg, L., Lamborn S.D., Dornbusch, S.M., & Darling, N.
Musitu, G., & García, F. (2001). ESPA29. Escala de Estilos de (1992). Impact of parenting practices on adolescent
Socialización Parental en la Adolescencia. Madrid: TEA achievement: Authoritative parenting, school involvement, and
Ediciones. encouragement to succeed, Child Development, 65, 1266-1281.
Musitu, G., & García, J.F. (2004). Consecuencias de la socialización Villalobos, J.A., Cruz, A.V., & Sánchez, P.R. (2004). Estilos
familiar en la cultura española. Psicothema, 16, 288-293. parentales y desarrollo psicosocial en estudiantes de
Oliva, A., Parra, A., & Sánchez-Quejía, I. (2002). Relaciones con Bachillerato. Revista Mexicana de Psicología, 21, 119-129.
padres e iguales como predictoras del ajuste emocional y Tucker, L.R. (1951). A method for synthesis of factor analysis
conductual durante la adolescencia. Apuntes de Psicología, studies. Personnel Research Sections. Report 984. Washington,
20, 3-6 DC: Department of the Army.
Parish, T.S., & McCluskey, J.J. (1992). The relationship between Zern, D.S. (1984). Relationships among selected child-rating
parenting styles and young adults’ self-concepts and evaluations variables in a cross-cultural sample of 110 societies.
of parents, Adolescence, 27, 915-918. Developmental Psychology, 20, 683-690.
Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L.B. (1979). A parental bonding
instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1-10. Received: February 2, 2008
Parker, G., Roussos, J., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Mitchell, P., Wilhelm, Revision received: November 28, 2008
K., & Austin, M.P. (1997). The development of a refined Accepted: January 19, 2009

También podría gustarte