Está en la página 1de 12

J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

DOI 10.1007/s10964-009-9478-9

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Clueless or Powerful? Identifying Subtypes of Bullies


in Adolescence
Margot Peeters • Antonius H. N. Cillessen •

Ron H. J. Scholte

Received: 8 August 2009 / Accepted: 6 November 2009 / Published online: 22 November 2009
Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Abstract This study examined the heterogeneity of bul- At the same time, other results associate bullying with
lying among adolescents. It was hypothesized that bullying good understanding of social relationships, social power,
behavior serves different social functions and, depending and covered aggressive assaults directed at the victim
on these functions, bullies will differ in their skills, status (Salmivalli et al. 2000; Sutton et al. 1999a). This duality
and social behavior. In a total sample of 806 8th graders, has lead researchers to the assumption that bullies are not a
120 adolescents (52 boys, 68 girls) were identified as uniform group. Indeed, previous studies have examined the
bullies based on peer nominations. An additional group of heterogeneity of bullying. For example, Salmivalli and
50 adolescents (25 boys, 25 girls) served as the non-bully Nieminen (2002) made a distinction between bullies and
comparison group. Cluster analysis revealed three corre- bully-victims (students who bully but are also bullied), and
sponding bully subtypes for boys and girls: a popular- found that bully-victims were the most aggressive sub-
socially intelligent group, a popular moderate group, and group of bullies. Another approach to the heterogeneity of
an unpopular-less socially intelligent group. Follow-up bullying is to examine different roles in the bullying pro-
analyses showed that the clusters differed significantly cess. Salmivalli et al. (1996), identified roles such as
from each other in physical and verbal aggression, lead- ringleader bullies versus assistants, and associated these
ership, network centrality, peer rejection, and self-percep- roles with social status. These studies describe different
tions of bullying. The results confirm the heterogeneous types of bullies; their results indicate that bullying behavior
nature of bullies and the complex nature of bullying in the varies among adolescents.
adolescent peer group. In spite of these developments, more can be learned
about the heterogeneity of bullying. For example, few
Keywords Bullying  Social intelligence  studies have explored differences in social skills among
Machiavellianism  Popularity  Relational aggression bullies and their associations with different social functions
of bullying. It may also be the case, consistent with the
work of Salmivalli et al. (1996) that different bully roles or
Introduction functions are associated with differences in social status.
Furthermore, social-cognitive skills may differ between
Lacking empathy or social understanding and being types of bullies. Bullies may be found along the range of
exceptionally aggressive have traditionally been associated the social competence continuum and vary in social intel-
with bullying (Pellegrini et al. 1999; Sutton et al. 1999a). ligence. A broader picture of the heterogeneous nature of
bullying is needed as our understanding of the behavior of
bullies affects the design and effectiveness of interventions
M. Peeters (&)  A. H. N. Cillessen  R. H. J. Scholte
(Arsenio and Lemerise 2001; Sutton et al. 1999b).
Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen,
P.O. Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands The goal of this study was exactly this: to examine the
e-mail: peetersmargot@gmail.com social functions of bullying and to identify different types
A. H. N. Cillessen of bullies based on patterns of behaviors, status, and social
e-mail: a.cillessen@psych.ru.nl skills. Below, these constructs that may differentiate bullies

123
1042 J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

from each other are discussed. Specifically, differences structure and obtain more power. Sutton et al. argued that
between bullies are addressed in terms of social intelli- the effectiveness of bullying is determined by the bully’s
gence, Machiavellian beliefs, popularity, and aggression. ability to attribute internal mental states to themselves and
others, or ‘‘theory of mind’’ (Bartsch and Wellman 1989).
Do Bullies Differ in Social Intelligence? This awareness of the thoughts and beliefs of others enables
them to predict and anticipate their behavior (Sutton et al.
The social information processing framework (SIP) has 1999b). Understanding the perspective of others and being
been used in previous research to understand children’s able to manipulate their thoughts demonstrates the presence
social behavior and adjustment. In a reformulation of this of social intelligence. Therefore, Sutton et al. (1999b)
framework, Crick and Dodge (1994) distinguished six steps argued that at least some bullies are socially intelligent and
(p. 76) in a child’s behavioral response to a social stimulus: have superior theory of mind skills.
(1) encoding of cues, (2) interpretation of these cues, (3) Previous studies indicate that the use of relational
clarification of goals, (4) response access or construction, aggression sometimes demands social intelligence. For
(5) response decision, and (6) behavioral enactment. example, Björkqvist and colleagues found that the efficient
According to this model, incompetent, maladaptive use of relational aggression requires social intelligence.
behaviors, such as persistent aggression, are the result of This sophisticated form of aggression demands under-
deficits in one or more of these steps (Sutton et al. 1999a). standing social relations (Björkqvist et al. 1992, 2000).
Although the model explains a range of social behaviors Putallaz et al. (2007) also suggested that a lack of social
(Crick and Dodge 1999), it has primarily been applied to intelligence may lead to an insufficient aggressive attack.
the display of aggression. The SIP framework provides an These interpretations suggest that some bullies may be
understanding of the social information processing of socially intelligent, depending on the type of aggression
children’s aggression in general. Even though the model that they use to harass their victims.
does not aim specifically at bullying, Crick and Dodge These two theoretical models imply different assump-
(1999) argued that bullying is a type of aggression, and tions about the causes of bullying. The SIP framework
therefore the model would apply to bullying as well. assumes that bullying is a result of social deficits. Crick
Crick and Dodge (1994) proposed that chronic aggression and Dodge (1999) describe the bully as socially inadequate
is the result of chronic processing styles. These persisting and define bullying as maladaptive behavior. Sutton and
processing styles are called biases or deficits because they colleagues see bullying as socially undesirable, but argue
are the tendency to respond in a consistent manner to that bullying may be very efficient, depending on the
ambiguous social cues. The SIP framework emphasizes that context in which it occurs (Sutton et al. 2001). Because
not all aggression is maladaptive or incompetent. Incidental both theoretical assumptions are supported by empirical
self defense, for example, is not the result of processing results, it seems incorrect to reject one of them. The
deficits, but is an adaptive response to threatening social solution is to assume that some bullies are socially intel-
situations (Crick and Dodge 1999). But the authors argue that ligent and victimize for their personal advantage. Others
persisting aggression, such as bullying, is the result of a may be less socially intelligent, and may attribute hostile
deficient information processing style. They acknowledge intent to others or are unable or unwilling to choose an
that some bullies may be proficient at a particular step of the appropriate response (Archer 2001; Crick and Dodge
SIP cycle, but they are deficient in at least one of the six SIP 1994). Therefore, we hypothesize that bullies are a heter-
steps. They reject the possibility that completely competent ogeneous group and can be found at both extremes of the
social cognitions can precede maladaptive behavior, such as social competence continuum.
bullying (Arsenio and Lemerise 2001; Crick and Dodge
1999). A study by Coie et al. (1991) verified this idea. They Do Bullies Differ in Machiavellian Beliefs?
found that aggressive children, such as bullies, attribute
hostile intentions to peers and subsequently react with Beliefs and impressions that we have about others are a
exaggerated aggression. This indicates that aggressive chil- result of previous social experiences. These experiences not
dren incorrectly encode or interpret social cues and exhibit only affect the impressions of specific others but also our
behavioral responses that are not normative. general belief in human nature (Sutton and Keogh 2001).
In contrast, Sutton et al. (1999b) introduced a different Differences in the perception of the human nature have been
theoretical framework that challenges the stereotypical described as part of the construct of Machiavellianism
view of the bully. According to these authors, bullying is an (Christie and Geis 1970; Sutton and Keogh 2001). Christie
interaction between two or more participants in a structured and Geis (1970) characterized the ‘‘Machiavellian’’ indi-
group. Some bullies misuse their skills to manipulate the vidual as someone who restrains emotional involvement
relationships in the group with the intention to change its in interpersonal relations and has little concerns with

123
J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052 1043

conventional standards of morality. The Machiavellian style, perceived popularity indicates social power and
easily lies and deceives and effectively controls interactions reputation and represents a more dominant and aggressive
with others. They also believe that they can easily manip- behavioral style (de Bruyn and Cillessen 2006; Parkhurst
ulate others in social and interpersonal situations (Andreou and Hopmeyer 1998; Rodkin et al. 2000). Thus, although
2004). both popularity types indicate high status, they reflect
Christie and Geis’ (1970) definition of the Machiavel- different behavioral profiles.
lian presents the image of a person lacking empathy and Bullying has traditionally been linked to peer rejection.
affect, a characteristic that has also been found in bullies. It has been suggested that aggression and disruptive
Gini (2006), for example, found that bullying is associated behavior, like bullying, is related to low acceptance (Coie
with moral disengagement and a lack of emotional under- et al. 1982; Dodge et al. 1982). Indeed, a negative asso-
standing. Bullies are less empathic than their peers (Gini ciation between bullying and peer acceptance has been
et al. 2007) and more likely justify the use of aggressive established (Pellegrini et al. 1999). However, recent studies
behavior without feeling guilty (Gini 2006). Similarly, have shown that aggressive children, such as bullies, can be
Björkqvist et al. (2000) found that aggression is associated disliked by their peers but also perceived as popular
with a lack of empathic responsiveness. Andreou (2004) (Caravita et al. 2008; Cillessen and Rose 2005; Farmer
found that bullying was associated with a lack of faith in et al. 2003). These studies indicate that there are differ-
human nature, and specifically for girls with acceptance of ences among bullies in peer status.
manipulation in social interactions. Estell et al. (2007) hypothesized that bullies would be
Sutton and Keogh (2000) suggested that there is an rated as popular but would not be well-liked by their peers.
association between Machiavellianism and relational Indeed, they found that bullies were rejected by peers
aggression. Several studies support this expectation. (disliked) or socially controversial (liked by some and
Björkqvist et al. (1992) defined indirect aggression as a way disliked by others). At the same time, they were perceived
to inflict pain on others without being identified as the per- as central members of the peer group and socially well-
petrator. Spreading rumors and manipulating relationships integrated. Findings by Vaillancourt et al. (2003) also
(i.e., relational aggression) are such indirect means to pro- supported the perception of the bully as either a rejected
voke others that are also typical Machiavellian traits. Fur- member of the peer group, or a popular member. There-
thermore, Sutton and Keogh (2000) found that bullies hold fore, we assume that bullies differ in their popularity.
more Machiavellian beliefs than non-bullies. They proposed
that different types of Machiavellianism may exist among Do Bullies Differ in Relational Aggression?
different groups. Relationally aggressive bullies, for exam-
ple, may have much stronger Machiavellian beliefs than Spreading rumors, telling lies, gossiping and social exclu-
physically aggressive bullies. sion are sophisticated forms of aggression that are defined
Thus, previous research suggests that some bullies are as indirect or relational aggression (Björkqvist et al. 1992;
effective in controlling and manipulating social situations. Salmivalli et al. 2000). Compared to verbal and physical
Prior studies also indicate that bullies accept inappropriate aggression, relational aggression is less outspoken. It is
behavior more easily than non-bullies, and may interpret more difficult to identify the perpetrator, and avoiding
their behavior as efficient and justified. Therefore, we accusations of deliberately harming others is exactly the
assume that bullies hold more Machiavellian beliefs than intention of the aggressor (Björkqvist et al. 1992; Garan-
non-bullies, and that bullies who are relationally aggressive deau and Cillessen 2006).
in particular distinguish themselves from non-bullies on It has often been suggested that the type and amount of
this measure. aggressive behavior differs between genders. Boys are
perceived by peers as more overtly aggressive, while girls
Do Bullies Differ in Popularity? seem to use more indirect means to harm others (Salmivalli
et al. 2000). In contrast, Putallaz et al. (2007) indicated that
The traditional method of defining peer status distinguishes gender differences in physical and relational aggression are
five sociometric types (popular, rejected, neglected, con- often minimal. That is, girls as well as boys display com-
troversial, and average). This classification system identi- parable amounts of relational aggression. However, boys
fies sociometrically popular children as those who are tend to use equal amounts of physical and relational
well-liked by their peers, a status type characterized by aggression, while girls are more inclined to harm others by
prosocial traits (Coie et al. 1982). Recently, a second type indirect means.
of popularity has been proposed, perceived popularity. Traditionally, social intelligence or social competence is
While sociometric popularity is a measure of peer accep- linked to adjustment and aggression to maladjustment
tance and represents a pleasant and prosocial behavior (Crick and Dodge 1994). However, Björkqvist et al. (2000)

123
1044 J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

indicated that social intelligence is a neutral concept secondary school tracks: pre-vocational (159 boys, 187
because it does not exclude the use of aggression. In a girls), general secondary (83 boys, 125 girls), and college
conflict situation, a socially intelligent adolescent still has preparatory (111 boys, 141 girls). The ethnic composition
the option to solve the problem aggressively rather than of the sample was 92.9% Dutch/Caucasian, .1% Moroccan,
peacefully. Thus, aggression and social intelligence are not .4% Turkish, and 3.5% of other ethnic or national origin
mutually exclusive, and the likelihood that they go together (3.1% did not specify their origin). Grade 8 was chosen
is larger when a relationally aggressive solution is chosen. because this is an age group in which reports of bullying
Björkqvist et al. (1992) argued that indirect aggression are particularly high (see Nansel et al. 2001; Olweus 1994).
requires influence in a social network. Indeed, it has been Passive parental permission was obtained through a letter.
found that although relational aggression is negatively Parents were informed about the purpose of the study and
associated with social preference, it is positively associated the voluntary nature of participation. Parents could react to
with perceived popularity (e.g., Cillessen and Mayeux this letter if they did not agree that their child would par-
2004). ticipate in the study. Students completed a questionnaire in
Thus, it has been argued that relational aggression their classroom under the guidance of a researcher who
requires social intelligence, social power, and the skills to explained the procedures and the confidentiality of the
manipulate a group. A good understanding of relationships answers.
and a central position in the peer group appear necessary to
relationally victimize others (Björkqvist et al. 1992; Sutton
et al. 1999b). Xie et al. (2002) found that the use of rela- Peer Nomination Measures
tional aggression did not lead children to be excluded from
the peer group. Instead, high network centrality enabled A standard sociometric instrument was used that included
them to use relational aggression even more. Therefore, it nine peer nomination questions. The reference group was
was hypothesized that some bullies are particularly rela- the classroom. For each question, students could name an
tionally aggressive, especially when they are also central or unlimited number of peers from their own classroom. Both
powerful in the peer group. same-sex and other-sex nominations were allowed. Self
nominations were not allowed.
The following constructs were assessed: perceived
The Current Study popularity (‘‘classmates who are most popular’’), rejection
(‘‘classmates you like the least’’, a continuous measure of
The overall goal of this study was to identify types of rejection), bullying (‘‘is a bully’’), physical aggression
bullies that differ in social intelligence, Machiavellian (‘‘hit, kick or push others’’), verbal aggression (‘‘call others
beliefs, relational aggression, and popularity. These con- names, laugh at them, or threaten to hurt them’’), relational
structs were selected based on their associations with bul- aggression (two items: ‘‘gossip or speak badly about oth-
lying in recent studies (e.g., Andreou 2004; Sutton and ers,’’ and ‘‘ignore or exclude others’’), and leadership
Keogh 2000) and their importance in recent debates on the (‘‘classmates who take the lead’’). For each question, the
nature of bullying (see Arsenio and Lemerise 2001; Crick number of nominations received was counted for each
and Dodge 1999; Sutton et al. 1999c, 2001). It was student and standardized to z-scores within classrooms to
hypothesized that the social function of bullying in the peer control for differences in classroom size. For bullying,
group depends on the status and position of the bully in the nominations received were also standardized within gender
peer network. Different provisions of bullying behavior (see Solberg and Olweus 2003). For relational aggression,
will demand different skills and influence levels. Thus, our the average of the two standardized scores (r = .70) was
goal was to identify different types of bullies and the taken.
unique behaviors, status positions, and social-cognitive A within-classroom friendship nomination was also
skills characteristic for each. used. The matrix of best friend nominations in the class-
room was analyzed with UCINET 6 (Borgatti et al. 2002)
to compute a network centrality score for each student. The
Method Bonacich (1987) power index was chosen as the measure
of network centrality. According to this index, students
Participants received a network centrality score that is a weighted
function of reciprocal friends they have in the classroom,
Participants were 806 adolescents (353 boys, 453 girls; M and the power of the peers that they have ties with. The
age = 13.37 years, SD = .57) in Grade 8 of two secondary Bonacich power index is generally recognized as an
schools in The Netherlands. Students were in three important measure of social network centrality.

123
J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052 1045

Self-Report Measures Identification of Bullies

Bullying Participants who scored in the top 15% of the distribution


of the standardized bullying score (standardized within
Students completed the bullying subscale of the Dutch classrooms and gender) were identified as bullies. This is a
version of the Olweus (1989) Bully/Victim Questionnaire relatively strict criterion but has been used more often to
(Sentse et al. 2007). The scale includes five items (e.g., identify bullies in previous studies (see e.g., Camodeca
‘‘How often have you bullied others in the last year?’’) that et al. 2003), and is also otherwise a common criterion to
are rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from ‘‘never’’ to identify subgroups in peer relations research using socio-
‘‘several times’’. The internal reliability of this scale was metric measures (see Cillessen 2009). As a result, 120 of
good (a = .70). the 806 participants (14.9%) were identified as bullies. This
method yielded a bully sample of 52 boys with an average
Machiavellianism standardized bully score of 2.04 (SD = .84), and a bully
sample of 68 girls with an average standardized bully score
Machiavellianism was measured with a Dutch translation of 1.79 (SD = 1.49). For comparison purposes, 25 boys
of the 20-item Kiddie Mach. The Kiddie Mach was origi- and 25 girls were randomly selected from the remainder of
nally designed by Christie and Geis (1970) to assess the sample to form the non-bully comparison group.
Machiavellianism in children and adolescents. A Greek
translation of the original measure was recently used suc-
cessfully by Andreou (2004). The items of this scale Results
measure Machiavellian beliefs (e.g., ‘‘Anyone who com-
pletely trusts anyone else is asking for trouble’’) and are Analysis Strategy
rated on a 5-point scale (1 = disagree very much;
5 = agree very much). The measure yields an overall The analyses of this paper proceeded in two steps. First,
Machiavellianism score that is the average of the scores for cluster analysis was conducted on the subsamples of male
the 20 items. and female bullies to identify subtypes. Second, the
The translation of the original scale was completed by resulting clusters were examined on a number of new
two researchers separately. Consensus about the proper variables, to validate their nature.
translation of each item was reached between them. A third Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations for
translator then checked if the translations agreed with the bullying, the clustering variables, and the profiling vari-
original items. The test–retest reliability of this new ables by gender in the analysis sample (N = 170). Gender
translation was tested on an independent sample of 44 7th comparisons indicated that boys scored significantly higher
graders from one of the schools involved in this project but than girls on peer- and self-reported bullying, physical and
who were not part of the actual study. They completed the verbal aggression, rejection, leadership, and Machiavel-
measure twice across 9 weeks in the fall of the school year. lianism. Girls scored higher than boys on social network
The test–retest correlation of the overall Machiavellianism centrality.
score in this sample was .62. Cronbach’s a of the scale was Table 2 shows the intercorrelations between the same
.76 at Time 1 and .70 at Time 2. Thus, the psychometric variables by gender in the analysis sample. The correlations
properties of the scale were acceptable for inclusion in the in general followed a meaningful pattern that is common
actual study. The internal consistency reliability of the 20- for these constructs in other research. Notice in particular
item scale in the total sample of 806 participants in this that, for both genders, all measures of aggression were
study was .61 (Cronbach’s a). positively correlated with each other, as well as with bul-
lying. All correlations were compared by gender using
Social Intelligence Fisher’s r-to-z transformations. As can be seen in Table 2,
a number of correlations were significantly different by
Social intelligence was measured with a translation of the gender. In particular, the association between bullying and
Tromsø Social Intelligence Scale (Silvera et al. 2001). The physical aggression was stronger for boys than for girls.
Dutch translation was used successfully in a recent study
by Meijs et al. (in press). The scale consists of 21 items Cluster Analysis
(e.g., ‘‘I can predict other people’s behavior’’), that are
scored on a 7-point scale (1 = does not describe me at all; Based on the considerations given above, the clustering
7 = describes me very well). The internal reliability of the variables were perceived popularity and relational aggres-
scale was found to be satisfactory (Cronbach’s a = .78). sion (from peer nominations), and social intelligence and

123
1046 J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of study variables in the analysis sample (N = 170)


Boys (N = 77) Girls (N = 93) Gender difference
M SD M SD t p

Social intelligence 4.80 .60 4.90 .53 -1.14 .261


Machiavellianism 2.58 .44 2.43 .33 2.47 .015
Relational aggression -.75 1.32 .91 1.27 -.80 .424
Perceived popularity .71 1.05 .46 .84 1.69 .094
Bullying 1.84 1.48 .71 1.09 5.57 .001
Physical aggression 1.41 1.44 -.13 .84 8.29 .001
Verbal aggression 1.60 1.45 .39 1.21 5.83 .001
Social rejection .73 1.22 .12 .95 3.58 .001
Leadership 1.03 1.47 .46 1.25 2.69 .008
Network centrality 3.63 2.31 4.78 2.80 -2.93 .004
Self-reported bullying 9.41 3.59 8.13 2.39 2.68 .008
Means that are italicized were significantly different by gender

Table 2 Correlations between study variables in the analysis sample (N = 170)


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Social intelligence .05 .35* .30* .24* .07 .28* .12 .38* .11 .17
2. Machiavellianism -.56* .24* .19 .19 .12 .19 .21* .22* .06 .15
3. Relational aggression .23* -.20 .55* .73* .28* .66* .39* .69* .02 .34*
4. Perceived popularity .39* -.19 .39* .50* .02 .31* .01 .64* .33* .33*
5. Bullying .09 .02 .66* .45* .28* .74* .49* .65* -.07 .13
6. Physical aggression .01 -.13 .44* .30* .55* .54* .39* .25* -.28* -.01
7. Verbal aggression .15 -.05 .71* .47* .85* .67* .49* .56* -.13 .11
8. Social rejection -.14 .09 .54* .03 .42* .41* .51* .23* -.19 .02
9. Leadership .26* -.16 .66* .65* .59* .54* .62* .32* .03 .24*
10. Network centrality .26* -.02 .16 .46* .14 .07 .16 -.02 .23* .27*
11. Self-report bullying -.10 .25* .10 .10 .32* .37* .35* .32* .17 .09
Correlations italicized were significantly different by gender. Girls above the diagonal, boys below the diagonal
* p \ .05

Machiavellianism (from self-reports). A hierarchical clus- second cluster, including 28 boys, was differentiated from
ter procedure was used, starting with one cluster containing the remaining boys based on relational aggression. This
all of the observations. Similar to previous studies in this group was significantly less relationally aggressive then the
field (e.g., Cillessen et al. 1992; Estell et al. 2002), Ward’s rest, M’s = .17 versus 2.15, F(1, 50) = 118.77, p \ .001.
clustering method based on squared Euclidian distances The third cluster, including 10 boys, differed significantly
between cases on each clustering variable was used. from the first two (see below), but not on a single variable.
Ward’s method minimizes the variance within clusters and The clustering process for girls also generated a three-
maximizes between-group variance (Estell et al. 2002; Hair cluster solution. The first cluster, including 31 girls, was set
and Black 2000). apart from the remaining girls based on relational aggres-
For boys, a three-cluster solution was identified as the sion. The girls in this cluster were significantly more rela-
most optimal solution to differentiate the boys on the four tionally aggressive than the remaining girls, M’s = 2.21
clustering variables. The first cluster, including 14 boys, was versus .42, F(1, 66) = 180.14, p \ .001. The second clus-
set apart from the remaining bullies based on social intelli- ter, including 20 girls, significantly differed from the other
gence and popularity. This cluster was significantly more clusters (see below), but not on a single variable. The
socially intelligent than the rest, M’s = 5.42 versus 4.68, remaining 17 girls had significantly lower scores on social
F(1, 50) = 15.37, p \ .001, and significantly more popular, intelligence than the other girls, M’s = 4.58 versus 5.07,
M’s = 1.94 versus .72, F(1, 50) = 39.03, p \ .001. The F(1, 66) = 14.90, p \ .001.

123
J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052 1047

The means and standard deviations of the clusters on the moderately popular and above average in social intelli-
four clustering variables are shown in Table 3. To further gence. They scored average on Machiavellianism and high
identify each cluster, two analyses were conducted. First, on relational aggression. Because of their generally mod-
the three clusters were compared to each other on each erate scores on the clustering variables, they were called
clustering variable in a three (Cluster) ANOVA. To popular moderate. Cluster 3 bullies were unpopular and
examine if the clusters differed in the extremity of their below average in Machiavellianism. They scored signifi-
bullying, this ANOVA was also run on the original bul- cantly below other bullies on relational aggression and
lying score. Significant cluster effects were followed up social intelligence. Therefore, they were called low popu-
with a Tukey post-hoc comparison. Second, each cluster lar-low socially intelligent.
was compared with the non-bully comparison group in a
two (Group: Cluster vs. Comparison) ANOVA. The results Cluster Profiling
of these analyses are also shown in Table 3.
The results for boys showed that Cluster 1 bullies were To further validate the clusters, they were compared on
socially intelligent and relationally aggressive. They were other variables that had not been used to identify them.
perceived as popular by their classmates and scored low on Therefore, a three (Cluster) ANOVA was conducted for
Machiavellianism. The differences with other bullies and each gender on the scores for physical aggression, verbal
non-bullies were most evident for social intelligence and aggression, social rejection, leadership, network centrality,
popularity. Therefore, this group was labeled popular- and self-reported bullying. Significant cluster effects were
socially intelligent. Cluster 2 bullies were relatively pop- followed by Tukey post-hoc comparisons. Each cluster was
ular and had average scores for social intelligence, rela- also compared to the non-bully comparison group in a two
tional aggression, and Machiavellianism. They only (Group: Cluster vs. Comparison) ANOVA. Table 4 pre-
differed from non-bullies in terms of popularity. Therefore, sents the results of these analyses.
they were labeled popular moderate. Cluster 3 bullies were For boys, popular-socially intelligent bullies scored
considerably relationally aggressive and had average significantly higher than other bullies on leadership. They
Machiavellian scores. Compared to other bullies and non- also scored significantly higher than low popular-low
bullies they were less popular and socially intelligent. The socially intelligent bullies on network centrality. They
differences with non-bullies and other bullies were most scored significantly higher than non-bullies on physical
obvious for popularity and social intelligence. Therefore, aggression, verbal aggression, leadership, social rejection,
this group was labeled low popular-low socially intelligent. and network centrality.
For girls, Cluster 1 bullies were socially intelligent and Popular-moderate bullies scored significantly lower on
above average Machiavellian beliefs. They were popular the aggression measures than other bullies, however,
and relationally aggressive. Differences with other bullies compared to non-bullies they were more aggressive. They
and non-bullies were evident for popularity, social intelli- scored significantly lower than popular-socially intelligent
gence, and relational aggression. Given the correspon- bullies on network centrality, social rejection, and leader-
dence of this cluster to the first cluster for boys, it was also ship, but their centrality and rejection scores did not differ
called popular-socially intelligent. Cluster 2 bullies were from non-bullies. Low popular-low socially intelligent

Table 3 Comparisons of bullying subtypes on clustering variables


Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Non-bullies

Boys n = 14 n = 28 n = 10 n = 25 F(3, 73)


Social intelligence 5.42a (.52) 4.74b (.66) 4.51b (.52) 4.66 (.36) 8.17*
Machiavellianism 2.30a (.36) 2.73b (.47) 2.56ab (.45) 2.58 (.39) 4.39*
Relational aggression 2.03a (.62) .17b (.70) 2.31a (.54) -.16 (.81) 60.00*
Perceived popularity 1.94a (.24) .85b (.78) .36b (.30) .01 (1.14) 23.38*
Girls n = 31 n = 20 n = 17 n = 25 F(3, 89)
Social intelligence 5.13a (.35) 4.99a (.55) 4.58b (.49) 4.78 (.59) 7.99*
Machiavellianism 2.56a (.35) 2.46ab (.28) 2.27b (.34) 2.36 (.31) 4.17*
Relational aggression 2.21a (.50) .77b (.33) .01c (.56) -.15 .79) 132.66*
Perceived popularity 1.07a (.69) .77a (.52) -.22b (.62) -.07 (.70) 23.89*
Means within rows that do not share a subscript were significantly different between clusters. Cluster means that are italicized were significantly
different from the non-bully comparison group. Standard deviations are between parentheses
* p \ .05

123
1048 J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

Table 4 Comparisons of bullying subtypes on validation variables


Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3 Non-bullies

Boys n = 14 n = 28 n = 10 n = 25 F(3, 73)


Verbal aggression 2.85a (1.20) 1.66b (.92) 2.96a (.62) .28 (1.17) 10.69*
Physical aggression 2.20 (1.30) 1.54 (1.21) 1.95 (1.81) .56 (1.30) 1.18
Social rejection .96ab (1.35) .43a (.98) 2.06b (.99) .37 (1.17) 8.29*
Leadership 2.78a (.93) .72b (1.21) 1.58b (1.38) .13 (1.07) 14.37*
Network centrality 5.32a (1.96) 3.72ab (2.14) 3.17b (2.02) 2.93 (2.34) 3.89*
Self-reported bullying 9.57 (3.32) 10.26 (3.95) 10.50 (2.72) 7.92 (3.32) .24
Girls n = 31 n = 20 n = 17 n = 25 F(3, 89)
Verbal aggression 1.22a (1.42) .01b (.65) .17b (1.16) -.19 (.69) 7.90*
Physical aggression .01 (1.12) -.18 (.69) -.02 (.97) -.35 (.27) .26
Social rejection .49a (.94) -.20b (.50) .19ab (.94) -.15 (1.11) 4.190*
Leadership 1.61a (1.31) .36b (.76) -.32b (.54) -.36 (.58) 21.79*
Network centrality 4.78 (2.94) 5.92 (2.44) 4.08 (2.76) 4.37 (2.78) 2.14
Self-reported bullying 8.87a (2.28) 9.25a (2.73) 7.18b (1.85) 6.69 (1.84) 4.16*
Means within rows that do not share a subscript were significantly different between clusters. Cluster means that are italicized were significantly
different from the non-bully comparison group. Standard deviations are between parentheses
* p \ .05

bullies looked like popular-socially intelligent bullies, with same time, the results also indicated that not all bullies can
the exception of network centrality, social rejection, and be typified as socially intelligent and well-accepted peer
leadership. For network centrality, they did not differ from group members. This is in agreement with Crick and
non-bullies, but for leadership and social rejection, they Dodge’s (1994) assumption that aversive behavior such as
scored significantly higher than non-bullies. They scored bullying is the result of incompetencies or deficiencies,
significantly lower than popular-socially intelligent bullies such as a malfunctioning social information processing
on leadership and network centrality. system. The current study demonstrated significant diver-
For girls, popular-socially intelligent bullies were more sity among bullies in the degree of social intelligence and
verbally aggressive than other bullies. They also scored status. A similar bullying profile was found for both gen-
significantly higher on leadership and social rejection. ders that included in both cases a socially intelligent and
They were more physically aggressive than non-bullies. powerful group and a less socially intelligent and unpop-
Popular-moderate bullies were more central members of ular group.
the peer group than non-bullies. They differed significantly It was hypothesized that the social function of bullying
from non-bullies on all measures except verbal aggression differs between bullies and is associated with status, skills,
and rejection. Nevertheless, their mean scores on the five and position in the peer network. Different functions of
profiling variables did not distinguish them from the other bullying will demand different skills and levels of social
bullies. Low popular-low socially intelligent bullies were influence. Indeed, the results suggest that the identified
physically and verbally more aggressive than non-bullies. profiles of bullies reflect variation in the social function of
They did not differ from other bullies or non-bullies in bullying. Our analyses showed that high social status is
social rejection. They scored lower on self-reported bul- accompanied by social intelligence and the use of relational
lying than the other two groups. One would expect them to aggression. The bullies with this set of characteristics, the
score higher on self-reported bullying than non-bullies, but popular-socially intelligent bullies, may use their skills to
this difference was not significant. gain dominance. Their centrality in the group would enable
them to persuade others to ignore the victim and believe
their backbiting. In this case, bullying is used to acquire
Discussion power and influence.
Our findings are consistent with previous findings of
This study confirmed the need to differentiate between strong associations between relational aggression, per-
bullies instead of assuming that bullies are a homogeneous ceived popularity, and social intelligence. This suggests
group. The results supported Sutton et al.’s (1999a) that these three concepts are unified, and that popularity
hypothesis that at least some bullies are socially intelligent and social intelligence may be preconditions for the use of
and have the ability to manipulate the peer group. At the relational aggression (Björkqvist et al. 1992; Salmivalli

123
J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052 1049

et al. 2000). Putallaz et al. (2007) suggested, that the nominations were used as a measure of social rejection
indirect character of relational aggression demands social instead of the categorical status types. However, the socio-
intelligence to cover the aggressive acts. Surprisingly, we metric status type ‘‘rejected’’ is a two-dimensional construct
found that male bullies who were unpopular and lacking based on both liked and disliked nominations. It is therefore
social intelligence were also highly relationally aggressive. different from the continuous measure used here. It is pos-
Thus, at least for boys, relational aggression may not sible that the bullies in our sample received not only disliked
always require social intelligence. nominations but also liked nominations and, as a result,
Findings by Xie et al. (2002) may explain this excep- would be classified as sociometrically ‘‘controversial.’’
tional finding. They distinguished social aggression from The gender differences in the social rejection of bully
direct relational aggression. Social aggression referred to types may be explained by gender differences in relation-
covert aggressive attacks, such as gossiping, while direct ships with peers more generally. They may explain why,
relational aggression referred to more visible attacks, such contrary to their male counterparts, popular-socially skilled
as ignoring or excluding others. The respondents in their female bullies scored significantly higher on social rejec-
study saw social aggression as provoking behavior causing tion than moderate-popular bullies and non-bullies. Eder
the most damage, while direct relational aggression was (1985) introduced the ‘‘cycle of popularity’’ to describe
perceived as reactive. It is perhaps the concealed nature of how disliking and popularity in girls go hand in hand.
social aggression that demands social intelligence, which Indeed, recent findings support the assumption that high
may explain why unpopular-low socially intelligent bullies status does not exclude the possibility of social rejection
were seen as relationally aggressive but lacking social (Caravita et al. 2008; Estell et al. 2007). The cycle of
intelligence. popularity (Eder 1985) addressed the role of friendships
The results reported by Xie et al. (2002) may also with popular girls in adolescence. Being friends with
contribute to typifying this last group as socially deficient, popular girls is important for one’s own status, however,
in agreement with Crick and Dodge (1999). They popular girls tend to avoid affiliations with lower-status
hypothesized that bullies attribute hostile intent to girls. Because they reject the offers of friendship from
ambiguous cues and therefore respond with excessive lower-status peers, popular girls are also the most disliked
aggression. Unpopular-less socially intelligent bullies may girls. Consistent with Eder’s cycle of popularity (1985), the
use more direct relational aggression than popular-socially most popular girls in our study were also the most disliked
intelligent bullies, who may use more social aggression. ones.
The reactive nature of direct relational aggression as Cillessen and Borch (2006) examined the development
proposed by Xie et al. (2002) is consistent with the per- trajectories of sociometric and perceived popularity and the
spective of the bully proposed by Crick and Dodge (1999) associations with aggression. The effect of relational
and the unpopular-socially less intelligent group of bullies aggression on perceived popularity changed over time.
in this study. Cillessen and Mayeux (2004) also discussed the changing
The social functions of bullying by unpopular-low association between aggression and perceived popularity
socially intelligent bullies may be more intuitive or auto- across development. These studies indicate that these group
matic. Perceiving ambiguous cues as hostile may reflec- constructs are sensitive to developmental changes. It is
tively trigger defending behavior. These bullies may possible that the different social functions of bullying are a
respond with aggression because they feel threatened. reflection of developmental changes, and that the different
Their aggression may be more reactive, and thus have a subtypes of bullies are in fact adolescents at different
different function than the bullying of popular-socially stages of social cognitive and behavioral development.
intelligent bullies that may be more instrumental to gain Furthermore, the different types of bullies may also
dominance. represent different responses to normal social contextual
While for boys the less popular and less socially intel- changes taking place in adolescence. Pellegrini and Long
ligent bullies were the most rejected ones, for girls social (2002) indicated that the transition from primary to sec-
rejection was associated with high social intelligence and ondary school, and the disruptions of peer relationships that
status. Contrary to boys, the popular-socially skilled female come with it, may increase bullying behavior depending on
bullies were the most disliked. They scored significantly adolescents’ needs to establish centrality in a peer group. If
higher on rejection than moderate-popular bullies and non- being central in a peer network is important, bullying may
bullies. be an efficient (but undesirable) way to accomplish it.
Before explaining the gender differences, we emphasize Individual differences between adolescents in this need
that the received dislike nominations are a measure for may also influence their bully subtype classification.
social rejection (Coie et al. 1982). Because the cluster In summary, our results highlight the significance of
analyses require continuous scores, the standardized dislike differentiating between bullies, and identifying unique

123
1050 J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

profiles of bully types. Gender appears to play a role in always create a cluster solution. Some groups will always
determining these profiles. Most importantly, bully types be found. Nevertheless, valid groups are not always found
can be described by the different social functions or pro- and the groups identified in our study clearly had validity.
visions that bullying may have for each of them. Thus, our Thus, despite the inherent exploratory nature of cluster
classification of bullies primarily reflects a functional analysis, we believe in its value in the current study, as also
approach to bullying. Underlying functions, combined demonstrated in previous peer relations projects (e.g.,
with available social-cognitive and behavioral skills, and Cillessen et al. 1992; Estell et al. 2002; French 1990;
afforded by a certain position in the peer group, may Salmivalli and Kaukiainen 2004).
interact to determine an adolescent male of female’s bul- A final limitation is that we only measured eighth
lying classification. The functional approach also has to be graders at one point in time. Inclusion of other age groups
a developmental approach, as functions, skills, and peer and longitudinal designs could identify developmental
group structures change across developmental time. changes in bullying classifications and perhaps reveal
Changes and transitions during adolescence are associated variation in bully subtypes across development. This would
with bullying behavior, and these developmental changes be consistent with the developmental functional approach
may also influence the social functions and provisions of advocated above.
bullying behavior. Still to be explored are the motives for bullying. The
current study identified types of bullies and their behav-
Limitations and Directions for Future Research ioral, social-cognitive, and status profiles. Motivation was
not considered. Yet, there may be important differences
Although we found significant diversity in behavior, status, between bully subtypes in underlying motives. In a recent
and skills among bullies, differences with non-bullies were study, LaFontana and Cillessen (in press) documented
not always obvious. The clustering variables in our study developmental changes in children and adolescents’ needs
clearly showed their usefulness in differentiating among to high status and popularity. Coincidentally, the peak of
bullies; however, some of them did not distinguish all types this need appears to be somewhere around the age of the
of bullies from non-bullies. Therefore, it may be useful to participants of the current study. Yet, there also appear to
include existing scales, such as the Participants Roles be large individual differences in this need. Thus, exam-
measure (Salmivalli et al. 1996), in a future study. Sepa- ining the degree to which adolescents prioritize peer status
rating non-bullies who help the victim from those who and how this is related to bullying aggression seems an
assist or reinforce the bully may lead to a clearer distinc- important avenue for follow-up research.
tion between non-bullies who have a pro-bullying attitude The findings of this study have implications for the
and those who have an anti-bullying attitude. This may design of anti-bullying programs. A single ‘‘one size fits
change the differentiation of bullies from non-bullies. all’’ intervention strategy for reducing bullying behavior
Currently, the non-bullies were a random sample of all may not be sufficient. It may be of crucial importance to
non-bullies existing in the larger normative sample, adjust bullying intervention programs to the bullying sub-
ignoring any differentiation that may exist among them. types one is working with. For some bullying situations it
Nevertheless, our main goal was to identify bully types and may be necessary to target the whole peer network, for
the clustering variables used in our study showed their instance by means of changing the attitudes of bystanders
relevance for this goal. (Salmivalli et al. 2005) or restructuring classroom networks
Our current study also did not examine the possible as suggested by Salmivalli (1999). Because socially intel-
influences of classroom or school norms. Multilevel anal- ligent bullies have the skills to interact and use the social
ysis would be required to examine the influence of class- structure to harass victims, group interventions may be the
room or school variables on the associations among the best choice to reduce their bullying. Individual interven-
clustering and profiling variables. However, the effects of tions, such as self-reflection or feedback about social
classrooms or schools may have been small as the two behavior (Salmivalli 1999), may be more appropriate for
schools in this study were quite similar and the overall bullies who are less well-connected and whose bullying is
sample rather homogenous in many ways. Nevertheless, more driven by individual forces rather than carried by the
the issue of school or classroom contextual effects is an structure of the peer network.
important one that has been generally undervalued in peer This study uniquely contributes to our understanding of
relations research and deserves more attention in future bullying behavior. The bully profiles that were identified
research. and the differences among them suggest that bullying in
Another limitation is the non-inferential and descriptive adolescence may serve a variety of social functions that are
nature of cluster analysis (Hair and Black 2000). Cluster not a homogenous set. A framework was also provided to
analysis requires few statistical assumptions and will relate the various social functions of bullying to

123
J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052 1051

developmental changes in adolescence. Recognizing the Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and
cross-sectional functional heterogeneity of bullying, in types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental
Psychology, 18, 557–570.
conjunction with its longitudinal developmental variability Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., Terry, R., & Wright, V. (1991). The role of
may be the next big challenge in bullying research. aggression in peer relations: An analysis of aggression episodes
in boys’ play groups. Child Development, 62, 812–826.
Acknowledgments This research was supported by a Master’s Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of
Research Grant from the Behavioural Science Institute to the first social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social
author. The authors are grateful to the students who participated in adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74–101.
this study. Special thanks are also due to the teachers and adminis- Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1999). ‘‘Superiority’’ is in the eye of
trators of the Valuas College, Venlo and the BBC College, Panningen, the beholder: A comment on Sutton, Smith, and Swettenham.
The Netherlands who made this research possible. Social Development, 8, 128–131.
de Bruyn, E. H., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2006). Heterogeneity of girls’
consensual popularity: Academic and interpersonal behavioral
profiles. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 435–445.
References Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Brakke, N. P. (1982). Behavior patterns
of socially rejected and neglected preadolescents: The roles of
Andreou, E. (2004). Bully/victim problems and their association with social approach and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child
Machiavellianism and self-efficacy in Greek primary school Psychology, 10, 389–410.
children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 297– Eder, D. (1985). The cycle of popularity: Interpersonal relations
309. among female adolescents. Sociology of Education, 58, 154–165.
Archer, J. (2001). A strategic approach to aggression. Social Estell, D. B., Cairns, R. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2002).
Development, 10, 267–271. Aggression in inner-city early elementary classrooms: Individual
Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2001). Varieties of childhood and peer-group configurations. Merrill -Palmer Quarterly, 48,
bullying: Values, emotion process, and social competence. 52–76.
Social Development, 10, 59–73. Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2007). Bullies and victims
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1989). Young children’s attribution of in rural African American youth: Behavioral characteristics and
action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60, 946–964. social network placement. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 145–159.
Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do Farmer, T. W., Estell, D. B., Bishop, L., O’Neal, K. K., & Cairns, B.
girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental trends regarding D. (2003). Rejected bullies or popular leaders? The social
direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117– relations of aggressive subtypes of rural African American early
127. adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 99, 992–1004.
Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukianen, A. (2000). Social French, D. C. (1990). Heterogeneity of peer-rejected girls. Child
intelligence - empathy = aggression? Aggression and Violent Development, 61, 2028–2031.
Behavior, 5, 191–200. Garandeau, C. F., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2006). From indirect
Bonacich, P. (1987). Power and centrality: A family of measures. aggression to invisible aggression: A conceptual view on
American Journal of Sociology, 92, 1170–1182. bullying and peer group manipulation. Aggression and Violent
Borgatti, S. P., Everett, M. G., & Freeman, L. C. (2002). UCINET for Behavior, 11, 612–625.
windows: Software for social network analysis. Harvard, MA: Gini, G. (2006). Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying:
Analytic Technologies. What’s wrong? Aggressive Behavior, 32, 528–539.
Camodeca, M., Goossens, F. A., Schuengel, F. E., & Terwogt, M. M. Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoè, G. (2007). Does empathy
(2003). Links between social information processing in middle predict adolescents’ bullying and defending behavior? Aggres-
childhood and involvement in bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 29, sive Behavior, 33, 467–476.
116–127. Hair, J. F., & Black, W. C. (2000). Cluster analysis. In L. G. Grimm &
Caravita, S. C. S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2008). Unique and P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understanding more multi-
interactive effects of empathy and social status on involvement variate statistics (pp. 147–205). Washington, DC: American
in bullying. Social Development, 18, 140–163. Psychological Association.
Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in machiavellianism. New LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (in press). Developmental
York: Academic Press. changes in the priority of perceived status in childhood and
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Borch, C. (2006). Developmental trajectories adolescence. Social Development.
of adolescent popularity: A growth curve modelling analysis. Meijs, N., Cillessen, A. H. N., Scholte, R. H. J., Segers, E., &
Journal of Adolescence, 29, 935–959. Spijkerman, R. (in press). Social intelligence and academic
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to achievement as predictors of adolescent popularity. Journal of
reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association Youth and Adolescence.
between aggression and social status. Child Development, 75, Nansel, T. R., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R. S., Ruan, W. J. K., Simons-
147–163. Morten, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Rose, A. J. (2005). Understanding popularity in youth: Prevalence and association with psychosocial adjustment.
the peer system. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 2094–2100.
14, 102–105. Olweus, D. (1989). The Olweus bully/victim questionnaire. Mimeo,
Cillessen, A. H. N. (2009). Sociometric methods. In K. H. Rubin, Bergen, Norwegen.
W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer Olweus, D. (1994). Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a
interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 82–99). New York: school based intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology
Guilford. and Psychiatry, 35, 1171–1190.
Cillessen, A. H. N., van IJzendoorn, H. W., van Lieshout, C. F. M., & Parkhurst, J. T., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and
Hartup, W. W. (1992). Heterogeneity among peer-rejected boys: peer perceived popularity: Two distinct dimensions of peer
Subtypes and stabilities. Child Development, 63, 893–905. status. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 125–144.

123
1052 J Youth Adolescence (2010) 39:1041–1052

Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School bullies, Sutton, J., & Keogh, E. (2000). Social competition in school:
victims, and aggressive victims: Factors related to group Relationships with bullying, machiavellianism and personality.
affiliation and victimization in early adolescence. Journal of British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 443–456.
Educational Psychology, 91, 216–234. Sutton, J., & Keogh, E. (2001). Components of machiavellian beliefs
Pellegrini, A. D., & Long, J. D. (2002). A longitudinal study of in children: Relationships with personality. Personality and
bullying, dominance, and victimization during the transition Individual Differences, 30, 137–148.
from primary school through secondary school. British Journal Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Social cognition
of Developmental Psychology, 20, 259–280. and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British
Putallaz, M., Grimes, L. C., Foster, J. K., Kupersmidt, J. B., Coie, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 435–450.
J. D., & Dearing, K. (2007). Overt and relational aggression and Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Bullying and
victimization: Multiple perspectives within the school setting. ‘‘theory of mind’’. A critique of the ‘‘social skills deficit’’ view of
Journal of School Psychology, 45, 523–547. antisocial behavior. Social Development, 8, 117–127.
Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & van Acker, R. (2000). Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999c). Socially
Heterogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial config- undesirable need not be incompetent: A response to Crick and
urations. Developmental Psychology, 36, 14–24. Dodge. Social Development, 8, 132–134.
Salmivalli, C. (1999). The participant role approach to school Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (2001). It’s easy, it works,
bullying: Implications for interventions. Journal of Adolescence, and it makes me feel good. A response to Arsenio and Lemerise.
22, 453–459. Social Development, 10, 74–78.
Salmivalli, C., & Kaukiainen, A. (2004). Female aggression revisited: Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is
Variable- and person-centered approaches to studying gender power: Implications for school-based intervention strategies.
differences in different types of aggression. Aggressive Behav- Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19, 157–176.
ior, 30, 158–163. Xie, H., Swift, D. J., Cairns, B. D., & Cairns, R. B. (2002).
Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. (2000). Aggression Aggressive behaviors in social interaction and developmental
and sociometric status among peers: Do gender and type of adaptation: A narrative analysis of interpersonal conflicts during
aggression matter? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41, early adolescence. Social Development, 11, 205–224.
17–24.
Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., & Voeten, M. (2005). Anti-bullying
intervention: Implementation and outcome. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 75, 465–487. Author Biographies
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., &
Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant Margot Peeters received her research master degree in Behavioral
roles and their relations to social status within the group. Sciences at the Radboud University of Nijmegen. Her major research
Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1–15. interests include consequences and determinants of bullying behavior.
Salmivalli, C., & Nieminen, E. (2002). Proactive and reactive
aggression among school bullies, victims and bully-victims. Antonius H. N. Cillessen is professor of psychology in the
Aggressive Behavior, 28, 30–44. Behavioral Science Institute at the Radboud University of Nijmegen.
Sentse, M., Scholte, R., Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2007). Person- His major research interests include peer rejection and popularity,
group dissimilarity in involvement in bullying and its relation aggression, and research methods for developmental psychology. He
with social status. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, received his Ph.D. in developmental psychology at the Radboud
1009–1019. University of Nijmegen.
Silvera, D. H., Martinussen, M., & Dahl, T. I. (2001). The Tromsø
social intelligence scale: A self-report measure of social Ron H. J. Scholte is associate professor of education in the
intelligence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 313–319. Behavioural Science Institute at the Radboud University of Nijmegen.
Solberg, M. E., & Olweus, D. (2003). Prevalence estimation of school His major research interests include family and peer relationships and
bullying with the Olweus bully/victim questionnaire. Aggressive behavior problems in childhood and adolescence. He received his
Behavior, 29, 239–268. Ph.D. in social sciences at the Radboud University of Nijmegen.

123

También podría gustarte