Está en la página 1de 27

679-133.MTS.

Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 265

A Theory of Motives for Prokofiev’s Music


deborah rifkin

This article develops a theory of motives that provides insight into Prokofiev’s chromatic language.
Although Schenkerian theory can account for the idiosyncratic chromatic slides in Prokofiev’s
music, it does not adequately explain why the chromaticism is there. The theory builds upon
Schenkerian and Neo-Riemannian principles, and explores the theoretical and analytic implica-
tions of three different types of motives, called systemic, functional pitch-class, and non-functional
pitch-class.

ecause of its eclectic mix of traditional and shift within an otherwise predictable phrase structure.3 The

B 20th-century sounds, Prokofiev’s music has been in-


terpreted from many different points of view, includ-
ing both tonal and non-tonal analytic methodologies.
background structure, shown in Example 2(c), can be under-
stood as an antecedent phrase in an interrupted structure;
the opening tonic moves to a half cadence at m. 17. In
Schenkerian approaches tend to emphasize the traditional measure 8, an F  -minor harmony, shown with an asterisk,
aspects of Prokofiev’s music, such as classic phrase structures seems sudden and out of context in relation to the surround-
and conventional cadential goals.1 Non-tonal approaches, ing F-major tonic and dominants. Many scholars have called
such as that of Neil Minturn, tend to focus on Prokofiev’s chromatic shifts such as this wrong notes.4 It is an unfortu-
idiosyncratic chromatic language.2 Minturn’s analyses, for nate term because there is, of course, nothing wrong about
example, invoke set-theory to explain Prokofiev’s chromatic these particular notes. I believe the term has prevailed for
slides and to associate unordered pitch-class collections. I more than fifty years because it captures an incongruous
also focus on Prokofiev’s unusual chromatic moments, yet in effect that many of Prokofiev’s chromatic shifts create—as if
contrast to Minturn’s unordered set motives, I advance a theory they don’t belong in their tonal contexts. In Example 2, the
of motives that considers pitches as ordered linear progressions harmonic motion from F major to F  minor—as triads—
—progressions that can have varying degrees of connection seems to snub conventional tonal syntax. Although such
to tonal structure. chromatic excursions may be uncharacteristic of an eighteenth-
Example 1, from the third movement of Prokofiev’s century tonal style, they can often be modeled by conven-
Violin Sonata op. 94, demonstrates a typical chromatic tional Schenkerian methodology. The F  -minor harmony of

I would like to thank Brian Alegant and Allen Cadwallader for their 3 This piece is also known as the Flute Sonata, op. 94. Prokofiev wrote
helpful comments and suggestions. versions of the work for both violin and flute.
1 Schenkerian interpretations of Prokofiev’s music include Ashley 1963, 4 A chronological list of scholars who use the term wrong note includes
Bass 1988, Kaufman 1987, and Salzer 1962, 204. Austin 1956; Ashley 1963; Kaufman 1987; Bass 1988; Kramer 1988;
2 Minturn 1997. Minturn 1997; and Rifkin 2000 and 2001.

265
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 266

266 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

Andante (ä = 60)
¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
² Ł ²Ł ²ð
Violin − .
Š 0 Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
]\
ÿ Ł Ł
Š − .0 Ł Łý ² Ł ² Ł ² ŁŁ Ł ² Ł ² Ł ² ŁŁ Ł
Piano! Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł Łý
\ legato

]\ Ł
Ý−. ÿ ÿ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ²ð
0 Łý ð ð ²Ł Ł
10
Ł Ł ¦Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł ¦ Ł
Š− ¾ ²Ł Ł Ł ² Ł Łý −Ł ð Ł Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł ð ð
¦Ł Ł Ł
Š− ¹ Ł ¦Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł ² Ł Ł ²Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł ¦ Ł Ł
! \
Ł Ł Ł \
Ł
Ý−
ð ð ð ð Ł ² Ł Ł Ł ²ð Ł Ł Ł ¦Ł ð
]\
example 1. Score of Prokofiev, Violin Sonata op. 94, third movement, mm. 1–17.

m. 8 can be interpreted as a confluence of chromatic voice- of a surface event at a middleground level. This concept of
leading events: C  is a lower neighbor to D; F  arises from motive derives from a Schenkerian procedure called motivic
an unfolding of the modally-mixed submediant harmony. In enlargement; 5 my approach builds on previous work by
general, Prokofiev’s quirky shifts involve either ascending Beach, Burkhart, Cadwallader, Laitz, Pastille, and Schachter
chromatic passing tones, or chromatic lower neighbors— in order to shed light on Prokofiev’s idiosyncratic chromatic
voice leading transformations available at any level above the language.6
first middleground. Although Schenker’s system can account To this end, I explore the theoretical and analytic implica-
for the chromaticism, it doesn’t adequately explain it. tions of three different types of motives, called systemic, func-
Motivic analysis can provide further insight into Prokofiev’s tional pitch-class, and non-functional pitch-class. Part one of this
chromatic language. (Later, I provide a motivic analysis for
5 Schenker uses the term Vergrössserung, which Charles Burkhart trans-
the passage in Example 1.) lates as “enlargement.” See, Burkhart 1978, 146–49. For another discus-
For present purposes, a motive is an ordered progression sion of motivic enlargement see Alegant and McLean 2001.
of pitches that is repeated within and across musical levels. I 6 See Beach 1984; Beach 1988; Burkhart 1978; Cadwallader 1983 and
am most interested in associations created by a repetition 1984; Cadwallader and Pastille 1992; Laitz 1996; and Schachter 1983.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 267

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 267


Ł Ł
Ł5
Ł Ł Ł Ł ² Ł ² Ł ¦
10
Ł
Ł Ł Ł 2̂
ŁŁ ŁŁŁ Ł
17

Š − ŁŁŁ ðŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ² Ł Ł ² Ł Ł − Ł ð Ł Ł ² Ł ¦Ł Ł Ł
(a) ! Ł
Ł Ł ²Ł

Ł
Ý− Ł ŁŁŁ ŁŁ
ð Ł Ł Ł ²Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦Ł ð
Ł Ł ( ) Ł ² Ł Ł Ł ²Ł

(a) I V


Ł

Š− ð ¦ ŁŁŁ Ł ð
(b) ! Ł
5
Ł ²Ł
 6
Ý− ð
ð Ł Ł ²Ł ¦( ŁŁ ) Ł Ł

(b) I V

3̂ 2̂

Š− ð ð
(c) !
Ý− ð
ð
(c) I V

example 2. Tonal structure of Violin Sonata op. 94, third movement, mm. 1–17.

essay illustrates each type of motive with an analytic exam- scribe a process that involves systemic motives. Cadwallader
ple. Part two discusses more complicated analytic situations, and Pastille offer a useful Schenkerian description of what
and shows the breadth and limits of the analytic approach. I refer to as a systemic motive. They argue that motives are
diminutions of middleground linear patterns, such as arpeg-
i. introduction to the three types of motives giations, stepwise progressions, or neighbor motions.7 This
perspective links the pitches of a motive to tonal functions,
Systemic Motives. When Schenkerian theorists discuss
motivic parallelisms or concealed motives, they often de- 7 Cadwallader and Pastille 1992.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 268

268 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

and asserts that a motive is defined by the repetition of a C: 3̂ 4̂ 3̂ 2̂ 1̂ G: 3̂ 4̂ 3̂ 2̂ 1̂

Ł Ł Ł Ł
function, and not by the repetition of pitch classes. In other repetition of the systemic motive

systemic motive
words, a recurrence of a systemic motive may involve different Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ² Ł Ł Ł
pitch classes, so long as the respective motivic elements are ŠŁ Ł Ł Ł
contextually similar. To illustrate, the beamed notes in
Examples 3(a) and 3(b) show a hypothetical systemic motive
and a repetition of it. Although the pitch classes in (a) and
(a) (b)
(b) differ, the corresponding elements of each motive share
identical harmonic and contrapuntal properties. The first example 3. A hypothetical systemic motive and its repetition.
note of each motive, the primary tone of a descending third
progression, is decorated by an unsupported upper neighbor;
the third note returns to the initial tone; the fourth pitch is a
passing tone that receives consonant support; and the final tion of a systemic motive is so restrictive. By confining systemic
note concludes the linear progression and the prolongation motives to the deepest middleground paradigms, I am able to
of the initial harmony. These functions are not only aspects assert that systemic motives replicate the roots of the tonal
of voice leading; they are also grounded in the underlying system itself. In contrast, many of the kinds of motives that
harmonic context of each pitch. As Burkhart states, “Schenk- Schenkerians discuss, while prevalent in tonal music, are not
erian motivic parallelism is indissolubly wedded to the har- enlargements of deep middleground processes. Instead, they
mony of the passage in which it occurs.”8 As a shorthand, I replicate events on more surface levels, where more diverse
shall refer to a systemic motive by its scale degrees; it should tonal transformations are available. Even such paradigmatic
be noted, however, that such a motive is defined by harmonic processes as voice exchanges and lower neighbor-note mo-
context, and not just by linear motion. Harmonic support tions do not belong to the first middleground level. Just as
and context are essential elements of a systemic motive. Schenkerians differentiate between tonal transformations of
This definition of a systemic motive is a particular type of the middleground and foreground, I differentiate between
motivic parallelism. I require the foreground presentation of motives of the middleground and foreground.10
a systemic motive to be a literal figuration or diminution of a
deep middleground event. A systemic motive embodies nor- systemic motives in the “classical symphony”
mative tonal paradigms derived from the Ursatz or deep
middleground levels.9 Because they are figurations of deep- The “Classical Symphony,” a piece composed in a self-
level progressions, most systemic motives involve diatonic pro- confessed neo-classical style, provides a good example of
gressions, although chromatic systemic motives can arise from systemic motives. Many aspects of the first movement’s expo-
modal mixture. sition conform to classical expectations of sonata form. As
Readers familiar with the broader definition of motive shown in Example 4, the tonic of the first theme (m. 1)
outlined in Free Composition might wonder why my defini- moves to the dominant of the second theme (m. 46) through
an intermediary submediant harmony (m. 28). Although
8 Burkhart 1978, 167.
9 For a sampling of what I mean by tonal paradigms see Schenker 1979, 10 For a discussion of the types of transformations available at the middle-
figures 15, 16, and 32. ground level, see Schenker 1979, pp. 29–52, 68–111.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 269

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 269

measures: 4 7 11 15 16 18 19 20 26 28
5̂ 4̂ 3̂
D: 5̂ 4̂ 3̂ 2̂ 1̂ C: 5̂ 4̂ 3̂ 2̂ 1̂ D: 5̂ 4̂ 3̂ 2̂ 1̂ b: 5̂ 4̂ 3̂ 2̂ 1̂

² ð Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ ð ¦ Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł ŁŁ Ł Ł ð Ł Ł
Ł ² Ł ŁŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł
Š ²Ł Ł Ł Ł
! 5 6
Ý ²² ð
First Theme
Ł Ł
¦Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł ¦ Ł ²Ł Ł Ł ð

I vi

36 40 42 46 52 58 65 66 74 194

á

Ł

A: 5̂ 4̂ 3̂
²Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ð5̂ Ł4̂ ¦ Ł3̂ Ł2̂ Ł á Ł Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł ² Ł 2̂Ł á Ł Ł Ł
2̂ 1̂
²² Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł
A:
A: 5̂ 4̂ 3̂
Ł Ł ð
Š ²Ł ²² ŁŁ
! Ł Ł 

á
Tonal closure in

Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Second Theme 5ths
recapitulation
Ý ²² Ł Ł ð ²Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł ( )
Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł ¦ Ł ²Ł ²Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ð
á  á
A: I V I LN vi V I V I
V I

example 4. Systemic motives in the “Classical Symphony,” i, exposition.

there are some unconventional moments (such as the aug- In addition to its traditional middleground structure, this
mented second in the bass from G  to F  in the second exposition features a deep-level descent from 5̂ to 1̂. The
theme), most of the exposition can be modeled by Schenker’s fundamental melodic descent, beamed in the example, is the
principles of scale-step progression.11

11 Although the number of parallel fifths that support the tones of the form the octaves or fifths relate as unequivocally to one another as in
Urlinie is certainly unusual, I would argue that these parallels do not strict counterpoint. But where such danger does not exist, voices at the
contradict Schenkerian principles of tonal structure. In each case, the foreground level, even the outer ones, can form octave or fifth succes-
middleground parallels are mitigated by foreground voice leading. As sions with impunity . . . Conversely, the middleground frequently dis-
Schenker states, “Often it is the task of the foreground to circumvent a plays forbidden successions; it is then the task of the foreground to
succession of fifths which threaten in the middleground”(Schenker eliminate them” (Schenker, 1979,56, Figures 51 and 52). Other exam-
1979, 60, see Figure 54, no.15). Schenker is unequivocal on this subject: ples in Free Composition that include parallels on the middleground
“[parallels are forbidden on the foreground] when the tones which level are: Figures 20, no. 3; 75, no. 5; 95d; and 136, no. 2.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 270

270 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

deepest presentation of this motive.12 The motive recurs at that particular chord or note is there.14 I interpret the role of
the middleground level not only in the tonic key of D major, chromaticism in a pitch-class motive in a slightly different
but also in A major, C major, and B minor.13 In each of these manner in order to provide a fuller explanation for the effect
local tonicizations, the motive retains its harmonic meaning of a particular chromatic note.
as a prolongation of tonic. Every presentation of the motive My restrictive definition of systemic motives covers a mere
features a descending fifth progression that can be summa- fraction of the kinds of motives available in tonal music.
rized in terms of scale degrees: 5̂–4̂–3̂–2̂–1̂. Many tonal analyses invoke what I call pitch-class motives. It
Because systemic and pitch-class motives relate differently is important to keep this distinction in mind: a systemic mo-
to chromaticism, it will be useful to clarify my interpretation tive is a replication of the roots of the tonal system; a pitch-
of the chromatic elements of this passage in order to contrast class motive is an associative recollection of more surface
it with subsequent discussions of Prokofiev’s chromatic lan- events.
guage. From a motivic standpoint, the most significant chro-
matic alternations in this passage are the C  s in mm. 11–16 pitch-class motives
and 46–61. These C  s participate in voice-leading progres-
sions. In mm. 11–16, the C  s are part of the prolongation of Pitch-class motives are ordered progressions of pitch classes
the initial tonic harmony, accomplished by a stepwise de- that occur on surface and middleground levels of music. To
scent to the dividing dominant in m. 18, D–C  –B–A. To put be more specific, pitch-class motives are not diminutions of
it simply, the C-major harmony of mm. 11–16 derives from a the deepest levels of tonal motions. As opposed to a systemic
passing tone. Although a C  would have been diatonic to the motive, which occupies a diatonic space, a pitch-class motive
key, C  can be understood as mixture of the seventh scale can associate chromatic events. Not all recurring pitch-class
step. As Patrick McCreless points out, this type of linear ex- patterns, however, are motivically significant. In order to in-
planation, which is typical of most Schenkerian analyses, voke motivic meaning, a progression of pitch classes must be
shows how a particular chromatic event fits into the linear- marked as a salient and memorable melodic event, which,
harmonic unfolding of a piece, but it does not explain why when repeated, is recollected as a version of the original
statement. Because of their anomalous character, Prokofiev’s
quirky chromatic motions are well suited for motivic associa-
12 The final 1̂ of the Ursatz motive occurs at the conclusion of the piece
tions. The very quality that makes them conspicuous in dia-
in mm. 194, not within the exposition. This is a special kind of systemic tonic contexts also makes them recognizable motivically.
motive because it consists of repetitions of the Ursatz. Burkhart pro- Pitch-class motives associate such chromatic melodic gestures
vides a specific name for this phenomenon: Ursatz parallelism (Burkhart across structural levels.
1978, 153). Schenker also discusses this concept under the heading of Because pitch-class motives are restricted to the middle-
Übertragung der Ursatzformen (Transference of the Ursatz form); see ground levels above the first level, they are not as grounded
Schenker 1979, 87.
in the tonal system. However, motivic associations created by
13 There are other surface presentations of fifth descents (namely, F  –E–
D–C  –B in mm. 5–7, and E–D–C–B–A in mm. 13–15), but they do
chromatic voice leading within middleground levels abound
not preserve the harmonic context of the progression as a fifth unfold-
ing within a tonic harmony. Consequently, I do not consider them in-
stances of a systemic motive. 14 McCreless 1990, 138–45.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 271

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 271

in tonal music. I assign a different label to motives that in- conventional capacity as a leading tone. If this leading tone
volve these less-rooted transformations. Because the deriva- were conventionally supported with a dominant, the opening
tional aspects of these motives are limited, I prefer to focus could be viewed as an antecedent phrase of a period. One
on the actual pitch classes, instead of their derived tonal might then expect a consequent phrase to begin in m. 9.
function.15 Thus, I can distinguish between motives that are Instead, the orchestra enters in m. 9 with a B-minor har-
rooted in the tonal system and those that are merely planted mony that supports the lingering F  from the end of the
within it. violin solo. This B-minor harmony initiates eight measures
of harmonic ambiguity. Harmonic functionality resumes in
non-functional pitch-class motives m. 17, where a dominant-seventh chord prepares the tonic of
the next thematic statement beginning in m. 18.
Some pitch-class motives are able to associate elements The B-minor harmony of this passage is intriguing.
independent of tonal structure. I call such motives non- Despite the thematic references of mm. 9–12, this B-minor
functional pitch-class motives. Example 5, from the first move- interlude sounds unusual, especially when followed by the
ment of the Violin Concerto No. 2, illustrates such a motive. return of the tonic in m. 18. There could be many reasons
At the beginning of the piece, the violin solo establishes the why B-minor sounds odd in this context. Most notably, B  is
tonic harmony by simple arpeggiations, (G–B  –D), and by surprising because it displaces the B  of the G-minor tonic.
chromatic neighbor motions that emphasize 5̂, (E  –C  –D). Although it can be integrated into the middleground as a
The phrase concludes with a descending seventh, E  –F . modally-mixed mediant chord (  iii), this structural interpre-
When F  is first heard in m. 8 of the solo violin introduc- tation does not explain why B-minor harmony appears.
tion, it does not sound anomalous: it seems to function in a The notion of a non-functional pitch-class motive helps ex-
plain the role of B minor in this movement. Example 6(a)
shows a voice-leading graph of mm. 1–17, which normalizes
15 Schenker, in his analysis of Chopin’s Etude op. 18, no. 12 (1979, 100), register and maximizes stepwise voice leading. Obscured by
makes special mention of a motivic association of pitch-classes. In Fig. intensely chromatic passing motions and frequent shifts of
119, 14(a) and (b), he notes that the pitch classes G–A–G recur with register, the underlying harmonic progression of the passage
different harmonic support. He writes, “Here we have the repetition of
is a prolongation of tonic harmony, which moves to a
a mere neighboring note figure which maintains its pitch position while
the harmonic degrees (V–I …) change; yet precisely this feature [the
dominant-seventh chord in measure 17. The beam connect-
pitch-class retention] contributes greatly to the cohesiveness of the ing the pitch classes B  –B–C on the graph draws attention
whole” (p. 100, emphasis added). I mention this example to show that to a linear motive of the movement. The enigmatic B-minor
what I call pitch-class motives are prevalent in tonal music and are freely harmony of measure 9 introduces the chromatically altered
acknowledged by Schenker. Although one could focus on the tonal B  . After the chromatic manipulations of mm. 9–16, this B 
function of this G–A–G progression as a neighbor-note figure, I be- ultimately passes to C in measure 17. Despite the obscuring
lieve Schenker’s special mention of the different harmonic support em-
effect of mm. 9–16, the B  –C passing motion becomes trans-
phasizes the pitch-class recurrence, not the tonal functions of the
pitches. Some contemporary Schenkerian scholars have also noted the
parent at the very last moment in mm. 16–17, where B leads
cohesive properties of what I call pitch-class motives. In chronological prominently to C in the contrabass part. This ordered pro-
order: Schachter 1982; Cadwallader 1983 and 1984; Beach 1988; and gression of pitch classes, (B  –B–C) is subsequently enlarged.
Laitz 1996. For a related discussion, see Cohn 1992. At a deeper middleground level, the pitch classes of this
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 272

272 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

ä = 108
Ł Łm Łm ð ² ð
Allegro moderato
Š −− 00 Ł ²Ł Ł Ł
Ł ð Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Ł Ł Ł
Ł Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Violino

]\
− ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ
Š − 00
Piano !
Ý −− 0 ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ ÿ
0

m ² Łm ¦ ðm Łný Łný
² Łm ¦ Ł
Ł ý ² Łný
¹ Ł Ł
Łý
1
Š −− ðý ¦ Łm
9
Ð Ł ²Łý Ł ¦Ł
²Ł Ł Łn ¦ Łn
cresc. [ ² Łn
n Ł n Łn
¦ Ł ² Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł − Ł Ł Ł Ł ²¦ ŁŁ Ł ² ŁnŁ Ł ŁnŁ Ł ¦ Łn Ł ² Łn Ł
² Ł Ł Ł
− V-le con sord. ² Ł ¦ ðm Ł ²Ł ²Ł
Š− Ł Ł ²Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł
¦ Ł Ł ²Ł ²Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł
! \\ cresc. ][ conn sord.
Archi

Ý −− ² Ł ¦ ðm Ł ² ŁŁ ² ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ −² ŁŁ ŁŁ Ł ² Ł Ł ² Ł Ł ¦ Ł
Ł Ł ² Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł ¦Ł −Ł Ł ² Ł ²² ŁŁ
¦ Ł Ł ²Ł ² Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł
C-b. con sord.

² Łný ðný
¹ ¦Ł ² Ł ý Ł Ł
pizz.
−Ł ¦ Ł ¦ Ł ¹ 2
Š −− ¦ Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł Ł −Ł −Ł Ł −Ł ðý
15

² ŁŁŁ
arco
ŁŁ ¼
3

 Ł
6

 ŁŁŁŁŁŁ Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł
n [ n n Ł Ł Ł
\
6

¦ n Ł − ŁŁ
Ł − Ł Ł Cl.¦ Ł m m Ł
− Ł − Ł Ł ¦ ² ŁŁ ¦ Ł ²−ŁŁ ŁŁ −Ł ¦ ðŁ ¦ Ł ð ý Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł
Š− Łð ý −Ł Ł ² ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ¼ Ý Ł Ł ² Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
! ][ n
ðý
− Łm
\ V-c.
Ý −− ¦ Ł
−ŁŁ −Ł −ŁŁ ¦Ł Ł Ł −ŁŁ ¦² ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł Ł
¦Ł Łm Łm Ł ¦Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł
¦ Łm C-b.

example 5. Prokofiev, Violin Concerto No. 2, i, mm. 1–20.


679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 273

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 273

1 9 13 14 15 16 17

Š − ŁŁ ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ² Ł ²¦ ŁŁ Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł ¦¦ ŁŁŁ ¦−ŁŁ −Ł ¦ Ł ²¦ ŁŁ ²¦ ŁŁ Ł Ł
!
( )

Ý −− Ł ¦ Ł ¦ ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ ²Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł
ð −Ł Ł ² Ł ¦ ŁŁ ¦ ¦ ŁŁ −Ł ŁŁ −Ł ¦ Ł ¦ Ł Ł ² Ł
ð
( )

i V7
B− B¦ C
Exposition

(a)

92 101 110 123 128 180 182



Š− Ł −Ł ²¦ ŁŁ ¦Ł ŁŁ Ł
²Ł Ł
 
Ý −− ð Ł ¦Ł ²Ł Ł ¦Ł ð
¦iii
pitch-class motive


III iv V
B− C
Development

(b)
example 6. Pitch-class motives in Violin Concerto No. 2, i.

motive are reiterated as tonicized keys of the development Example 6(b), however, C is given consonant support on the
section. Example 6(b) shows the deeper-level presentation of middleground within a C-minor harmony. Despite the dif-
this motive. ferent tonal functions of the pitches in these two presenta-
If this were a systemic motivic enlargement, one would ex- tions of the motive, the recurrence of the melodic line itself
pect consistent harmonic support between pattern and repe- encourages associative recollection. The point I wish to stress
tition. For example, in the surface presentation shown in is that non-functional pitch-class motives associate pitch-
6(a), pitch-class C is a dissonant seventh within a V7 har- classes, not tonal processes.
mony. If the dissonant aspect of the C were retained in the Although the chromatic elements of the B-minor passage
middleground, then some aspect of its tonal function would in the exposition can be explained as passing or neighboring
be preserved between the motivic presentations. As shown in tones (F  as lower neighbor to G; B  as passing tone to C),
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 274

274 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

they also create extra-systemic associations, which I refer to 1 5 2 associates R


as pitch-class motives.16 Because B minor recurs prominently 6 3 7 agents R
in both the exposition and development of the Second 4 1 5 bases R
Violin Concerto, it gains an associative meaning beyond its Subdominant Tonic Dominant
participation in voice-leading structures. Pitch-class motives
associate chromatic events such as this across structural example 7. Scale degrees and their functional descriptions
levels. Patrick McCreless has a similar description for an as- (from Harrison, 1994, 45, Fig. 2.1).
sociative motive, “It [a motive based on association] requires
voice leading to be worked into the musical structure, but it
is ‘detachable’ as a motive and cross-referenced in a way that
voice leading is not.”17 tions by disassembling the traditional harmonic unit, the
triad, into its constituent elements. Based upon its role in
functional pitch-class motives the primary triads, each scale degree is ascribed a function.
As shown in Example 7, each primary triad contains three
In the Violin Concerto No. 2, the pitches of the fore- scale-degree functions—a base, an agent and an associate.
ground and middleground versions of the motive have dif- Voice leading is a motion between these functional states.
ferent tonal functions. Consequently, the B  –B–C motive is The functional aspect of some pitch-class motives is based
not dependent on tonal structure. Sometimes, however, a upon the tonal associations created by the discharge of
pitch-class motive has a tonal “feel” to it, despite the fact that agents, bases and associates.
it is not a diminution of a deep middleground tonal para-
digm. I call such a motive a functional pitch-class motive. functional pitch-class motives in “the young juliet”
With a functional pitch-class motive, an ordered progression
of pitch-classes embodies a functional voice-leading pattern, A clear example of a functional pitch-class motive occurs
even though the motive’s harmonic support is not a direct in Prokofiev’s “Young Juliet” from Romeo and Juliet, shown in
diminution of the deepest middleground. Example 8. This example is especially useful because it high-
My conception of a pitch-class motive draws upon the lights several kinds of harmonic support for the motive in
theoretical model of scale-degree functions proposed by question. The first eight measures of the piece can be divided
Daniel Harrison in Harmonic Function in Chromatic Music.18 into two four-measure phrases, each ending with an authen-
Harrison’s theory of scale-degree functions provides a basis tic cadence. Within each phrase, the bass descends by major
for illuminating tonal associations created by voice leading thirds, featuring a succession of major triads built upon
connections. He systematically describes scale-degree func- C, A  , E, and C. In both cases, this progression is marked
by a sudden change of surface design: the texture shifts to a
chordal style, the harmonic rhythm accelerates dramatically,
16 For a similar analytic strategy, see McCreless 1990, particularly his
a higher register is introduced, and the harmonies receive
discussion of Brahms’s “Wie Melodien zieht es mir,” and Schubert’s
‘Pause’.”
special emphasis.
17 McCreless 1990, 131. The distinctive surface realization endows this chromatic
18 The following discussion is based upon the theory of scale-degree har- gesture with the potential for association. The gesture is, in a
monic functions found in Harrison 1994, 43–72. manner of speaking, marked. Indeed, the B–C discharge of
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 275

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 275

l −− ŁŁŁl l ¦ ² ŁŁŁl l ¦ ŁŁŁn l Łl Łn Ł Ł Ł Ł n n


² ŁŁ Ł ŁŁl Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł Ł ² Ł
ä = 144
o l Ł l
−ŁŁŁ
Vivace
l Ł Łl ŁŁl ² ¦ ŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ¼ŁŁ ¹
Š 00 Ł ¾ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¾ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł  Ł
! n
][ Łl

][l
lŁŁn
\
² Łl Łl
n l l
Ý0 Ł ¹ ¼ ½ −− ŁŁ ¹ ² ŁŁl ¹ ¦ Ł ¹ ¹ Š ŁŁ ¹ ¼ Ý Ł ¹
Ł ¼ ¹ ¦ ¦ ŁŁ ¹ Ł ¹ ¼
−ŁŁ
0 ŁŁ ¦ Ł ¦ ŁŁ   ² Łn Ł Ł ŁŁ
n n n

Ł n
Ło ¾ ŁŁŁŁŁ o n¦ ² ŁŁŁo n ¦ ŁŁŁo ² Ł ² Ł ¦ ŁŁ ² Ł ² ŁŁl Łl ŁŁ ŁŁl ŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ ¼ŁŁ
Ł Ł ¾ Łl Ł ² Ł ² Ł
5
Ł Ł Ł ¼ Ł ²Ł ² Ł ¾
Š l Łl Łn −−ŁŁ Łu ¾ Ł Ł Ł Ł ² Ł ² Łu ¹
! ][n
Ł

Łl
( )

Łn
\
Ł ¹ Łl ² Łl ¹ ¹
]\ ][
n l l
Ý ŁŁ ¹ ¼ ½ −− ŁŁ ¹ ² ŁŁl ¹ ¦ Ł ¹  ² Ł Ł ¹ ¹ Ł ¹ ¼
 ¦ Ł ¦ ŁŁ ²Ł ² Ł ²Ł Łl Łl Łn
Ł Ł
n n ² Łl
[bm: i V I]
nŁl Łl Łl Łl Łl l l Łl l l Łl l l l l l
Łl Ł Łl Łl Łl Ł Łl Łl Łl Ł Łl Łl Łl Ł Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Ł Łl Łl Łl Łl Ł
9

ŠŁ Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl l Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl ² Łl Łl Łl ² Łl .0
1

!
2

l
[ Łl \l Łl
Łl Łl ¹ Łl l
¹ Łl ¹ Ł ¹ Łl ¹ Łnl ¹ Ý Ł l Łl Łl
Ý Ł ¹ ¹ Š  ¹ Ł Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Ł Łl .0
ŁŁ  l
n Łl Łl

−Ł Ł Ł Ł
13

Š .0 Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Ł 00 Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Ł Ł
Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl l l l Łl Łl l Łl Łl Łl l Łl Łl
! l
l
[
n l Łl l l Łl l l Łl l l Łl
l l Łl
l
l ¹ Ł ¹ Łl ¹ − Łl
Ý . Ł ¹ ŁŁ ¹ 00 ŁŁ ¹ ¹ Š Ł Ł ¹ Łl ¹ ¹ Łl ¹ Łl ¹ Ł ¹ l Ł ¹
0 Łl Łn
l ¦ ŁŁŁl l ²²² ŁŁŁl Łn ² Łl
o l ² l
Ł Ł lŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ Łl Łl
¾ ² Ł ² Ł Ł Ł ² Ł ² Ł Ł ¾ Ł Ł Ł ²Ł
17

Š Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl ² Łl Łl ² Ł ² Łl Ł ² Ł Łl Ł ² Ł ² Łl Łl ² Ł Ł Ł 
! Łl ²\Łl subito ² Łl
Łl l l Łl l l Łl l l Ł Ł Ł Ł
l l l Łl l ² Łl l Łl l Łl Łl ² Łl
[
Ł
n
Łl ² Łl

][l
Ł
¹ ¹ ¹ Łl ¹ Łn ² Łl Łl Łl Ý ² Ł ¹ ½ ŁŁ ¹ ² Ł ¹ ¦ Ł ¹ ¹ Š Ł
² Ł ²
Š ² Ł Ł Łl Łl ² Łl ŁŁ ¼ ²Ł Łn
 n
example 8. Prokofiev’s piano transcription of “The Young Juliet” from Romeo and Juliet, op. 75, no. 4, mm. 1–30.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 276

276 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

² Łnl Ł ² Ł ² Ł Ł nl n Ło ¾ Ł Ł Ł Ł
21
²² ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ¦ Ł Ł ² Ł ² Ł ² Ł ¦ ŁŁ Łl −Łl l ¦ Ł ¼
5

Ł Ł o¾Ł n¦ ² ŁŁŁl n¦ ŁŁŁl


Š Ł
1 3

Ł Ł − ŁŁ ŁŁ ¦ ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ¹ Ł l Łl Ł Ł
−ŁŁ Ł Ł ŁŁ
! n
² ŁŁl ¹ \ ² Łl
ŁŁl ¹ Ł ¹ ² Ł ¹
n l l [
¦ ŁŁn

 l Ł
n
( )

Ł
− l Ł ¦
n l
Ł
²
ݲ Ł ¹Ł Ł ¹ ¼ ½ −ŁŁ ¹ ² ŁŁ ¹ Ł ¹ ¦ Ł ¹
Š  ¼ ² Łn ¼ ²Ł Ł Ł
Łn ¼  − l ¦ Łl Ł Ł
¦ Łl Łl
l
² Ł Ł ² Ł Łl l ŁŁŁŁ Łl ŁŁn m Ł Ł ¹ Łm Ł Ł Ł Łm Ł ¹ Łm Ł Ł Ł Ł
25

Ł ² Ł ² Ł ¾ ² Ł ² Ł ² Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¹ −− − Ł ¹ Ł Ł Ł Łm  ¹ Ł ý
Š Ł ¾ Ł Ł Ł Ł ² Ł ² Łu − ŁŁ
! u
\o ]\ ][ n Ł


m
\¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ
m ¦Ł
Ý Ł ¹ Łl ² Łl ² Ł ¹ ¹ Ł ¹ ¹ Ł ¹ −− − Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¹ ¦ ŁŁ Ł ¹ −Ł
Ł ¼ ¹ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł
²Ł ² Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł ¹ ¼ − ½ Ł Ł Ł ¹ ð ýý
n ² Łl l Łl Łl Ð ð 5 3

example 8. [continued]

the progression is a key component of several motivic repeti- support of a melodic neighboring motion, and each includes
tions throughout the movement; these are bracketed in a discharge of the dominant agent, 7̂–1̂. Regardless of its har-
Example 9. Initially, B is supported by an altered mediant monic context, when a dominant agent discharges to a tonic
(III  ); in m. 4, it is supported by a cadential dominant; and a base, it contributes a melodic tonal association by reproduc-
few measures later, it is tonicized by a i–V–I progression ing a strong element of the tonal motion from a dominant
(mm. 7–8.) The root of the B harmony acts as a dominant chord to a tonic chord.19
agent to the C harmony that ends the phrase in m. 8. The In this excerpt, the motivic repetition involves not only
deeper-level repetition of the motive, shown in Example a recurrence of pitch classes, but also a recurrence of scale-
9(c), occurs in mm. 19–23, where the theme is presented in degree discharge, which imbues the motive with some degree
E major—a key that refers to the fleeting E-major harmony of tonal affect. By introducing scale-degree functions, it may
in m. 2. seem as if I am blurring the distinction between the func-
Despite the fact that the pitch-class motives in “Young tional basis of a systemic motive and the associative basis of a
Juliet” occur in different harmonic contexts, the melodic
function of the motive never changes; in every recurrence, B 19 Some may be inclined to call the descending major-third progression a
acts as a dominant agent to C. Conventionally, a dominant wrong-note progression because of its bold chromatic shifts. Although
the term does capture an insouciant character of the music, there is
agent is supported harmonically within a dominant triad, as
nothing tonally transgressive about this progression. Schenker presents
in m. 4 and m. 8. In Prokofiev’s descending major-third ver- a similar descending arpeggiation by major thirds in Free Composition,
sions, the dominant agent is supported as the fifth of the Fig. 100, 6. As Example 9 shows, the progression also participates in
mediant triad (E major). Example 9(b) compares these pro- normative voice-leading patterns. Because the term wrong note is par-
gressions. Observe that each progression features consonant ticularly misleading, I refrain from using it.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 277

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 277

1 5
7̂ 1̂


1̂ 7̂ 1̂ 7̂


Ł Ł Ł
Š ŁŁŁ −− ŁŁŁ ¦ ² ŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁŁ −Ł ¦Ł ¦Ł ŁŁ −−ŁŁŁ ¦ ² ŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ ²Ł
Ł
! Ł Ł
ÝŁ Ł Ł
−Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł Ł −Ł ¦ Ł ²Ł Ł
Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł
( )


( )

Major-third progression [b: i V I]


Major-third progression
I I ii V I I I V I

(a)

1 19 23 24

7̂ 1̂

7̂ 1̂ 7̂

1̂ Ł Ł Ł
Š −−ŁŁŁ ¦ ² ŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ² ŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ
! − ŁŁ Ł Ł
E-major
presentation of
Ý Ł Ł
theme mm. 19–22
Ł Ł
−Ł ¦ Ł −Ł Ł
Ł Ł
I −VI V I I I6 I
Major-third Conventional
version Version

(b) (c)

example 9. Functional pitch-class motives in “The Young Juliet,” Romeo and Juliet.

pitch-class motive. The functional aspects of a pitch-class mo- lower neighbors—again, transformations unavailable at the
tive are limited, however, unlike those in a systemic motive. first middleground level. The functional aspect of a pitch-
The harmonic progression from C major–A  major–E major– class motive derives from its melodic discharge of a scale-
C major cannot exist at the first middleground level; conse- degree function, irrespective of its harmonic support. By
quently, any functional aspect of the motive derives from divorcing the voice-leading motion of a functional pitch-class
more surface tonal events. In addition, the B  s in Example 9 motive from its harmonic context, the motive manifests only
function as either chromatic ascending passing tones, or as one aspect of tonal association—a tonal allusion, so to speak.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 278

278 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

In contrast, systemic motives reify the roots of the tonal sys- As described earlier in Example 2, the opening of the
tem. Although the systemic motive demonstrated in the movement features an antecedent phrase within an inter-
“Classical Symphony” is expressed in several different keys, rupted structure. Despite its conspicuousness, the F  -minor
it can always be heard locally as an expansion of tonic harmony in m. 8 does not contribute significantly to the har-
harmony—its harmonic context remains unchanged. In monic syntax of the phrase. From a structural perspective,
the functional pitch-class motive of Example 9, the harmonic the most important mid-phrase harmony is the D-minor
context changes. harmony of m. 10. As shown in level (b) of Example 2,
In summary: systematic motives are diminutions of first- D minor results from a 5–6 motion above F.
level middleground progressions; non-functional pitch-class This structural interpretation coexists with an alternate
motives are ordered progressions of pitch classes; and func- reading that is based on motivic relationships. Example 10(a)
tional pitch-class motives are ordered progressions of pitch shows three chromatic ideas of the first and second theme
classes with scale-degree functions. I have provided three areas: x, C–C  –D; y, F–F  –G; and z, B  –B–C. The
analytic examples to demonstrate each type of motive. The F  -minor harmony in m. 8 supports pitches in both motives
“Classical Symphony” illustrates an example of a systematic x and y. As part of motive y, the opening bass F moves to
motive, in which the fundamental descent is repeated in dif- F  in m. 8. Because it is the leading tone to the G-major ap-
ferent keys and at different structural levels. In the Violin plied dominant, one would expect this F  to resolve to G.
Concerto No. 2, a non-functional pitch-class motive, B  –B–C, This resolution, however, is delayed by the D-minor har-
is repeated at different structural levels, and the notes in each mony and is not achieved until m. 16. In this interpretation,
occurrence of the motive have different tonal functions. In the D-minor harmony is not a goal but rather a parenthetical
“Young Juliet,” a functional pitch-class motive is also repeated insertion. Thus, a structural interpretation emphasizes
at different structural levels, but each recurrence features a D minor whereas a motivic interpretation de-emphasizes
dominant agent that discharges to a tonic base. Despite vary- D minor in favor of the G-major harmony (the resolution of
ing harmonic contexts, this motive’s voice leading reproduces the motive’s F  leading tone). Aspects of both readings are
a strong element of the tonal motion from a dominant chord supported by surface elements. D minor is highlighted by a
to a tonic chord. stepwise bass descent to D (m. 9), after which this harmony
is sustained for four measures. In a motivic reading, the par-
ii. a closer look at pitch-class motives enthetical feeling of the D-minor harmony is reinforced by a
sudden change to a softer dynamic level and to a less active
This section explores more complicated examples of pitch- bass. (The dynamic level and bass activity resume after the
class motives, paying particular attention to the interaction be- D-minor insertion.)
tween tonal process and motivic design. Pitch-class motives
can model associations among events, including notes that combinations of pitch-class motives
are not privileged in a structural manner; notes that are em-
phasized by motivic relationships can differ from the notes Pitch-class motives can also inform interpretations at the
emphasized by the tonal structure. To demonstrate this middleground level. A comparison between a tonal and mo-
phenomenon I will re-examine the third movement of the tivic analysis of this same movement proves instructive in
Violin Sonata op. 94, and compare a structural interpretation this regard. Each section of the ternary form is clearly delin-
of the opening phrase with a motivic interpretation. eated by sharp contrasts on the surface. The opening A sec-
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 279

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 279

tion (mm. 1–34) features a lyrical melody; the B section (mm. pretation adds an extra-systemic association that enriches
35–64) features a frenetic and stuttering violin solo; the A our understanding of chromatic events.
section (mm. 65–94) returns in the distant key of G  major.
From a tonal perspective, G  major is an unusual harmony functional pitch-class motives—
for the return of the main theme. Although there are many a closer look
notable exceptions, thematic recapitulations often begin with
the resumption of tonic harmony. However, it is extremely While the previous discussion highlights the indepen-
rare for a thematic return to occur in the Neapolitan, or  II. dence of pitch-class motives from tonal structure, the follow-
As Example 10(b) shows, we can explain this harmony as ing discussion takes a closer look at functional pitch-class mo-
the by-product of modal mixture, a neighboring tone, and tives and their special connection with tonal structure. An
passing tones—although it is indeed unusual for a  II chord examination of several excerpts demonstrates that tonal asso-
to lead to a diatonic II.20 Although a Schenkerian interpre- ciations created by functional pitch-class motives can vary in
tation accounts for the G  harmony as a chromatically- strength. In other words, some motives sound more “tonal”
altered II chord, it does not fully explain the unusual setting than others. The strength of a motive’s tonal association de-
of the thematic return. pends upon the type of scale-degree function it contains, and
As described previously in Example 10(a), the first two the degree to which that function is harmonically supported.
themes contain references to motives x, y, and z. Example The functional pitch-class motives of the “Young Juliet” ex-
10(c) shows the interaction of these motives at a middle- ample feature dominant agents. As the strongest communi-
ground level. As the example shows, each of the three mo- cators of scale-degree function, agents control much of the
tives is enlarged. Moreover, the enlargement procedures perception of tonal motion in these motives. An agent ’s
overlap at m. 66, the return of the initial theme. This re- tonal force is based upon its singularity of purpose. 6̂, 3̂, and
markable moment features modal mixture, thematic return, 7̂ function solely as agents (refer to Example 11). Unlike
and a multi-level integration of these motives. Thus, I would agents, bases and associates share scale degrees with other
argue that motivic design helps explain the unusual key of functions, and are therefore more dependent upon context.
the thematic return, relating the quirky F  harmony of the As shown in Example 11(a), 5̂ can be both a dominant base
first phrase to the middleground G  -major return of the and a tonic associate. 7̂, on the other hand, always functions
opening theme. While a tonal interpretation would tend to as a dominant agent, regardless of context.21
relegate the G  -major harmony to a subsidiary function (one Not only do agents have a singular purpose, but they also
that would be subsumed by the structural tonic and domi- have consistent behaviors. As Example 11(b) shows, the
nant harmonies), a motivic interpretation highlights this dominant agent, 7̂, normally discharges to 1̂ both in progres-
harmony as the point of coincidence—the “nexus” between sions to tonic function as well as progressions to subdomi-
the three chromatic ideas. In summary, an analytic strategy nant function. Similarly, the subdominant agent, 6̂, norma-
that includes pitch-class motives can offer explanations for tively discharges only to 5̂.22 Since agents have consistent
Prokofiev’s chromatic language that differ from interpreta-
21 Scale-degree functions are determined by the primary triads (I, IV, V).
tions based solely on tonal structure. Thus, a motivic inter- When 7̂ is heard in other triads, such as a mediant harmony, it still
functions as a dominant agent. See Harrison’s discussion of functionally
mixed chords, Harrison 1994, 60–72.
20 Schenker (1979, 41 and 71) discusses this chromatic expansion. 22 Harrison 1994, 91–96.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 280

280 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

First Theme Second Theme


1 8 10 15
y
17
á 41 53
A = A-section material

Ł
B = B-section material
Š − Ł ²Ł ¦ ŁŁ Ł ¦Ł
!
1 35 53 66 72 79 81
Ł ( )
3̂ 2̂ 1̂
z
Š − ðŁ ² Ł ŁŁ ² Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł − Ł −Ł ð ð
!
x ( )

Ý− Ł ¦Ł Ł A B A
Ł ²Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦Ł Ł −Ł ¦ ŁŁ ¦ Ł Ł
y á Ý−
ð Ł Ł Ł − Ł −Ł ¦ Ł ð ð
Pitch-class motives in the first and second themes

−II − ¦
(a) I II V I
Tonal Structure

(b)

A = A-section material
B = B-section material
1 17 41 66 72 z 79 80 81
18 35
z −Ł Ł
Š − Ł ² Ł ¦ ŁŁ ² Ł Ł −Ł ¦ Ł ŁŁ
y

! x x
−Ł ¦Ł

Ł Ł
A B A
Ý− Ł ¦Ł Ł Ł
Ł −Ł ¦Ł
y
I V I
Pitch-class motives in Violin Sonata op. 94, mvt. 3

(c)

example 10. Tonal structure and pitch-class motives in Violin Sonata op. 94, iii.

normative resolutions, they can create an expectation for therefore agents, with their singularity of purpose and behav-
a specific voice-leading motion. As demonstrated in the ior, are the strongest ambassadors of tonal function. When a
“Young Juliet” example, the presence of 7̂ can incite an ex- note discharges as a functional agent, it contributes a tonal
pectation for a resolution to 1̂. Other scale-degree functions association because it embodies normative tonal melodic
do not share the agent ’s clarity of function or discharge; motions.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 281

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 281

Dominant Dominant Tonic framed by a beginning D-pedal point in m. 31 and an end-


Agent Base Associate
Š ŁŁ Ł ing A-pedal point in m. 48. Despite the lack of an explicitly

! Ł Ł stated D-minor triad in mm. 31–34, there is a tangible


D-minor “feel” to these measures, owing to the pedal point,
Ý Ł Ł ŁŁ the D–A interval between the outer voices, and the inner
voice’s outline of the triad A4–F5–D5. Given these implica-
C: 7̂ 5̂ 5̂ tions and the fifth relationship between the pedal points, I
interpret this passage as a progression from a D-minor tonic
(a) to an A-major dominant. At m. 40, the D of the D-minor
harmony slides to D  , which is subsequently re-interpreted
in m. 44 as a C  . As shown in Example 13, the upper voice
Dominant agent discharge Subdominant agent discharge follows the half-step slide in the bass, creating parallel fifths
C: 7̂ 1̂ 7̂ 1̂ 6̂ 5̂ 6̂ 5̂ between the outer voices.23 The resultant melodic A  of mm.
Š ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ 40–44 (enharmonically reinterpreted as a G  in m. 44),
! Ł Ł Ł functions as a dominant agent to the A of the final A-major
harmony implied in mm. 48. The G  dominant agent, circled
ÝŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł in the example, is supported by an E-dominant base, also cir-
D - T D - S S - D S - T cled, which descends by fifths from E to A.
However, E is not always given a proper bass context in a
(b) lower voice; thus the supportive role of the E as a base func-
tion is not emphasized in the measures leading up to m. 47.24
example 11. Unique agent function and agent discharges. In fact, as the third of the C  harmony, E is not explicitly
stated in mm. 44–46. The base support of E changes drasti-
cally, however, in m. 47, when E discharges by falling fifth in
the lowest sounding register. This base position and dis-
combinations of scale-degree functions charge of E adds a strong dominant flavor to the C  har-
mony as it leads to A major. In fact, the discharge of both G 
The tonal association of a functional pitch-class motive can and E articulates the concluding A-major harmony in m. 48.
be strengthened if the agent function is supported by another
scale-degree function of the same primary triad. For exam-
ple, if a motive contains a dominant agent, the tonal associa-
tion will be greater if the agent is also supported by a domi- 23 One could also interpret the D  harmony of m. 40 enharmonically as
a  vii chord. This passage remains tonally ambiguous until the end of
nant base.
m. 47.
Example 12, from the Piano Sonata No. 2, illustrates the 24 Unlike an agent, the function of a base is dependent upon its context.
discharges of both an agent and a base dominant function In order to invoke its function, a base must be the lowest sounding voice
and demonstrates the strongest possible tonal association for in a chord, or it must be accompanied by a functional agent. For a dis-
a pitch-class motive. The middle of the scherzo movement is cussion of these conditions of base function see Harrison 1994, 45–47.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 282

282 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

Ł Ł
Ł −Ł Ł Ł −Ł Ł
rit.
ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ² ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł ² Ł Ł −Ł Ł ² Ł Ł
25

Š Ł −ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ Ł Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ
! ^[ Ł ²Ł ²Ł Ł [ ]\ \ \\
Ý
Ł ¹ ¹ ŁŁ Ł
Ł ¹ Ł ¼ ÿ ÿ ÿ ½ ¼ Ł
Ł 3 ŁŁ 2

Ł Ł Ł Ł l Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł ŁŁl Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł
a tempo

Ł ² ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł −ŁŁ
31

Š ²¦ ŁŁ Ł ¦ ŁŁl Ł ²² ŁŁl Ł ¦ ŁŁl Ł ¦−ŁŁ Ł ²¦ ŁŁ Ł ¦Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł


! Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Ý
Ł 1
Ł Ł
5 Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
2
5 5

¼ − ¼ŁŁ Ł rit. − Ł
l Ł Łl Ł − ðð ýý Ł ¼ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł l Ł l Ł
a tempo
37
² Ł − Ł Ł ¼ −Ł − −− ŁŁ Ł −ŁŁ Ł ¦ ŁŁ Ł −ŁŁ Ł ¦ ŁŁŁ Ł −¦ ŁŁŁ −Ł

Š ²− ŁŁ Ł ² ŁŁ Ł − ð ý Ł Ł
! Ł Ł Ł
l l l l
Ł Ł Ł
Ý −−−−
Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł −Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
rit. a tempo
Ł " Ł
− ¦ Łl Ł l Ł ² ² Łl Ł Łl Ł
agent
Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Š − −− ¦ ŁŁ Ł −ŁŁŁ Ł ² ² ŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁl Ł
43

¦ ŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁl Ł ¹ ²Ł Ł
Ł
ŁŁ Ł ¦ Ł ½ Ł ² ŁŁl Ł ¦ ŁŁl
! Ł
¼ Ł Łð Ł ² Ł
²²² Ł Ł Ł Ł
base
Ý −− − Ł
− Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
example 12. Piano Sonata No. 2, Scherzo, mm. 25– 48.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 283

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 283

31 34 38 40 44 47 48 1 6 8 10 16 17
Motive
Base scale
Ł
− Ł ²Ł ² Ł ¦² ŁŁ ² ŁŁŁ
agent 3̂ 2̂
ð ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł − Ł Š − ðŁ
degree

² Ł Ł Ł − Ł Ł ² ŁŁ ŁŁŁ ð ²²ŁŁ
!
Ł − Ł ¦ Ł − Ł − Ł Ł
( )
Š 
!
( )

agent
A: 7̂ 1̂ D: 7̂ 1̂ discharge: 7̂ 1̂
Ý 5̂ 1̂
Ý− 5̂ 5̂ oblique Ł Ł
ð −Ł ²Ł Ł ð ð ²Ł Ł Ł ð
base
 base
discharge: 5̂ 1̂
Dm: i V I V

example 13. Dominant agent and dominant base, Piano (a) (b)
Sonata No. 2, Scherzo, mm. 31–48.
example 14. Dominant agent and dominant base in Violin
Sonata op. 94, iii, mm. 1–17.

The more scale-degree functions a harmony contains (i.e.


the more it resembles a dominant or subdominant triad), the
greater its potential for tonal associations. For example, a and D-minor harmony. If a base is not the lowest sounding
chord that contains one scale-degree function, such as a voice, it will usually not discharge by fifth. More likely, in
dominant agent, has the potential only to replicate one voice- terms of voice-leading conventions, a dominant base in an
leading discharge of the dominant-tonic progression, namely upper voice will be maintained in the subsequent tonic
7̂–1̂. A chord that contains two scale-degree functions has chord; this is shown in Example 14(a). The lack of base dis-
the potential to replicate two voice-leading discharges. In charge (from A to D) considerably weakens its dominant
other words, voice leading creates the tonal associations in support of the C  leading tone. A base that is in an upper
pitch-class motives, not the mere presence of a scale-degree voice can add a dominant presence to a harmony, however, if
function. Example 14 clarifies the distinction between the the base does not discharge, it cannot create tonal associations
presence of a scale-degree function and the voice leading generated by voice leading motions. Example 14(b) shows a
created by its discharge. hypothetical re-voicing of the F  harmony that emphasizes
A closer evaluation of the opening of the Violin Sonata is the base discharge possible between F  minor and D minor.
instructive. Here, a dominant agent is supported by a domi- Were the A in the lowest voice and were it to discharge to
nant base, but the base does not discharge. As Example 14(a) the tonic base of the D harmony, it would then have a signif-
shows, the opening tonic of the movement moves to a domi- icant impact on the tonal association of the motive.
nant half-cadence at m. 17. In the middle of the phrase, the Examples 13 and 14 demonstrate varying degrees of tonal
C  in m. 8 acts as a leading tone to the D harmony of m. 10. association that a pitch-class motive can acquire. The tonal as-
C  is supported by an F  -minor harmony, which contains A, sociation of the voice leading in mm. 47–48 of the Piano
5̂ of D minor. 5̂ is not, however, in a salient base position in Sonata is stronger because the scale-degree discharge of its
the lowest voice; the A is maintained between the F  -minor pitch-class motive is supported harmonically. By contrast,
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 284

284 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

the pitch-class motive in the Violin Sonata receives far less shown in Example 16(a), C  is part of a pitch-class motive de-
support. fined by B  –C–C  –D. Notice that the tenor voice retains the
C  as the remaining voices progress to a D-minor chord in
weak tonal associations in pitch-class motives m. 8. This dissonant context for C  accentuates its “wrong-
ness.” C  does not resolve to D in the upper-voice register
The tonal associations of motives in the examples dis- (  2̂–3̂) until the B  harmony appears in m. 9.26 Because the
cussed thus far have been fairly clear. Agents provide clear motive does not contain a dominant agent, the pitches of this
tonal expectations in terms of voice-leading resolutions; motive are less functional compared to earlier examples. In
these motions can be bolstered by supportive bases. Some this case, the chromatic pitch of the motive accentuates and
pitch-class motives, however, form weaker associations. resolves to the third of the following chord, instead of the root.
“Masks” from Romeo and Juliet illustrates a pitch-class mo- The motion from  2̂–3̂ is not as functionally prescribed as
tive that has a weaker tonal reference than the motive exam- the motion from 7̂–1̂. Other scale-degree functions do not
ined in “Young Juliet” or the Violin Sonata. At first glance, share an agent ’s authority as purveyors of functional voice
“Masks” conforms to many classical tonal expectations. As leading.27
shown in Example 15, its formal organization is conven- The B section begins in m. 22 with a sudden drop to a
tional, comprising an introduction (mm. 1–4) and a ternary piano dynamic level, and ends with the only authentic cadence
design: A (mm. 5–21); B (mm. 22–29); and an abbreviated of the movement (mm. 29–30). In between, there is another
recapitulation (mm. 30–39). In addition, the first phrase of startling F  -minor harmony (mm. 26–28), which supports a
the A section resembles a sentence, with a two-measure restatement of the section’s opening measures, transposed to
melodic idea (mm. 5–6), a loosely-varied repetition of this this unconventional key. As shown in Example 16(b), the
idea (mm. 7–8), and a four-measure continuation that cul- pitch-class motive recurs in the B section, shortened to
minates in tonic harmony (m. 12). Although the melodic B  –C  –D (shown by the beam). The scale-degree functions
construction of this phrase seems reminiscent of a classical ( 2̂–3̂) are preserved in the repetition of the motive in the
style, the harmonic language is less conventional, especially B-section, creating a weak, yet still existent tonal association.
the startling F  -minor harmony in m. 7.25 The C  of the motive initially receives consonant harmonic
If we were to disregard Prokofiev’s spelling, we could hear support as part of the F  harmony in m. 26. C  is retained
the opening as a descending arpeggiation, B  –G  –D–B  . throughout the chromatic progression leading from the domi-
Spelling notwithstanding, I hear mm. 5–9 as a prolongation nant of F  (m. 28) to the dominant of B  (end of m. 29.)
of B  , particularly with the stepwise descent of the bass. This
interpretation, however, does not negate the quirky character 26 The C  of the motive receives an even more prominent emphasis in the
of the passage, which is emphasized by Prokofiev’s choice of varied repetition of this phrase that immediately follows (mm. 14–21).
spelling. In the repetition of the phrase, C  is enharmonically reinterpreted as
The F  -minor harmony that occurs in the middle of the D  and supported with a D  -major harmony (m. 17). In the first ver-
sion, C  sounds dissonant against the prevailing D-minor harmony of
phrase (m. 7) supports the chromatic passing tone C  . As
m. 8, whereas in the second version, C  is consonant on the surface of
the music because it motivates a D  -major harmony (m. 17).
25 Another unconventional aspect of the harmonic language is the om- 27 Harrison calls this scale-degree succession a chromatic projection of the
nipresent semitonal clashes in the left-hand part, which add unsettling resolution of a dominant agent. For a discussion of chromatic accompa-
dissonance to the scene. niments to agent discharges see Harrison 1994, 106–8.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 285

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 285

Masks
Transcription: Op. 75, No. 5 (1937)
Introduction
Andante marciale ä = 72 From Romeo and Juliet, Op. 64 (1935–36)

Š − 00 Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl Łl ¹ ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ
! \ ¦ ŁŁ ]1 ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ¦ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ
l l
ŁlŁ ŁlŁ ¦ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ ŁlŁ
l l
un poco cresc.

Ý −− 0 ²Ł ¦ Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl Ł Ł Łl − Ł
Łl ²¦ ŁŁ Łl Ł ŁlŁ ¦ Łl Ł Łl ² Ł Łl Ł ŁŁ l ² Ł Łl Łl Łl Ł Ł
0 l l l − Łl Łl Łl
n l l
A section
n n l l n l l n l l n m m n
con effetto
l l l l Ł Ł ¦ ðý ¦Ł Ł Ł ð ý Ł Ł −Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
− Ł
Š − ŁŁ Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł¹ Ł¹ ŁŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ( ) ¹ ŁŁ ŁŁ ¼ Ł ( ) ¹ ŁŁŁ Ł¹ ¦ ŁŁŁ ² Ł¹ ²² ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ m
5

! ¦ ŁŁ
]\
l
2
]1
¹ ŁŁ
l
l l − ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Łl ŁŁ
l ln l n l
l l l
n ²
l
Łl
²Ł
l ln l ln l
Ł Ł
n Łl
Ý −− Łl Ł Ł Ł Ł l Łl Łl Łl Łl ¦ Łl l Ł Ł Ł
ŁŁ ŁŁ l ŁŁ l ŁŁ l ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ² ŁŁ Łl −Łl ²² ŁŁln ¦ ŁŁ ² Ł l Ł l Ł l Łl
ln ln ln ln ln ln ln ln ln Łln Łln Łln ¦ Ł
n l l n l l l n
l l l n n ll n
1 1 1 5 1

Ł Ł
−− Ł −Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁŁŁ
9

Š ¹ Ł ¹ Ł ²Ł Ł ¦ Ł
¹ Ł
Ł ¦ Ł¹ ² ŁŁ Ł ² Ł Ł ² Ł ² Ł Ł − Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ð ý Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
!
¦Ł (¦) Ł Ł Ł ¹ ² ŁŁ ¹ ¦ − ŁŁ ¹ ¦ Ł ¼ ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł −Ł
l l l l l l l ¦Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
l lnl lnl
Łl Łl Ł l Łm ¦ Łm
Ý −− Ł Ł Ł Ł  ¾ Ł Ł Ł Ł  ¾ Ł Ł¾ Ł Ł Ł
3

Ł Ł  Ł
Ł $$ ¾ Ł Ł
l Ł Ł −Ł Ł l Ł l Ł l Š
Ý
Łý Ł Łý Ł Łý Ł Ł
Ł Łl n l ² Ł ý Ł Ł Ł Ł − Ł
n l l ln ln ln

Ł n Łn Łn Łn n l l l l n Łl Łl ¦ ðný l l
Ð n Ł l ŁŁ l ŁŁ l ŁŁ l Ł
Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ
Ł ¦ ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ¦ ð ý ŁŁ ŁŁ Ł
()
− Ł Ł Ł
Š − ¹ ¦ ŁŁŁŁ Ý ¦ ŁŁŁ Š ŁŁŁŁ Ý ŁŁŁ Š ŁŁŁŁ Ý ŁŁŁ ŠŁŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ ¹ ŁŁŁ Ł¹ Ł¹ Łl ¹ l ( ) ¹ −ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ¼ Ł
12

!
2

[ lŁ l n l n l [[ l l l l l lŁl ln lŁl ln lŁl Łl


]1

n Łl Ł Ł Ł
Ý −− Ł Ł Ł l ŁŁ
Ł Łl
ŁŁ
Ł Łl
ŁŁ
Ł Łl −ŁŁŁlŁ ŁŁŁlŁ ŁŁŁlŁ ŁŁŁlŁ Łn Ł Łn Ł Łn Ł Łn Ł ² Ł Ł Łn Ł Łn Ł Łn Ł
ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ

7777777

7777777
7777777
7777777
² ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ
777777

777777
777777
n n n n n n n n 777777 Ł Ł Ł Ł
example 15. Prokofiev’s piano transcription of “Masks” from Romeo and Juliet, op. 75, no. 5.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 286

286 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

n l l l l n n ll n
¦ Ł n ² Ł Ł − ð ý l l l n
Ł ¦ ŁŁŁ ² Ł ²Ł ² Ł ²² Ł ŁŁ − ð ý Ł ŁŁ Ł l l l
−− (¦) Ł ŁŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ¦ ¦ŁŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
()
16
Ł Ł ² Ł¹ ² Ł ¹ −Ł Ł Ł Ł −Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł ²Ł
ŁŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ
¦ Ł
Š ¹ ¹ ¦ ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ¦ Ł ¹ ¦ ŁŁ ¹ (¦) ŁŁ ¹
! Łl
l l
Łl n²² ŁŁŁl
l l [[ l ln l
− Łl Łl
ln l
Łl Łl
l l l l

n
Ý −− ² Ł Ł n
Ł Ł ²Ł Ł ²Ł Š n
Ł −Ł n
ŁŁ Ł n
ŁŁ Ł n Ł Ł Łl ¾ Ł Ł Łl ¾ Ł Ł Łl ¾ 3

² ŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ ²² ŁŁŁ


²Ł Ý
²²² ŁŁŁl −−−ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ Ł 
Ł Ł 
Ł Ł 
Ł Ł Ł $$ ¾
²² ŁŁŁ
² ŁŁ Ł ýŁ Ł ýŁ Ł ýŁ Ł Ł Łl

7777777

7777777
7777777
7777777
7777777

77777777
777777
n
n l ln n ll n Ł Ðn ŁŁ Ý Ł Ł Ł
1 1 1 5 1

19
−− Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł ŁŁŁ Ý ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁ Ł
Š ¹ ² ŁŁ ² Ł ¦ − ŁŁŁ ² Ł ² Ł Ł − Ł Ł Ł Ł
Ł ¹ Ł Ł ð ý Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Ł Ł Ł −Ł Ł ¹ ¦ ŁŁ Š ŁŁ
!
Š
¹ ¹¦ Ł ¼ ¦ Ł
Ł Ł
Ł Ł
Ł Ł
Ł Ł
Ł Ł Ł l l l l n l ln l
l l l l ln l ln l m [ n
Ł ¦ m
Ł
dim.

Ý −− Łl Łl Łl Š Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łl
Łl Ł Ý ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ
² Łný Ł Łl Łn Łl − Łl ŁŁl ŁŁl − ŁŁl ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ
n n n n n n n
n n l l Łm Łl m Łl Łl n l l l l l l l ¦ ²²²
Łl Ł n n n
B section
l
3

−− Ł Ł l Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
22 3

Š ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Łl ŁŁ ýý ŁŁ ŁŁ ² ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ¦
¦ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł Ł
! \ l l
nl Ł nl Ł nl Ł nl Ł l l
¹ Ł ¹ Ł ¹ Ł ¼
l l l n Ł
Ł
l
n Ł Ł
l n Ł n Łll
Łý
n
Ł ð

Ý −− Ł Ł Ł Ł ¦ Łl Łl Ł Łl Łl Ł Łl Łl Łl Łl ¦¦ ²²²
ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ Łl ŁŁ Łl ŁŁ Łl ŁŁ Łl ¦ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ − ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ Ł
7777777
7777777
7777777

7777777

7777777

7777777
7777777
7777777
ln ln ln ln ln ln ln Łln
n
²²² Ł Ł Ł Łn ² Ł Ł Łl Łl n n n marcato e corto
¦
² Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ² ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ²² ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁ ¦ ¦ ŁŁŁ ² ŁŁŁ −¦ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ¦¦ −−
26

Š ¾ Ł Ł
! \
¹
l
¹
l
¹
l ² Łln Łl ² Łln Ł Łln Łl Łln Łl Łln Łl ² Łcresc.
ŁŁ ]\
n Łm ¦ Łn Łm ² Łn Łm n m
Ý ²²² Ł ŁŁ ¾ Ł ŁŁ ¾ Ł ŁŁ ¾ Ł ŁŁ ¾
3 3 3 3 3 ŁŁ 3ŁŁ
Ł  Ł Ł  Ł Ł  ¾ ² Ł Łl Ł Łl ² Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łm ² Ł Łm Ł Łm ¦² ŁŁ m ¦¦¦ −−
¾ ¾
3
Ł Ł
Ł  Ł  Ł  Ł  Ł Łln Łl Łln Łl Łln Łn ² Łn n n Ł
ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ n n
n n n n n n n
example 15. [continued]
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 287

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 287

n n l l n l l n m m
A' section
l l l l n Ł Ł ð ý Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
Ł Ł ŁŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ² Ł ² Ł
− Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ ¦ ŁŁŁ Ł¹ Ł ŁŁ ð ý Ł ŁŁŁ Ł ŁŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ¦Ł Ł ² Ł ² Ł Ł ŁŁ
Š − ¦ ŁŁŁ
30

¹ ŁŁ ¹ ¹ l ¹ −Ł ¹ Ł¹ ¹ ¹
! l Ł Ł ¼
2

l
]1
l l l l ln l ll l ll l
[[n Łl n Łl n Łl n Łl Łl n Łl Łl Łl Ł Ł
Ý −− ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł ² Łn Ł Łn Ł Łn Ł Łn Ł ²Ł Ł Łl −Łl ¦ Łn Ł ŁŁ ² Ł Ý
Ł −Ł ²² ŁŁ

777777
ŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁŁ ² ŁŁŁ Ł Ł Ł ² ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ¦ Łn Š ² Łl

777777
ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ

7777777

7777777

7777777

7777777

7777777
777777

777777

777777

777777
n n l l n
ðý l l l n l l l n n l l
Ł l l n
−− ð ý Ł ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł −ŁŁ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł −Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
33

Š ¹ Ł ¦ ŁŁ Ł ŁŁ Ł m ² Ł ¦ Ł Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł Ł ŁŁŁ Ł
− Ł
¹ ² ŁŁ ² Ł¹ ¦ − ŁŁŁ ² Ł¹ ¦² ŁŁ
Ł
¹ ¦ ŁŁ ¹ (¦) ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ ¹ ŁŁ
! ²Ł Ł l Ł
l ln l n l

[ l
Ł Łl ¾
l
Ł Łl ¾
l
Ł Łl ¾
l
][ l l l
¼

Ý −− ² Ł Łl Ł Łl Ł Łl n Ł Ł Ł
3

ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ Łl Ł ŁŁ  ŁŁŁ  Ł Ł Ł  Ł Ł Ł $$ ¾ Łl Ł
ln ln ln ¦ Ł Łý Łý Łý Ł Łl ² Łný Ł Łl Łn Łl − Łl

1 1 5 1
m ri - - te n - l- l nun - - to
n Ł ¦ Ł Ðn
1

− ðý Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ð¹ ý ŁŁ ŁŁ Ý Ł Ł Ł ¦ Ł Ł Ł ð ý ¦ Ł ŁŁ Ł Lento
36

Š ¹ Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ¹ Ł
Ł Ł ¹ ŁŁ ¹ Ł
¹ Ł
−Ł Ł Ł ŁŁŁ ¦ ŁŁ ŁŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁŁ ¼ ¹ l l l ¹ Ł ¼
! ¦ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ ŁŁ
l ln l ln l
Ł Ł Ł ŁŁ
Łm ¦ Łm
\ l ln l n l l n
dim.
l l l l l
\\
Ý −− Łl Łl Łl Ý ¦ ŁlŁ Ł
Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł
ŁŁ ŁŁ − ŁŁ Š ŁŁ ŁŁ l ŁŁ l ŁŁ Łl − ŁŁl Łl ŁŁ Łl ŁŁ Łl − ŁŁ Łl ŁŁ − ŁŁ −ŁlŁ ŁŁ ŁlŁ ŁŁ ŁlŁ Ł
ln ln ln ln ln ln l n ln ln ln l l l l
8vb

example 15. [continued]

Another manifestation of the motive occurs in mm. dominant-to-tonic motions in the first theme avoid a root
10–12, near the cadence of the first theme. This pitch-class motion from 5̂–1̂, exhibiting a lack of dominant base dis-
motive occurs in retrograde in the bass voice, as shown by the charge. The dominant harmonies occur in second inversion,
bracket in Example 16(a). The motive is particularly notice- which emphasizes the linear connection from C to B  of the
able because a base discharge is expected at this cadential bass voice (mm. 8–9 and 10–11).
moment. By subverting our expectations, Prokofiev draws This retrograde form of the motive invokes a different
attention to the motive at the cadence. In fact, the two kind of association than the other iterations of the motive.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 288

288 music theory spectrum 26 (2004)

First Theme denies a functional association, but still creates an associative


5 6 7 8 9 10 11
connection owing to pitch-class content.
3̂ 2̂ 1̂
Ł In retrospect, Prokofiev exploits the dichotomy between
−− Ł Ł Ł ² Ł ¦ ŁŁ −ŁŁ ŁŁŁ Ł ¦ Ł ²xŁ Ł Ł two versions of 2̂, namely C  and C . The diatonic version
Š Ł −Ł ¦ Ł ² Ł Ł Ł ²Ł Ł ²Ł Ł Ł Ł (C ) behaves according to tonal norms as part of second-
!

inversion dominant harmonies. On the other hand, the chro-
Motive X ²2̂ 3̂ matic version (C  ) motivates unusual harmonies (mm. 7, 16,
Ý −− Ł ŁŁ ¦ Ł ² Ł ŁŁ
5 6 26), which add excitement and color. In addition, Prokofiev
−Ł ² Ł ¦ Ł Ł plays with an enharmonic pun between C  and D  (mm. 10,
Ł ¦Ł
 Ł Ł Ł −Ł Ł Ł 17, 19.) With reference to the piece’s title, perhaps these
Retrograde of Motive X compositional features are Prokofiev’s way of presenting
III ²
*
I iii V 43 I V 64 I musical “masks.”

(a) conclusion

The unusual chromatic motions that characterize much


Second Theme of Prokofiev’s music have more than a surface effect. As seen
22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 in the examples from the Violin Concerto No. 2, “Young
shortened Motive X ²2̂ 3̂ Juliet,” Violin Sonata op. 94, and “Masks,” surface chromatic
− ²²
Š − Ł Ł ŁŁŁ ¦ Ł Ł ¦ ŁŁŁ Ł −ŁŁŁ ² ŁŁ Ł Ł ²² ŁŁŁ ² ŁŁŁ ¦ Ł ² Ł ¦ −ŁŁŁ Ł
progressions often have middleground references and mani-

! 
Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł ²Ł ¦ Ł
festations. These embedded chromatic ideas at both surface
and deeper levels of the music invite motivic recollection.
Ý −− ²²² However, these associative connections are different than

Ł Ł Ł Ł Ł ² Ł ¦ Ł ¦Ł −Ł systemic motives, which replicate transformations available


 only at the deepest middleground level. Nonetheless, some
[ F ²m: i V] 5̂ 1̂ pitch-class motives reify well-established functional voice-
I * Ger4 ² V75 ² I
6 leading patterns, providing them with a distinctive tonal fla-
2 vor. Depending upon the functional strength of the recurrent
voice-leading pattern, and the number of corroborating
(b) scale-degree functions within the harmonic support of the
example 16. Pitch-class motives in “Masks” from Romeo motive, pitch-class motives can manifest a wide range of tonal
and Juliet. associations.
Pitch-class motives enrich our understanding of Prokofiev’s
quirky chromatic gestures. For more than fifty years, we have
The retrograde version of the motive substitutes the upward called Prokofiev’s chromatic slides wrong notes, a term that
melodic resolution,  2̂–3̂, for a descent towards the tonic highlights their disconnection with tonal structure. A mo-
pitch, thus reversing the directed tendencies of the pitch- tivic perspective provides an alternative viewpoint with a
classes within the motive. Reversing the order of the pitches more optimistic reading. I would argue that Prokofiev’s
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 289

a theory of motives for prokofiev’s music 289

chromatic excursions are not “wrong” at all. I prefer to call Kramer, Jonathan. 1988. Listen to the Music: A Self-Guided
them “motivically optimized.” Tour Through the Orchestral Repertoire. New York:
Schirmer Books.
references
Laitz, Steve. 1996. “The Submediant Complex: Its Musical
Alegant, Brian and Don McLean. 2001. “On the Nature of and Poetic Roles in Schubert’s Songs.” Theory and Practice
Enlargement.” Journal of Music Theory 45: 31–72. 21: 123–65.
Ashley, Patricia. 1963. Prokofiev’s Piano Music: Line, Chord, McCreless, Patrick. 1990. “Schenker and Chromatic Tonici-
Key. Ph.D Dissertation, University of Rochester. zation: A Reappraisal.” in Schenker Studies, ed. Hedi
Austin, William. 1956. “Prokofiev’s Fifth Symphony.” Music Siegel. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 125–45.
Review 17: 205–20. Minturn, Neil. 1997. The Music of Sergei Prokofiev. New
Bass, Richard. 1988. “Prokofiev’s Technique of Chromatic Haven: Yale University Press.
Displacement.” Music Analysis 7: 197–214. Rifkin, Deborah. 2001. “What’s Wrong? Tonal Theories
Beach, David. 1984. “Motive and Structure in the Andante and Prokofiev’s ‘Wrong-Note Music’.” presented at the
Movement of Mozart’s Piano Sonata K. 545.” Music Society for Music Theory conference in Philadelphia, PA.
Analysis 3: 227–41. ———. 2000. “Tonal Coherence in Prokofiev’s Music: A
Beach, David. 1988. “Motivic Repetition in Beethoven’s Study of the Interrelationships of Structure, Motives and
Piano Sonata Op. 110, Part I: The First Movement.” Design.” Ph.D. dissertation, University of Rochester.
Intégral 2: 75–97. Rothgeb, John. 1971. “Design as a Key to Structure in Tonal
Burkhart, Charles. 1978. “Schenker’s Motivic Parallelisms.” Music.” Journal of Music Theory 15: 230–53.
Journal of Music Theory 22: 145–76. Salzer, Felix. 1962. Structural Hearing: Tonal Coherence in
Cadwallader, Allen. 1983. “Motivic Unity and Integration of Music. New York: Dover Publications, Inc.
Structural Levels in Brahms’s B Minor Intermezzo, Op. Schachter, Carl. 1982. “Beethoven’s Sketches for the First
119, No. 1.” Theory and Practice 8: 5–24. Movement of Opus 14, No.1: A Study in Design.”
———. 1984. “Schenker’s Unpublished Graphic Analysis of Journal of Music Theory 26: 1–21.
Brahms’s Intermezzo Op. 117, No.2: Tonal Structure and _____. 1983. “The First Movement of Brahms’s Second
Concealed Motivic Repetition.” Music Theory Spectrum 6: Symphony: The Opening Theme and its Consequences.”
1–13. Music Analysis 2: 55–68.
Cadwallader, Allen and William Pastille. 1992. “Schenker’s Schenker, Heinrich. 1979. Free Composition (Der Freie Satz):
High-Level Motives.” Journal of Music Theory 36: 119–48. Volume III of New Musical Theories and Fantasies, trans-
Cohn, Richard. 1992. “The Autonomony of Motives in lated by Ernst Oster. New York: Longman.
Schenkerian Accounts of Tonal Music.” Music Theory
Spectrum 14: 150–70.
Harrison, Daniel. 1994. Harmonic Function in Chromatic
Music Theory Spectrum, Vol. 26, Issue 2, pp. 265–290, ISSN 0195-6167,
Music: A Renewed Dualist Theory and an Account of Its
electronic ISSN 1533-8339. © 2004 by The Society for Music Theory.
Precedents. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photo-
Kaufman, Rebecca. 1987. “Expanded Tonality in the Late copy or reproduce article content through the University of California
Chamber Works of Sergei Prokofiev.” Ph.D. Dissertation, Press’s Rights and Permissions website, at http://www.ucpress.edu/
University of Kansas. jounals/rights.htm.
679-133.MTS.Rifkin_pp265-290 8/30/04 11:45 AM Page 290
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

También podría gustarte