Está en la página 1de 8

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2005, 40 (1), 19–26

Aesthetic preferences and the attribution of meaning:


Environmental categorization processes in the
evaluation of urban scenes

Ma- Paz Galindo Ma- Carmen Hidalgo


Universidad de Sevilla, Spain Universidad de Málaga, Spain

I n the context of research into scenic quality, the purpose of the present paper is to establish categories of
urban landscapes on the basis of users’ aesthetic judgements. It also explores the role that the restorative
capacity of a place—in terms of the attentional restoration theory (ART)—together with a set of aesthetic
attributes, may play in more or less valued places in a city. A total of 132 residents from Málaga (Spain) were
chosen, with the city providing the physical framework for environmental reference. A questionnaire designed for
easy self-administration by subjects was used to collect information. Subjects were asked to identify three places
in Málaga that they considered to be most attractive and three that they considered least attractive. Participants
were asked to evaluate both the restorative properties—in terms of the ART—and the extent to which their first
choice displayed certain environmental characteristics. Participants expressed a clear aesthetic preference for
recreational sites for leisure/walking as well as those closely linked to the city’s historical-cultural identity. The
research also identified other categories of visual settings that could be used as a focal point around which to
centre future samples of scenes in a city context. Finally, the results obtained from the characterization of more
and less attractive places, from the variables used in this study, shed light on the dimensions of underlying
meaning that individuals use to categorize their environment and reinforce the idea that environmental aesthetics
seem to play an important role in individuals’ general well-being.

D ans le cadre de la recherche sur la qualité scénique, le but du présent article est d’établir des catégories de
paysages urbains sur la base des jugements esthétiques des utilisateurs. Il explore aussi le rôle joué
par la capacité de restauration d’un lieu—selon la Théorie de restauration attentionnelle (ART)—conjointement
à un ensemble d’attributs esthétiques, sur les endroits plus ou moins appréciés dans une ville. Au total, 132
résidents de Malaga (Espagne) furent sélectionnés, en considérant que cette ville offre un cadre physique
de référence environnemental. Un questionnaire auto-administré fut complété par les participants. Ces derniers
devaient identifier trois lieux à Malaga qu’ils considéraient comme plus attrayants et trois autres qu’ils trouvaient
moins attrayants. Ils devaient également évaluer les propriétés de restauration—selon l’ART—et l’ampleur avec
laquelle leur premier choix concordait avec certaines caractéristiques environnementales. Les participants ont
exprimé une préférence claire pour les aires récréatives de loisirs et de marche ainsi que pour les lieux associés à
l’identité historique et culturelle de la ville. La recherche a aussi permis d’identifier d’autres catégories d’aspects
visuels qui pourraient être utilisées comme éléments à partir desquels seront créées les scènes futures en contexte
urbain. Finalement, les résultats obtenus à partir de la catégorisation des endroits plus ou moins attrayants, à
partir des variables utilisées dans cette étude, nous éclairent sur les dimensions des significations sous-jacentes
utilisées par les individus pour catégoriser leur environnement. Ceci renforce l’idée que les aspects esthétiques
environnementaux joueraient un rôle important dans le bien-être général des individus.

E n el contexto de los estudios sobre calidad escénica, el presente trabajo se ha diseñado con el objetivo de
determinar categorı́as de paisajes urbanos a partir de los juicios estéticos de sus usuarios. Adicionalmente,
se ha explorado el papel que la capacidad restauradora de un lugar—en términos de la Teorı́a de la Restauración
Atencional—ası́ como un conjunto de atributos estéticos pueden desempeñar en los lugares más y menos
valorados de una ciudad. Para ello se seleccionó un total de 132 residentes en Málaga (España) ası́ como el
contexto fı́sico de dicha ciudad como marco de referencia ambiental del estudio. Como instrumento de recogida

Correspondence should be addressed to Ma- Paz Galindo, Departamento de Psicologı́a Experimental, Facultad de Psicologı́a,
Universidad de Sevilla, C/Camilo José Cela s/n, 41018, Sevilla, , Spain (E-mail: mpaz@us.es).).

# 2005 International Union of Psychological Science


http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/pp/00207594.html DOI: 10.1080/00207590444000104
20 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

de información se utilizó un cuestionario diseñado para ser autoadministrado fácilmente por los participantes. La
tarea que se les solicitó a éstos fue la de identificar los tres lugares de Málaga que les resultaran visualmente más
atractivos, ası́ como los tres que consideraran más feos. Asimismo se les pidió, en relación al lugar identificado en
la primera posición, que evaluaran tanto sus propiedades restauradoras—en términos de la ART—como el grado
en que exhibı́a determinadas caracterı́sticas ambientales. Los participantes expresaron una clara preferencia
estética por los lugares recreativos de ocio/paseo, ası́ como aquellos estrechamente vinculados con el desarrollo
histórico—cultural de la ciudad. Asimismo, se han identificado otras categorı́as de entornos visuales que pueden
servir como eje en torno al que articular futuros muestreos de escenas en el contexto de la ciudad. Finalmente, los
resultados obtenidos a partir de la caracterización de los lugares mas y menos atractivos—en las variables objeto
de este estudio—ofrecen luz sobre las dimensiones de significado subyacentes a través de la que los individuos
categorizan su ambiente y apoyan la consideración de la importante función que la estética ambiental parece
desempeñar en el bienestar general de los individuos.

In the field of research into environmental memory systems) and depends on the criteria of
aesthetics, preference studies have attempted to individual experts. Furthermore, very little of the
understand the scenic quality of a certain place by research mentioned above evaluated urban stimuli,
analysing the responses to it given by different or the role that familiarity or recognition of
groups of ‘‘nonexpert’’ participants. These visualized scenes may play in processes of aesthetic
responses have generally been used as a unit of evaluation. The development of evaluation proce-
analysis for generating predictive models of visual dures aimed at identifying categories of known
quality (psychophysical models) or frameworks for and/or familiar places for participants, that meet
explaining aesthetic experience (cognitive models). their own selection criteria, is an area that has
In research into cognitive models, some studies received little attention and could also provide
have used preference judgements as a unit of important information in terms of optimizing
judgement to delimit landscape categories that are processes of environmental sampling.
important for users. The results of such studies To overcome some of the above-mentioned
show that one of the underlying dimensions of limitations, an evaluation procedure was designed
perceptual differentiation is related to the ‘‘extent in a previous study to identify urban scene
of human intervention’’; in other words, the extent categories that were known and/or familiar to
to which the evaluated scene contains buildings nonexpert participants, using selection criteria
and what the perceived equilibrium is between established by the subjects themselves (Galindo,
human and natural elements. On this point, for 1994). A random sample of residents from the city
example, a clear distinction in the aesthetic of Seville was asked to identify three public
judgements of several groups of subjects has been landscapes in the city that they found most
established between natural and urban contexts, attractive in visual terms. The analysis of their
especially when the latter lack natural elements responses allowed us to establish four main
such as water and vegetation (Herzog, 1985; perceptive-evaluative categories for urban scenes
Kaplan, 1987; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989; Peron, that were important for users: (1) historical-
Purcell, Staats, Falchero, & Lamb, 1998; Purcell, cultural landscapes, (2) leisure areas, (3) recrea-
Lamb, Peron, & Falchero, 1994). Similarly, when tional landscapes, and (4) housing areas. These
the study has included a large variety of scenes, categories were configured, however, from the
certain attributes of spatial configuration (such as analysis of a single range of aesthetic preferences
the extent of openness and spatial definition, the (‘‘attractive’’ places) and were not characterized
normal function and/or use of the place, and the according to any other variable by the subjects
age of the site and its upkeep) form additional interviewed. Exploring the generality of such a
bases for the perceptive-evaluative categorization categorization in other geographical scenes, incor-
made by nonexpert individuals (for a review of porating the analysis of places considered ‘‘unat-
these studies see Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989). tractive,’’ is one of the aims of the present study. It
Most studies attempting to delimit landscape is based on the assumption that identifying more
categories have used photographs of natural or less ‘‘attractive’’ landscapes of one specific
scenes that are unfamiliar to participants, having molar setting provides an opportunity not only for
been selected by researchers using different cri- registering its main scenic resources (descriptive
teria. This procedure of stimuli selection and objective), but also for clarifying which implicit
presentation requires the fragmentation of the criteria of visual quality are used by nonexpert
molar environment (as represented in subjects’ individuals (explanatory objective).
AESTHETIC JUDGEMENT OF URBAN SCENES 21

Regarding the (implicit) standards of scenic 3. Characterize the more and less valued places
beauty, Nasar proposed (1994, 1997) two types of within the set of aesthetic attributes (sensorial and
variable in the context of urban design: Attributes semiotic) delimited in Nasar’s proposal.
of formal aesthetics and attributes of symbolic 4. Explore the relationships between the restora-
aesthetics. Among the former, the authors highlight tive capacity of a place and the aesthetic attributes
‘‘openness’’ (and/or spaciousness), ‘‘mystery,’’ selected in this study.
‘‘complexity’’ (both related to visual diversity),
and ‘‘order’’ (or congruence between the elements
that make up the scene). In relation to the symbolic METHOD
aesthetic attributes, Nasar mentions variables of
content such as ‘‘vegetation,’’ ‘‘upkeep,’’ ‘‘style,’’ Participants
and ‘‘perceived use.’’ Results from a substantial
number of empirical studies have demonstrated the The sample consisted of 132 university students
role that these characteristics play in aesthetic (72 women and 59 men, average age 26 years), all
evaluation responses. Some of the variables men- residents of Málaga city. They were selected from
tioned by Nasar have also been adopted in various a larger sample of 202 subjects. All participants
evolutionist theoretical proposals that have linked complied with the following criteria: (1) they had
aesthetic evaluation responses to certain scenes with been born in Málaga or had been living there for a
the important psychological benefits likely to arise minimum of 9 years; (2) they expressed a strong
from contact with these scenes (Appleton, 1987; sense of belonging to the city. This information
Kaplan 1987; Ulrich, 1983). In this context, the was collected from two questions in the ques-
Attentional Restoration Theory (ART; Kaplan, tionnaire described in the following section.
1995; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989) was developed
around the idea that certain settings may reduce
Procedure
tiredness caused by directed attention, thus
enabling the re-establishment of certain cognitive A questionnaire was designed to be easily self-
abilities. Other research has focused on the administered by the participants themselves. They
construction of an instrument to measure the were asked to do the following. First: ‘‘Think, for
restorative capacity of a specific place—in terms a while, about the city of Málaga itself; its streets,
of the ART—(Hartig, Korpela, Evans, & Gärling, neighbourhoods, squares and open spaces of any
1997) as well as analysing the role that this capacity type (large and small, attractive or unattractive…);
may play in the selection of ‘‘favourite’’ places think about all the places you have been to or
(Korpela & Hartig, 1996; Korpela, Hartig, Kaiser, seen.’’ After this, using the procedure followed in
& Fuhrer, 2001) or in aesthetic judgements (Peron, Galindo’s (1994) study, participants were asked to
Berto, & Purcell, 2002; Purcell, Peron, & Berto,
identify the three places in the city that were of
2001). Although, in this context, the existence of
greatest visual interest to them (open question),
relationships between the restorative capacity of
and state how often (in a question with closed
natural surroundings and their aesthetic evaluation
categories) they went to or passed by those places.
has been documented in some studies, there is very
The rest of the questions referred exclusively to the
little actual evidence of the existence of this
place that was their first choice; participants were
association in urban settings.
asked to complete the Spanish version (Hidalgo &
Within this framework, the overview guiding
Hernández, 2001) of the Perceived Restorativeness
the design of this study is establishing categories of
Scale (Korpela & Hartig, 1996), and a battery of
urban places from the perspective of their daily
11 questions related to the (sensorial and semiotic)
observers, as well as attempting to identify some of
aesthetic attributes identified by Nasar (1994,
the important dimensions of meaning that underlie
1997) and those featured in the research by
aesthetic preferences. Based on this overview, we
Galindo (1994). Specifically, participants were
have drawn up the following specific objectives:
asked for their opinion about the extent to which
1. Establish categories of more and less valued their first choice had the following characteristics:
urban places and analyse any similarities/differ- (1) vegetation; (2) visual diversity/richness; (3)
ences existing on the aesthetic value assigned (as harmony/congruence between its different ele-
well as their frequency of use). ments; (4) openness and/or spaciousness; (5)
2. Determine the role that the restorative capacity luminosity; (6) a historic place or representative
of a place (in terms of the ART) may exercise in its of the city; (7) cleanliness; (8) maintenance/upkeep;
aesthetic evaluation. (9) place for leisure activities; (10) meeting place;
22 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

(11) a novel place. Participants had to assess each the most attractive and unattractive places in the
characteristic on a 5-point scale with values city (those in the first place), from the percentage
ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (a lot). Participants of responses in each category.
were also asked to state how often they visited or The data shown in Table 1 reflect a notable
walked by their first choice. Once they had difference in the percentages of responses asso-
completed the ‘‘attractive’’ places section, partici- ciated with each of the categories identified
pants were asked to place themselves on the other depending on the aesthetic label (‘‘attractive-
side of the aesthetic spectrum and name, in their unattractive’’). The ‘‘attractive’’ places were
opinion, the three ‘‘most unattractive’’ places in mainly grouped around the following categories:
the city. These places were assessed with the same ‘‘recreational places’’ (mentioned by 38.75% of
variables as the ‘‘most attractive’’ places. Finally, participants), ‘‘historical-cultural places’’ (men-
the questionnaire included a battery of questions tioned by 35.65%), and ‘‘panoramic places’’
related to sample identification data considered (mentioned by 21.7%). On the other hand, these
to be variables of interest in the study (gender, three categories are scarce or even nonexistent in
age, length of residence in the city, level of the unattractive places, with the most numerous
familiarity with the city, and the extent to which being ‘‘housing areas’’ (73.17%), mostly working-
they identify with the city). class housing estates, followed by ‘‘industrial
areas’’ (8.13%).
If we consider the specific places that form the
RESULTS categories themselves, rather than the categories as
such, we find that 28 different places were
‘‘Most attractive’’ and ‘‘most unattractive’’ mentioned as the most attractive places, while
places in the city: General categorization there were 45 most unattractive places. In aesthetic
and frequency of use terms, the most highly appreciated place in the city
was the Gibralfaro Castle (n526, 19.7%) and,
First, all the first choices were recorded and more specifically, the panoramic view of the city
classified depending on the type or category of from the castle, which is why it was placed in the
place to which they belonged. As a provisional panoramic places category. In second place (n517,
instrument, the general system of categories 12.9%) came the Paseo Marı́timo (the Promenade),
obtained in Galindo’s (1994) study was used. a long esplanade running along the seafront which
The analysis of the characteristics of the spatial we placed in recreational places for leisure/walking.
and functional configuration of the places men- In third and fourth position came two places
tioned by the participants yielded a total of five closely related to the city’s history (placed in the
categories, three of which were the same as in category historical-cultural places): the Alcazaba,
Galindo’s (1994) study: ‘‘cultural-historical an Arab fortress dating from the 11th century
places,’’ ‘‘recreational places for leisure and/or (n515, 11.4%), and the Cathedral, built between
walking,’’ and ‘‘housing areas.’’ However, our the 16th and 18th centuries (n514, 10.6%). The
findings suggested that two new categories not rest of the places were mentioned by less than 10%
considered in the aforementioned study should be of the sample, with 13 places being mentioned by
introduced: ‘‘panoramic places’’ and ‘‘industrial just one participant (0.8% of the sample). As for
places,’’ while the category ‘‘places for having fun’’ the most unattractive places, the most mentioned
was excluded. Table 1 shows the representativeness was the neighbourhood of La Palmilla (n541,
of each of the categories established in the list of 30.8%), a working-class district with a high crime

TABLE 1
Frequency of most attractive and most unattractive places associated with each established category

Most Most
attractive places unattractive places
Frequency Percentage Frequency Percentage

Recreational places (for leisure or walking) 50 38.75 7 5.69


Cultural-historical places 46 35.65 9 7.21
Panoramic places 28 21.70 0 0
Housing areas 4 3.10 90 73.17
Industrial places 1 0.70 10 8.13
AESTHETIC JUDGEMENT OF URBAN SCENES 23

rate. The second, mentioned by 10.5% of subjects, TABLE 2


was another working-class area (housing area) with Characterization of most attractive and most unattractive
a high crime rate. The remaining places were places on the Perceived Restorativeness Scale
mentioned by less than 10% of participants, with ‘‘Most attractive’’ ‘‘Most unattractive’’
31 places being cited on just one occasion. Figure 1 place place
shows the frequency with which people visit these Subscales (PRS) M SD M SD
places: 11.5% go to the most attractive place daily
Being away 6.53 1.99 2.31 1.76
(against 7.8% who go to the most unattractive
Fascination 6.73 1.68 1.67 1.74
place); 18.3% go there once a week (11.6% go to Coherence 6.80 1.67 2.89 2.05
the most unattractive place); 14.5% go several Compatibility 6.32 2.02 2.00 1.68
times a month (10.9% to the most unattractive); Extent 7.00 1.73 3.56 1.81
24.4% go once a month (10.9% to the most General restorat. 6.66 1.45 2.35 1.16
unattractive); 21.4% have not been recently PRS: Perceived Restorativeness Scale. Values fall on a scale
(17.8% to the most unattractive place); 8.4% from 0 to 10 where lower values indicate, for example, lower
hardly ever go (31% to the most unattractive ‘‘being away.’’ Difference scores for the two types of places
place); and 1.5 have never gone to the most were statistically significant in all the subscales (p , .0001).
attractive place (10.1% to the most unattractive
place). Therefore, if we look at the percentages of
accumulated answers, 68.4% of the sample go at away (128)518.05, coherence (129)517.87, and
least once a month to the most attractive place, extent (128)517.81 (p , .0001 in all cases).
while the majority (58.8%) never, or hardly ever,
go to the places considered least attractive.
Characterization of ‘‘most attractive’’
and ‘‘most unattractive’’ places
Characterization of the ‘‘most attractive’’ according to the sensorial and
and ‘‘most unattractive’’ places on the semiotic aesthetic attributes
Perceived Restorativeness Scale
In order to meet the third objective, we calculated
The city’s ‘‘most attractive’’ places obtain high the average scores obtained by the attractive and
average scores on the Perceived Restorativeness unattractive places (the first choices) for each of
Scale (PRS), always over 6, in contrast to the the aesthetic attributes analysed. In addition, to
unattractive places whose values range from 1 to 3 check that the resulting differences were statisti-
on the scale (see Table 2). To check whether such cally significant, the T-test for related samples was
differences were statistically significant, the aver- used. As with the Restorativeness Scale, places in
age scores obtained were contrasted using the T- the city considered the most attractive obtained
test analytical procedure for related samples. All the highest scores in all the variables considered
the differences were statistically significant. The (see Figure 2). The analyses carried out showed
two types of scene differed both in general that the differences were statistically significant in
restorativeness (129)526.42, and in fascination all cases (p , .0001): novel place (128)512.42,
(128)524.59, compatibility (128)519.79, being facilities for leisure activities (125)510.57,

Figure 1. Frequency of use of most attractive and most unattractive places.


24 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

Figure 2. Average scores in the aesthetic attributes of the most attractive and unattractive places.

presence of vegetation (129)511.03, meeting place some cases at a lower level (p , .05) of significance
(128)59.97, cleanliness (129)517.22, upkeep/ (with the exception of the correlation between
maintenance (128)517.18, harmony or congruence harmony and fascination, which is not significant).
of scenic elements (128)519.34, visual diversity or On the other hand, it is worth pointing out that in
richness (129)525.86, luminosity (128)59.52, his- our study the presence of vegetation, a character-
toric or emblematic place (128)519.12, openness istic closely related to the restorative capacity of a
and spaciousness (128)512.71. place, does not present a significant correlation
with the total scale or with any of the factors.
Neither does the level of upkeep/maintenance, a
Relationship between the Perceived
Restorativeness Scale and the aesthetic variable closely associated with a preference for
attributes urban spaces, present a significant correlation with
the PRS. The three remaining attributes—diversity,
A correlation analysis was performed between the cleanliness, and historical characteristics—present
aesthetic attributes and the scores on the Perceived correlation values that, although low, are in some
Restorativeness Scale. The results are shown in cases significant at a reliability level of .05.
Table 3. Of the different attributes evaluated, five
correlate significantly (p , .01) with restorative
capacity: harmony, openness, luminosity, suitabil- DISCUSSION
ity for leisure, and meeting place. Of these, the last
two, along with openness, also present significant The analysis of the characteristics of the most
correlations with the five subscales, although in attractive and most unattractive places in the city

TABLE 3
Correlations between the aesthetic attributes and the Perceived Restorativeness Scale

Being away Fascination Coherence Compatib. Extent General restorat.

Vegetation .08 .14 .21 .07 .11 .14


Visual richness .10 .15 .14 .00 .17* .13
Congruence .26** .15 .26** .18* .26** .27**
Openness .30** .23** .27** .28** .38** .36**
Luminosity .19* .15 .16 .30** .17* .26**
Historical place 2.12 .02 2.07 2.20* .06 2.09
Cleanliness .05 .11 .20* .05 .19* .13
Maintenance 2.13 .07 2.01 2.04 .00 2.04
Facilities for leisure .20* .30** .18* .46** .19* .35**
Meeting place .30** .21* .28** .43** .22** .37**

*p,.05 (bilateral); **p,.01 (bilateral).


AESTHETIC JUDGEMENT OF URBAN SCENES 25

identified in the study has allowed us to establish viewing area (prospect). The settings that possess
the existence of five broad aesthetic categories of these characteristics—in other words, that allow
urban scenes: (1) recreational places for leisure/ the individual to observe the scene from a safe
walking, (2) historical/cultural places, (3) places viewing point and with a wide perspective (open
with panoramic views, (4) housing areas, and (5) places)—will also be the aesthetically preferred
industrial areas. The first three categories men- settings. The last of the categories, the ‘‘industrial
tioned form 96% of the visually most attractive areas,’’ represents the second most frequently
places; the last two groupings form the majority of selected unattractive places, which, together with
the settings considered most unattractive. Three the role this variable has in studies of environ-
categories (historical-cultural places, recreational mental preference, confirms it as a category to be
places, and housing areas) agree with the char- taken into account in future urban landscape
acterization made in Galindo’s (1994) study, which samples.
was used as a provisional instrument in this study. The differences found between this study and
In our opinion, this suggests that the function and Galindo’s (1994) may be due to the different
historical value (cultural representativity) of a characteristics of the geographical settings evalu-
particular scene—in other words, its social leg- ated. The city of Seville lacks public places that
ibility, to use Stokols and Shumaker’s (1981) could be clearly identified as panoramic places,
term—seem to constitute two important dimen- and in Málaga the ‘‘places for having fun,’’ a
sions providing the basis for the establishment of category in the Seville study that does not appear
aesthetic categories that are important in urban in this one, are spread throughout the city and not
contexts. Although the function of a scene has concentrated in one geographical area. The
been an important selection criterion in urban ‘‘industrial areas’’ category is one that appeared
landscape preference studies that have incorpo- in the responses to the most unattractive areas, a
rated photographic stimuli, the historical-cultural quality that was not evaluated in the first study. In
dimension has been rather neglected. The impor- any case, the categories that have emerged in the
tance that this dimension acquires when evalua- present study should be confirmed in other
tion procedures are developed that facilitate its geographical settings and using other nonexplora-
appearance seems to confirm the need for its tory procedures; an aim that we consider to be of
inclusion in contextualized analyses in scenes that great importance for research in this field.
are familiar to and/or known by subjects. Another of the goals of the present study was to
The groupings labelled ‘‘places with panoramic explore the role that the restorative capacity of a
views’’ and ‘‘industrial areas’’ provide two cate- place, in terms of the Attentional Restoration
gories not included in Galindo’s (1994) previous Theory, may play in its aesthetic evaluation. To do
study. The first category groups together 21.7% of this, subjects evaluated two places (the ones
the scenes identified as the most attractive and considered most attractive and most unattractive)
includes the place that was named in first position using the Spanish version (Hidalgo & Hernández,
by the greatest number of subjects, the Gibralfaro 2001) of the Perceived Restorativeness Scale
Castle. This place presents a set of physical (Korpela & Hartig, 1996). The results obtained
characteristics that help explain its high scenic show that the restorative capacity of a place seems
evaluation. It is an Arab fortress dating from the to be a characteristic dimension of those places
14th century, which was built into the side of a considered to be attractive. These places obtained
mountain that was originally beyond the city a high score both on the general scale (average
walls. The site was chosen for its strategic position, 6.66) and on the five subscales, with values ranging
overlooking the city and the sea beyond. Due to from 6.32 in the ‘‘compatibility’’ factor and 7.00 in
growth and expansion it has now become part of the ‘‘extent’’ factor (see Table 2). These results
the city, although certain planning laws have support those obtained by Korpela and Hartig
limited urban development in its immediate (1996) and Korpela et al. (2001), in which they
surroundings. It has therefore managed to pre- analysed the differences in restorative capacity
serve a significant amount of the surrounding between favourite places and hostile/disagreeable
vegetation and is an important transit point places. In the latter study, the greatest differences
between the city and nature. The visual config- between the two types of place were recorded in
uration characteristics of this place tie in directly the subscales of evasion and compatibility.
with Appleton’s (1975) prospect-refuge theory, However, the results of our research—given the
according to which the qualities of the setting that objective of characterizing the main scenic
make it aesthetically preferred allow one to see resources of a city—show that the main differences
without being seen (refuge) and provide a wide between attractive and unattractive places seem to
26 GALINDO AND HIDALGO

lie in the characteristics grouped in the fascination psicosocial de carácter integrador (in Spanish).
subscale. This subscale includes aspects related to Unpublished doctoral thesis, Universidad de
Sevilla.
the quantity of information available in the Hartig, T., Korpela, K., Evans, G. W., & Gärling, T.
evaluated place and is closely related to the (1997). A measure of restorative quality in environ-
psychological exploration process considered to ments. Scandinavian Housing and Planning Research,
be one of the basic cognitive requirements linked 14, 175–194.
to environmental aesthetics in Attentional Resto- Herzog, T. R. (1985). A cognitive analysis of preference
for waterscapes. Journal of Environmental
ration Theory (see, for example, Kaplan & Kaplan,
Psychology, 5, 225–241.
1989). In short, the results mentioned support the Hidalgo, M. C., & Hernández, B. (2001). Si te sientes
suggestions of recent studies (Peron et al., 2002; bien te recuperarás mejor: La evaluación de los
Purcell et al., 2001) that the restorative capacity lugares restauradores In C. San Juan, B. Rodrı́guez,
of a specific place may constitute one of the & A. Vergara (Eds.), Monografı́as Socioambientales:
implicit scenic quality criteria used by subjects in VII Congreso de Psicologı́a Ambiental. Human
Habitats–XXI (in Spanish). Barcelona: Publications
their aesthetic judgements. Universitat de Barcelona.
Furthermore, the scores that the attractive and Kaplan, S. (1987). Aesthetics, affect and cognition:
unattractive places obtained regarding the aes- Environmental preferences from an evolutionary
thetic attributes included in this study demonstrate perspective. Environment and Behavior, 19, 3–32.
their suitability in the study of aesthetic prefer- Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature:
Toward an integrative framework. Journal of
ences, and therefore suggest that these attributes,
Environmental Psychology, 15, 169–182.
together with restorative capacity, constitute Kaplan, S., & Kaplan, R. (1989). The experience of
important criteria for determining scenic quality. nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge:
The relation that could be established between Cambridge University Press.
both types of criteria raises a theoretical and Korpela, M. K., & Hartig, T. (1996). Restorative
empirical issue that could be dealt with in future qualities of favourite places. Journal of
Environmental Psychology, 16, 221–233.
studies. The results of the correlation analyses Korpela, M. K., Hartig, T., Kaiser, F. G., & Fuhrer, U.
between the score obtained in the PRS and these (2001). Restorative experience and self-regulation in
attributes—developed in an exploratory manner in favorite places. Environment and Behavior, 3,
this work—suggest that restorative places tend to 572–589.
be open, luminous, and coherent, and are places Nasar, J. L. (1994). Urban design aesthetics. The
for leisure and for meeting people. It is, however, evaluative qualities of building exteriors.
Environment and Behavior, 26, 377–401.
surprising that the presence of vegetation, a Nasar, J. L. (1997). New developments in aesthetics for
characteristic closely related to the restorative urban design. In G. T. Moore & R. W. Marans
capacity of a place in other research, did not have (Eds.), Advances in environment, behavior, and design,
a significant relationship with the factors on the Vol. 4 (pp. 149–193). New York: Plenum Press.
Perceived Restorativeness Scale. Given the Peron, E., Berto, R., & Purcell, A. T. (2002).
Restorativeness, preference and the perceived natur-
exploratory nature of these analyses, it may be alness of places. Medio Ambiente y Comportamiento
premature to venture an explanation. The possi- Humano, 3(1), 19–34.
bility does exist, however, that other aesthetic Peron, E., Purcell, A. T., Staats, H. J., Falchero, S., &
attributes, such as those mentioned here, might Lamb, R. J. (1998). Models for outdoor scenes: Some
have a greater influence on the evaluation of a experimental evidence. Environment and Behavior, 33,
place as a restorative setting. Future research 261–305.
Purcell, A. T., Lamb, R. J., Peron, E., & Falchero, S.
along these lines will clarify this hypothesis. (1994). Preference or preferences for landscape?
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 14, 195–209.
Purcell, A. T., Peron, E., & Berto, R. (2001). Why do
REFERENCES preferences differ between scene types? Environment
and Behavior, 33, 93–106.
Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of landscape. Stokols, D., & Shumaker, S. A. (1981). People in places:
London: John Wiley. A transactional view of settings. In J. H. Harvey
Appleton, J. (1987). Landscape as prospect and refuge. (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior, and the environment.
In J. A. Jackle (Ed.), The visual elements of landscape Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
(pp. 39–74). Amherst, MA: The University of Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to
Massachusetts Press. natural environment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill
Galindo, M. P. (1994). Evaluación de la preferencia (Eds.), Behavior and the natural environment
ambiental de paisajes urbanos. Hacia un modelo (pp. 85–125). New York: Plenum Press.

También podría gustarte