Está en la página 1de 7

Current Biology 23, 1663–1669, September 9, 2013 ª2013 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.

056

Report
Rapid and Persistent Adaptability
of Human Oculomotor Control in Response
to Simulated Central Vision Loss
MiYoung Kwon,1,4,* Anirvan S. Nandy,3,* and Bosco S. Tjan1,2 Rapid Development of Preferred Retinal Locus for Fixation
1Department of Psychology After having performed the experimental task with simulated
2Neuroscience Graduate Program central scotoma for approximately 3 hr spread over 2 to
University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA 3 days (free exploration, Table S1 available online), all six sub-
3Systems and Computational Neurobiology Laboratories, the jects in the experimental condition spontaneously followed the
Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA 92037, USA jumping target with eccentric fixation, keeping it out of the
4Schepens Eye Research Institute, Department of central scotoma (Figure S1).
Ophthalmology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Of the six subjects, five (two shown in Figure 2A) spontane-
MA 02114, USA ously formed a single preferred retinal locus (PRL) [6] for fixa-
tion near the border of the scotoma, whereas the sixth subject
developed two PRLs at roughly opposite sides of the scotoma
Summary (S5 in Figure 2A). For the five single-PRL subjects, the variance
of the fixational PRL decreased rapidly during this free explo-
The central region of the human retina, the fovea, provides ration period (Figure 2B). The variance (defined as bivariate
high-acuity vision. The oculomotor system continually contour ellipse area [BCEA]) measured at the end of this period
brings targets of interest into the fovea via ballistic eye move- was significantly smaller than that at the beginning (54%
ments (saccades). Thus, the fovea serves both as the locus reduction, t(4) = 2.97, p < 0.05). Fixation stability continued to
for fixations and as the oculomotor reference for saccades. improve with practice in the presence of the simulated
This highly automated process of foveation is functionally scotoma.
critical to vision and is observed from infancy [1, 2]. How
would the oculomotor system adjust to a loss of foveal vision Rapid Development of Rereferenced Saccades near
(central scotoma)? Clinical observations of patients with the Fixational PRL
central vision loss [3, 4] suggest a lengthy adjustment period More importantly, for the five subjects with a single PRL, their
[5], but the nature and dynamics of this adjustment remain first saccade after each target movement placed the target at
unclear. Here, we demonstrate that the oculomotor system or near their fixational PRL (Figures 3A and S2) and away from
can spontaneously and rapidly adopt a peripheral locus for the fovea, demonstrating a shift in oculomotor reference from
fixation and can rereference saccades to this locus in nor- the fovea to the PRL. Similar to that of the fixational PRL, the
mally sighted individuals whose central vision is blocked variance of the first saccade landing site (i.e., target location
by an artificial scotoma. Once developed, the fixation locus on the retina after the first saccade following each target
is retained over weeks in the absence of the simulated sco- movement) decreased rapidly (Figure 3B). The variance
toma. Our data reveal a basic guiding principle of the oculo- (BCEA) of the first saccade landing site during the last block
motor system that prefers control simplicity over optimality. of free exploration was reduced by 28% in comparison to
We demonstrate the importance of a visible scotoma on the that of the first block (t(4) = 5.02, p < 0.05). This noticeable
speed of the adjustment and suggest a possible rehabilita- change in the first saccade landing site after about 3 hr of
tion regimen for patients with central vision loss. exposure to the simulated central scotoma demonstrates a
remarkable adaptability of the oculomotor system.
We simulated a central scotoma in six normally sighted young The variance of the first saccade landing site was signifi-
adults using a gaze-contingent display while the subjects per- cantly larger than that of the fixational PRL, even for the last
formed a demanding visual search task. The simulated sco- block of the free exploration (t(4) = 2.98, p < 0.05). Oculomotor
toma appeared as a visible gray disc and blocked the screen rereferencing may require a longer time to fully develop. The
content at and around the center of gaze. A separate group slower time course of saccadic rereferencing has been
of six normally sighted subjects participated in a control observed in adult monkeys with bilateral foveal lesions [7].
experiment in which they performed the identical task without Thus, it appears that the refinement of the fixational PRL pre-
a simulated scotoma. The task alternated between two main ceded the refinement of saccade rereference.
components (Figure 1A): (1) object following, in which subjects
maintained fixation on an object as it was randomly reposi- Refinement of the Fixational PRL and Saccadic
tioned on the screen against either a uniform or cluttered back- Rereference after Explicit Training
ground, and (2) visual search, in which subjects searched for Despite the rapid emergence of both fixational PRL and
the target object shown in object following. Subjects were saccade rereference with simulated central scotoma during
instructed to perform the search task as quickly and accu- the free exploration, we observed that the variances of both
rately as possible during the visual search phase. During the remained higher than those of the foveal viewing control sub-
initial period of the experiment, subjects were not told to use jects (fixational PRL: t(8) = 2.70, p < 0.05; saccade rereference:
a particular eccentric location to guide their eye movements t(8) = 2.69, p < 0.05). We asked whether it would be possible to
(free exploration). further reduce the variances with explicit training. We dis-
played a small white cross at the retinal location of each
subject’s emerged fixational PRL (Figure 1B) and instructed
*Correspondence: miyoungkwon02@gmail.com (M.K.), nandy@salk.edu subjects to follow the target with this gaze marker (explicit
(A.S.N.) training). Otherwise, subjects performed the identical task as
Current Biology Vol 23 No 17
1664

A B Figure 1. Task and Experiment Conditions


Free exploration Explicit training (A) Task sequence during the free exploration
(i) Object following period. This sequence is an example of one trial.
with a blank background
+ There were 30 trials per block. Each trial was
comprised of four phases: object following
(ii) Centering of gaze against a gray background (i), centering of gaze
(ii), searching for the target object presented in
the object following phase with an array of
C nontarget distracters (iii), and object following
(iii) Visual search Invisible scotoma
(i) Object following amidst a cluttered background (iv).
(B) An example of object following during the
(iv) Object following (ii) Visual search
explicit training period. A small white cross
TI

(w0.7 in height) appeared at each subject’s esti-


M

with a cluttered
E

background mated PRL. Subjects were instructed to use the


cross as a gaze reference point for fixation and
saccadic eye movements.
(C) Task sequence of the invisible scotoma
experiment. The first phase was identical to the
first object following phase of the main experi-
ment except that the color and luminance of the simulated scotoma was matched to those of the background. As a result, the simulated central scotoma
was invisible to the subjects. Then, subjects searched for the target object among an array of distracters presented against a uniform gray background. (For
ease of visibility in the figure, target objects and the gaze reference cross were rendered at 2.53 the size used in the experiment relative to the rest of the
displayed elements).

before. With 15 to 25 hr of such explicit training (Table S1), all t(8) = 1.36, p = 0.21; number of saccades: t(8) = 1.30, p = 0.23).
six subjects were able to refine their oculomotor control such Although there were no significant changes in fixation dura-
that the variances of the PRL, and the first saccade landing site tion, there was a slight upward trend during the course of the
became comparable to those of the control subjects (Figures experiment (Figure 4C). The rate of change in both saccade la-
2B and 3B). Contrary to conventional wisdom [8], we found tency and the number of saccades mirrored those of the
that, using an effective training regimen, the oculomotor con- refinement of the fixational PRL and the saccadic rereference:
trol with peripheral vision can be as precise and accurate as a rapid decrease during the free exploration followed by a
that with foveal vision. persistent decrease during explicit training to levels that
were comparable to controls.
Retention of the Learned PRL in the Absence
of the Simulated Scotoma Changes in Visual Search Performance during PRL
To investigate whether the learned fixational PRL and saccade Development
rereference could be retained over an extended period of time Performance during the visual search component of the task
without practice, subjects were recalled for a retention assess- also improved over the course of PRL development (Figure 5).
ment (retention, Table S1) at least 1 week (up to 1 month) after Search accuracy was high (w89% 6 2.1% SD, relative to the
completion of the explicit training. Subjects performed the chance level of 50%, Figure 5A) and remained unchanged
same task (visual search and object following in clutter) as throughout the experiment. The average search time showed
they did during the free exploration period of the experiment a considerable decrease (42% reduction, Figure 5B) during
without any gaze marker. Furthermore, to assess the robust- the free exploration period followed by a gradual decrease
ness of the adaptation, we also tested with an invisible during explicit training. A corresponding decrease was
scotoma (in separate sessions, Figure 1C) during the object observed in the number of saccades that was required to
following phase by matching the color and luminance of the find the target (60% reduction, Figure 5C). At the end of explicit
scotoma to those of the uniform gray background (invisible training, subjects were performing the search task as fast as
scotoma, Table S1). All subjects retained the same PRLs, (t(8) = 20.94, p = 0.38) and with as few saccadic eye move-
even in the invisible central scotoma condition (Figures 2A ments as (t(8) = 1.30, p = 0.23) the foveal viewing controls.
and 3A). However, the variances of both the fixational PRL
and first saccade landing site for the retention and invisible Discussion
scotoma conditions were larger in comparison to those
measured at the end of the explicit training period (p < 0.05, We found that a stable PRL spontaneously emerges within
both comparisons). hours of performing a visual task with a simulated central sco-
toma. Saccades were rereferenced to the PRL. The acquired
Changes in the Characteristics of Eye Movements during PRL was retained for at least 1 week, during which subjects
PRL Development went about their daily lives with normal central vision. The
The characteristics of eye movements covaried with the devel- same PRL was used even when the simulated scotoma was
opment of PRL. We examined the time course of changes in not visible. With explicit training using a gaze marker, the
saccade latency, the number of saccades after each object fixation stability at the PRLs and the precision of the targeting
movement, and fixation duration during the object-following saccades became as good as those with normal foveal vision.
component of the task. We found a significant decrease in These findings imply a flexible and adaptive oculomotor
both saccade latency (42% reduction, Figure 4A) and the num- system. Akin to learning a motor skill such as riding a bicycle,
ber of saccades (60% reduction, Figure 4B). The saccade la- the system can rapidly develop a new motor plan even
tency and number of saccades at the end of explicit training with limited and sporadic exposure. Moreover, the motor plan
became comparable to those of controls (saccade latency: improves with use and is retained without continuous practice.
Rapid Adaptation of Saccades without Fovea
1665

A S2 S3 S5 B
15°
50

Variance (BCEA) of fixation (in deg2 )


10°
Average

First block of 0°
40 Controls
free exploration

30

Last block of 20
free exploration
Retention
10

Explicit training 0
Invisible
Free Exploration Explicit Training Scotoma
-10
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
Block Number
Retention

Controls

Invisible scotoma
0.001 0.05 0.1
Probability Density

Figure 2. Development and Retention of Preferred Retinal Locus for Fixation


(A) Probability density maps of the retinal positions of a target object at fixation are shown for three of the six subjects. (Refer to Table S1 for the specific
number of blocks each subject performed in the different stages of the experiment.) From the top to bottom, the rows represent densities estimated from the
first and last blocks of the free exploration period, density estimated from the last block of the explicit training period, density estimated from all five to seven
blocks of the retention assessment, and density estimated from all five blocks when the scotoma was made invisible. The density of the fovea viewing
control experiment, averaged across all control subjects, is also shown. Data from the object following components with blank background and cluttered
background were combined. Each polar plot represents the visual field. The gray patch depicts the central scotoma. The red dot marks the location of peak
density, which we took as the estimated location of the fixational PRL. All six subjects exhibited a rapid and spontaneous emergence of fixational PRLs near
the border of the scotoma within 3 hr (eight to nine blocks). All subjects were able to further refine the precision of their fixational PRLs during a subsequent
explicit training period that lasted at least 15 hr (40 blocks). Not only was the PRL retained after being established, but the same PRL was also used when the
scotoma was made invisible.
(B) Variance of the fixational PRL as a function of block number. Variance was defined as the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) that encompassed 68% of
fixations around the mean. The blue solid line indicates the average BCEA across all five subjects who spontaneously developed a single PRL (one subject,
shown here as S5, developed two PRLs); the shaded blue area indicates 61 SD of the BCEA, showing intersubject variability. The gray rectangular box rep-
resents the distribution of BCEA from the control subjects in the fovea viewing condition (61 SD). A gap of five blocks was inserted on the abscissa between
free exploration and explicit training to denote a break of 1 to 2 days from the experiment between these two stages. The BCEA of the fixation PRLs from the
five subjects with a single PRL decreased rapidly within a short period of time without explicit training. Explicit training reduced the BCEA to the range of
foveal fixation. It also eliminated one of the two PRLs of S5, as demonstrated in the retention test.

Our findings suggest that the oculomotor system prefers closest to the target in order to minimize saccade amplitude
developing a motor plan that is simple over one that may be and, thus, improve accuracy. Alternatively, it could choose
optimal with respect to saccade amplitude and accuracy. a point on the peripheral retina that avoids occluding the infor-
With intact central vision, the fovea has the highest acuity mative parts of the target with the scotoma (i.e., for reading
and, therefore, is a unique point in the visual field. The oculo- English text, placing the current word below the scotoma
motor system is presented with the simple goal of bringing could be advantageous). Despite these possibilities for an
the high-resolution fovea to the target of interest. Losing on-demand variable fixation strategy that is perhaps more
central vision eliminates this unique point. It is conceivable efficient in terms of accuracy or information gain, the oculomo-
that there exists a unique ‘‘best’’ point in the spared peripheral tor system opts for a strategy that is, from a control perspec-
retina for optimal form vision, and the location of this point may tive, exceedingly simple: using a single point in the periphery
be partly determined by oculomotor factors, such as fixational as the preferred locus for fixation. This amounts to adding a
drift, that contribute to spatiotemporal sensitivity [9, 10]. How- constant vector offset to the existing oculomotor control
ever, the difference in functional acuity between the best system.
peripheral point and the next best is likely to be small and Our observations are qualitatively different from what is
certainly smaller than the difference between normal fovea generally known as saccadic adaptation [12]. Saccadic adap-
and parafovea. Indeed, preliminary data from a recent study tation is a continuous recalibration process that minimizes
showed that local acuity did not predict the location of PRL the perceived saccade error; it is thought to be necessary for
in macular degeneration (MD) patients [11]. Hence, given a adapting to an ever-changing oculomotor plan due to growth,
target of interest, there are most likely multiple points (or aging, and diseases. Saccadic adaptation, although rapid
contours) on the peripheral retina that are equally adequate. in humans (in the order of a couple hundred saccades), is
As such, the oculomotor system could choose a point that is magnitude and direction specific [13–15]. Establishing a PRL
Current Biology Vol 23 No 17
1666

A S2 S3 S5 B
15° 80
Average

first saccade landing site (in deg2 )


10°
5° 70
First block of 0° Controls
free exploration 60

Variance (BCEA) of
50

40
Last block of
free exploration 30 Retention

20

10
Explicit training
0 Invisible
Free Exploration Explicit Training Scotoma
-10
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
Block Number
Retention

Controls

Invisible scotoma
0.001 0.05 0.1
Probability Density

Figure 3. Development and Retention of Rereferenced Saccades Measured by the First Saccade Landing Site
(A) Probability density maps of the retinal position of a target object at the completion of the first saccade after target movement (same format as in
Figure 2A).
(B) Variance (BCEA) of the first saccade landing site as a function of block number (same format as in Figure 2B). The first saccade landing site after each
target movement was near the fixational PRL and distant from the fovea. Similar to the fixational PRL, the variance of the first saccade landing sites
decreased rapidly within a short period of time without explicit training, demonstrating a spontaneous shift in oculomotor reference from the fovea to
the PRL. With explicit training, the BCEA was further reduced to the normal range of intact foveal vision.

with saccadic adaptation would require one to adapt to a large, from 1 to 6 months, depending on the age of MD onset [5].
if not infinite, number of magnitudes and directions. More The difference in the time courses between simulated and
importantly, saccadic adaption affects the current state of real scotoma is even more remarkable when considering the
the oculomotor controller. It takes time to adapt and to recover fact that the simulated scotoma in our experiment was
from adaptation. This contradicts the observations that (1) our imposed for only about 1 hr per day. It is quite plausible that
subjects’ normal foveation behavior was not affected by per- the gradual deterioration of vision in MD patients interferes
forming the task with their PRL and that (2) subjects retained with the development of adaptive oculomotor behavior. Age
their PRL between the daily experimental sessions and after may also be a contributing factor to the slow development.
weeks of not performing the task. Hence, the development Patients have better fixation stability if their central vision
of PRL that we observed is unlikely to be due to saccadic loss occurred at an early age [20]. In a recent study, we found
adaptation. that older adults were slower and used excessive eye move-
Our results with simulated central scotoma echo some of the ments during a visual search task with simulated central sco-
clinical findings. A majority of MD patients with bilateral central toma [26], although the same study did not examine PRL
vision loss utilize a single PRL for viewing or fixating [16–18], development. Another likely cause for the slow development
whereas some patients develop two or three task-dependent is that real scotomas are often invisible or with unclear bound-
PRLs [19]. A portion of these patients make saccades that ary [27–29]. A visible scotoma can provide accurate positional
are rereferenced to their fixational PRLs [20]. Patients’ visual feedback and, thus, may speed up PRL development. Given
performance is closely correlated with the establishment of a our results of a robust development of PRL and oculomotor
stable PRL [17, 21, 22], further suggesting that variable fixation rereferencing with a visible scotoma and the persistence of a
is not an option used by the oculomotor system. However, un- PRL even when the scotoma was rendered invisible, it might
like MD patients, who apparently take months to develop PRLs be possible to speed up the process of PRL development in
and rereference the oculomotor system to the PRLs [5], we patients by superimposing a simulated scotoma on the real
found that PRL development was rapid with a (visible) simu- scotoma, thereby creating a visible border.
lated central scotoma. Our results are consistent with a report The remarkable ability of the oculomotor system to rapidly
of a similarly rapid development in the case of pursuit eye adjust to an occluded central vision reveals a basic guiding
movements with a sinusoidally moving target [23] and qualita- principle of oculomotor control. Given the time pressure
tive accounts of the fast emergence of visual-field preference underlying saccadic eye movements, the system strives to
for target identification [24, 25]. In contrast, although the natu- maintain simplicity—a new motor plan is formed by adding a
ral development of PRLs may occur in some patients within constant vector offset to the existing and well-practiced motor
weeks of vision loss, saccade rereferencing to PRLs in macu- plan. Our finding opens up the rich possibility of exploring
lar patients is a slow process that has a median time ranging plasticity in the cortical and subcortical structures associated
Rapid Adaptation of Saccades without Fovea
1667

A Saccade latency B Number of saccades C Fixation duration


Average
450 6
Saccade Latency (ms)

Fixation Duration (ms)


Controls

Number of Saccades
800
5
Invisible
350 Scotoma
4 600
Invisible Invisible
250 3 Scotoma Scotoma
400
2
150 200
1
Free Free Free
Explicit Training Retention Explicit Training Retention Explicit Training Retention
50 Exploration 0 Exploration 0 Exploration
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
Block Number Block Number Block Number

Figure 4. Changes in Eye Movement Characteristics during Object Following


(A) Median saccade latency (the time taken to make the first saccade upon object movement) as a function of block number (same format as in Figure 2B).
(B) Mean number of saccades per object movement (i.e., the number of corrective saccades plus one) as a function of block number.
(C) Mean duration of fixation between object movement as a function of block number. Error bars represent 61 SD.

with oculomotor control and in the visual areas that are retino- background (22 cd/m2). The average delay between actual eye movement
topically associated with the newly formed PRL. and screen update was estimated to be approximately 10 ms (range from
2 ms to 15 ms on the basis of the eye data to frame time latencies that we
measured on the stimulus computer and on the manufacturer’s data on min-
Experimental Procedures imum eye data latency). The gaze position error (i.e., the difference between
target position and computed gaze position) was estimated during the nine-
Participants point validation process. A chin-and-forehead rest was used throughout the
We recruited 12 normally sighted subjects from the University of Southern experiment in order to minimize head movements and trial-to-trial variability
California campus (ages ranging from 19 to 30 years, one female). Six in the estimate of gaze position. The emergence of an eccentric retinal locus
subjects participated in the simulated scotoma experiments. The other six with rereferenced saccades observed in the free exploration period of the
subjects participated in the control experiment. They all had normal or cor- experiment, in contrast to the normal foveating behavior observed in the
rected-to-normal vision without known cognitive or neurological impair- control experiment, served as confirmation that the slight amount of spatio-
ments. The mean acuity (assessed with a lighthouse distance acuity chart) temporal imprecision in the eye-tracking system was not sufficient to inter-
was 20.1 logMar (Snellen 20/15). Subjects received monetary compensa- fere with the effectiveness of the simulated central scotoma.
tion for their participation. The experimental protocols were approved by
the internal review board of the University of Southern California, and written Procedure
informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to the experiment. Subjects performed a demanding visual task in uninterrupted blocks of 30
trials each. On average, subjects took 30 hr to complete the entire experi-
Stimuli and Apparatus ment (excluding breaks and calibration time). This was spread into several
All stimuli were high-contrast 24 bit RGB color images of indoor scenes and sessions spanning two months. Each block took approximately 25 min to
objects. Images of 49 indoor scenes (1,024 3 768 pixels) were selected complete. Subjects performed the task in a dimly lit room while they were
from an image database [30]. Images of 140 objects (average diagonal size seated in a comfortable position with chin and forehead supports. Each
of 2.4 with a range of 2 to 2.7 ) were selected from a commercially available block started with the calibration and/or validation sequence described
set of photographs of real objects at www.photos.com (now at www. earlier (w5 min) followed by drift correction. The trial started with an auditory
thinkstockphotos.com). The stimuli were generated and controlled with beep immediately after drift correction. For each trial, subjects had to com-
MATLAB (version 7.9) and Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Windows 7 plete four task phases: object following, gaze centering, visual search, and a
OS) [31, 32] running on a Dell PC. The display was a 19’’ CRT monitor (refresh second object following (Figure 1A).
rate, 85 Hz; resolution, 1,024 3 768). The stimuli were presented at a viewing Object Following
distance of 57 cm. The displayed scenes subtended visual angles of 39 3 29 . During object following, subjects followed a target object as it was randomly
repositioned four times against a gray background (Figure 1A, i). The center
Eye Movement Recoding and Simulated Scotoma of the object was uniformly distributed within the central 31 3 22 region of
Subjects’ eye movements were monitored with an infrared video-based eye the display. Each move was initiated only when the onscreen position of the
tracker sampled at 2,000 Hz (EyeLink 1000 Tower Mount monocular subjects’ scotoma did not occlude the target object for at least 1.5 s. This
eyetracker, SR Research) with a maximum spatial resolution of 0.02 . A was done to encourage eccentric fixation. Subjects were told to examine
nine-point calibration and/or validation sequence was performed at the the target object as accurately as possible, given that it was the search
beginning of every block, and drift correction was made at the beginning target for the upcoming visual search phase. Otherwise, subjects did not
of every trial. Calibration and/or validation were repeated until the validation receive any explicit instructions on how to use their gaze.
error was smaller than 1 on average. The average gaze error was 0.5 , Gaze Centering
ranging from 0.1 to 1 . A gaze-contingent visual display was used to simu- During gaze centering, subjects centered their gaze in the middle of the
late central visual field loss in normally sighted subjects. This paradigm, screen so that their scotoma was placed inside a black rectangular box
referred to as ‘‘artificial scotoma’’ [33], has been used in previous studies for 1.5 s (Figure 1A, ii). This was performed right before the onset of visual
to investigate various issues related to central field loss [23, 34, 35]. The search in order to minimize any positional bias.
real-time gaze position was sent to the display computer through a high- Visual Search
speed Ethernet link. The continuous gaze information was used to draw a During visual search, subjects searched for the target object (the same
scotoma on the display screen at a refresh and update rate of 85 Hz. The object as in phase 1) amidst a cluttered background with an array of
size and shape of the scotoma were derived from the visual field measure- nontarget distracters (Figure 1A, iii). Both the target and nontarget dis-
ment obtained from a patient with age-related MD [36]. The scotoma, as tracters were superimposed on the scene rather than being part of the
shown in Figure 1A, was a nearly circular disc, subtended about 10 of visual scene. This was done in order to minimize any contextual effects on search
angle, and was rendered as a uniform gray patch (luminance 18 cd/m2) on performance. Subjects were given an unlimited amount of time to perform
the screen. We also ran an invisible scotoma condition with a slightly lighter the search, after which they indicated the presence or absence of the search
gray patch, the luminance of which was matched to that of the gray target by a key press (‘‘p’’ for presence and ‘‘a’’ for absence). The probability
Current Biology Vol 23 No 17
1668

A Search accuracy B Search time C Number of saccades


100 5 30
Average

Number of Saccades
Invisible

Search Time (sec)


4 Scotoma 25 Controls
Accuracy (%)

90
20 Invisible
3 Scotoma
80 15
Invisible
Scotoma
10
70 2
5
Free Free Free
Explicit Training Retention Explicit Training Retention Explicit Training Retention
Exploration Exploration Exploration
60 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
Block Number Block Number Block Number

Figure 5. Changes in Visual Search Performance


(A) Visual search accuracy (percent correct) as a function of block number (same format as in Figure 2B). The mean accuracy level (89% 6 2.1% SD) was
similar to that of the control group and was constant through the course of the experiment.
(B) Time taken to complete a search trial (either target present or target absent) as a function of block number.
(C) Number of saccades required for the completion of a search trial. Only correct trials were included for (B) and (C). Error bars represent 61 SD.

of the target being present was 0.5. Subjects were instructed to perform the (SD in gaze position did not exceed 0.5 in any direction) and ended with
search task as quickly and as accurately as possible. An auditory feedback one of the following conditions: (1) a period of missing data (a blink)
was provided for a correct response. exceeding 500 ms, (2) the start of a saccade, or (3) the start of a reliable drift
Second Object Following that exceeded 1 .
A second object following phase was conducted similarly to the first object Fixation density maps were derived from the retinal positions of the target
following phase, except that the target was a randomly drawn object that objects during periods of fixation via kernel density estimation with a bivar-
appeared on a cluttered background (Figure 1A, iv). Oculomotor perfor- iate Gaussian kernel [41]. The PRL was defined as the peak of the density.
mance (fixational PRL and saccadic rereference) was assessed during the Fixation stability has traditionally been quantified by calculating the area
two object following phases (phases 1 and 4) under the assumption that of the ellipse that encompasses a given proportion of eye positions during
the target object was the intended target of gaze. fixation [42]. This area is termed the BCEA [43]. A more stable fixation cor-
Subjects performed at least two or three blocks per day and completed responds to a smaller BCEA. In the current study, the variance of fixation
9 to 30 blocks of the task during free exploration. Subjects then received was defined as the BCEA that encompassed 68% of fixations around the
explicit training (42 to 72 blocks) for refining the emerged PRL. This was mean [42] and was used as an indicator of fixation stability. BCEAs were
accomplished by having the subjects perform the same task as in the free calculated from the density maps. (The fixational BECAs measured from
exploration period except that their PRLs were marked with a small 0.7 the current experiment are most likely to be overestimations because we in-
white cross (Figure 1B), and subjects were instructed to use the cross as structed our subjects to simply ‘‘follow and examine’’ an object, 2 to 2.7 in
the point of gaze. This effectively encouraged the subjects to rely on the size without specifying where on the object a subject must fixate.) The den-
emerged PRLs for the task and possibly improve fixation accuracy with sity maps for the first saccade landing site were obtained in a similar manner
the peripheral locus. For normal foveal vision, a significant decrease in the from the retinal positions of the target objects at the end point of the first
accuracy of eye positions has been reported in the absence of a fixation saccade after object movement.
marker [37]. Upon the completion of explicit training, subjects took a break
for at least 1 week before being recalled for another round of the task for five Supplemental Information
to seven blocks (retention assessment). After the retention assessment,
subjects performed five additional blocks of the object following and visual Supplemental Information contains two figures and one table and can be
search tasks against a gray background, a process that was identical to the found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.056.
first object following phase and the third visual search phase of the free
exploration period of the experiment but with one exception: the luminance
Acknowledgments
of the simulated scotoma was matched to that of the background (Figure 1C).
As a result, the simulated central scotoma was not visible to the subjects
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grants
(invisible scotoma). Thus, the invisible scotoma experiment consisted of
R01EY017707 and R01EY016093. The authors would like to thank A. Disney,
two phases: object following (Figure 1C, i) and visual search (Figure 1C, ii).
G. Legge, G. Timberlake, and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful
A separate group of six subjects participated in a control experiment (a total
comments and discussion of the manuscript and J.L. Gonzales and E. Trinh
of five blocks) identical to free exploration but without the simulated sco-
for their help with subject recruitment and data collection.
toma (control experiment). The specific numbers of blocks each subject per-
formed in the different stages of the experiment are listed in Table S1.
Received: May 25, 2013
Data Analysis Revised: June 14, 2013
Gaze position data were first preprocessed by an edge-preserving median Accepted: June 24, 2013
filter with a 200 ms window in order to remove transient noise. Then, a modi- Published: August 15, 2013
fied version of a standard parsing algorithm [38, 39] was applied to the pre-
processed data in order to robustly classify saccades and fixations while References
excluding microsaccades [40]. We defined a period of eye movement as a
saccade if the following conditions were met: (1) the eye velocity exceeded 1. Slater, A.M., and Findlay, J.M. (1972). The measurement of fixation
20 per s during the entire period, (2) the peak velocity during this period was position in the newborn baby. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 14, 349–364.
in the top 25th percentile of the recording phase, (3) the beginning and end of 2. Aslin, R.N., and Salapatek, P. (1975). Saccadic localization of visual tar-
this period had a velocity of at least 15% of the peak velocity, and (4) the gets by the very young human infant. Percept. Psychophys. 17, 293–302.
amplitude of the eye movement during this period exceeded 0.9 . Periods 3. Cummings, R.W., Whittaker, S.G., Watson, G.R., and Budd, J.M. (1985).
that were not saccades were candidates for fixations. A candidate period Scanning characters and reading with a central scotoma. Am. J. Optom.
was classified as a fixation if it started with at least 50 ms of stable gaze Physiol. Opt. 62, 833–843.
Rapid Adaptation of Saccades without Fovea
1669

4. Schuchard, R.A., and Fletcher, D.C. (1994). Preferred retinal locus: a 30. Luo, G., Satgunam, P., and Peli, E. (2012). Visual search performance of
review with applications in low vision rehabilitation. Ophthalmol. Clin. patients with vision impairment: effect of JPEG image enhancement.
North Am. 7, 243–256. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 32, 421–428.
5. Crossland, M.D., Culham, L.E., Kabanarou, S.A., and Rubin, G.S. (2005). 31. Brainard, D.H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10,
Preferred retinal locus development in patients with macular disease. 433–436.
Ophthalmology 112, 1579–1585. 32. Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics:
6. Crossland, M.D., Engel, S.A., and Legge, G.E. (2011). The preferred transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437–442.
retinal locus in macular disease: toward a consensus definition. 33. Aguilar, C., and Castet, E. (2011). Gaze-contingent simulation of retinop-
Retina 31, 2109–2114. athy: some potential pitfalls and remedies. Vision Res. 51, 997–1012.
7. Heinen, S.J., and Skavenski, A.A. (1992). Adaptation of saccades 34. Fine, E.M., and Rubin, G.S. (1999). Effects of cataract and scotoma on
and fixation to bilateral foveal lesions in adult monkey. Vision Res. 32, visual acuity: a simulation study. Optom. Vis. Sci. 76, 468–473.
365–373. 35. Bernard, J.B., Scherlen, A.C., and Castet, E. (2007). Page mode reading
with simulated scotomas: a modest effect of interline spacing on
8. Sansbury, R.V., Skavenski, A.A., Haddad, G.M., and Steinman, R.M.
reading speed. Vision Res. 47, 3447–3459.
(1973). Normal fixation of eccentric targets. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 63,
36. Chung, S.T.L. (2011). Improving reading speed for people with central
612–614.
vision loss through perceptual learning. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci.
9. Kuang, X., Poletti, M., Victor, J.D., and Rucci, M. (2012). Temporal
52, 1164–1170.
encoding of spatial information during active visual fixation. Curr. Biol.
37. Cherici, C., Kuang, X., Poletti, M., and Rucci, M. (2012). Precision of
22, 510–514.
sustained fixation in trained and untrained observers. J. Vis. 12, 1–16.
10. Rucci, M., Iovin, R., Poletti, M., and Santini, F. (2007). Miniature eye 38. Fischer, B., Biscaldi, M., and Otto, P. (1993). Saccadic eye movements
movements enhance fine spatial detail. Nature 447, 851–854. of dyslexic adult subjects. Neuropsychologia 31, 887–906.
11. Chung, S., and Bernard, J.-B. (2013). Does the location of the PRL corre- 39. Gitelman, D.R. (2002). ILAB: a program for postexperimental eye move-
spond to the retinal location with the best acuity? Invest. Ophthalmol. ment analysis. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 34, 605–612.
Vis. Sci. 54, 2183. 40. Engbert, R., and Mergenthaler, K. (2006). Microsaccades are triggered
12. Hopp, J.J., and Fuchs, A.F. (2004). The characteristics and neuronal by low retinal image slip. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 7192–7197.
substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity. Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 41. Botev, Z.I., Grotowski, J.F., and Kroese, D.P. (2010). Kernel density
27–53. estimation via diffusion. Ann. Stat. 38, 2916–2957.
13. Albano, J.E. (1996). Adaptive changes in saccade amplitude: oculocen- 42. Crossland, M.D., Sims, M., Galbraith, R.F., and Rubin, G.S. (2004).
tric or orbitocentric mapping? Vision Res. 36, 2087–2098. Evaluation of a new quantitative technique to assess the number
14. Abel, L.A., Schmidt, D., Dell’Osso, L.F., and Daroff, R.B. (1978). and extent of preferred retinal loci in macular disease. Vision Res. 44,
Saccadic system plasticity in humans. Ann. Neurol. 4, 313–318. 1537–1546.
15. Moidell, B.G., and Bedell, H.E. (1988). Changes in oculocentric visual 43. Steinman, R.M. (1965). Effect of target size, luminance, and color on
direction induced by the recalibration of saccades. Vision Res. 28, monocular fixation. J Opt Soc Am. 55, 1158–1165.
329–336.
16. Von Noorden, G.K., and MacKensen, G. (1962). Phenomenology of
eccentric fixation. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 53, 642–660.
17. Whittaker, S.G., Budd, J., and Cummings, R.W. (1988). Eccentric fixa-
tion with macular scotoma. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 29, 268–278.
18. Fletcher, D.C., and Schuchard, R.A. (1997). Preferred retinal loci
relationship to macular scotomas in a low-vision population.
Ophthalmology 104, 632–638.
19. Timberlake, G.T., Mainster, M.A., Peli, E., Augliere, R.A., Essock, E.A.,
and Arend, L.E. (1986). Reading with a macular scotoma. I. Retinal
location of scotoma and fixation area. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 27,
1137–1147.
20. White, J.M., and Bedell, H.E. (1990). The oculomotor reference in
humans with bilateral macular disease. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci.
31, 1149–1161.
21. Falkenberg, H.K., Rubin, G.S., and Bex, P.J. (2007). Acuity, crowding,
reading and fixation stability. Vision Res. 47, 126–135.
22. Tarita-Nistor, L., González, E.G., Markowitz, S.N., and Steinbach, M.J.
(2009). Plasticity of fixation in patients with central vision loss. Vis.
Neurosci. 26, 487–494.
23. Pidcoe, P.E., and Wetzel, P.A. (2006). Oculomotor tracking strategy
in normal subjects with and without simulated scotoma. Invest.
Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 47, 169–178.
24. Ness, J.W., Zwick, H., and Molchany, J.W. (1996). Preferred retinal loca-
tion induced by macular occlusion in a target recognition task. Proc.
SPIE 2674, 131–135.
25. Varsori, M., Perez-Fornos, A., Safran, A.B., and Whatham, A.R. (2004).
Development of a viewing strategy during adaptation to an artificial
central scotoma. Vision Res. 44, 2691–2705.
26. Kwon, M., Ramachandra, C., Satgunam, P., Mel, B.W., Peli, E., and Tjan,
B.S. (2012). Contour enhancement benefits older adults with simulated
central field loss. Optom. Vis. Sci. 89, 1374–1384.
27. Schuchard, R.A. (1993). Validity and interpretation of Amsler grid
reports. Arch. Ophthalmol. 111, 776–780.
28. Schuchard, R.A. (2005). Preferred retinal loci and macular scotoma
characteristics in patients with age-related macular degeneration.
Can. J. Ophthalmol. 40, 303–312.
29. Fletcher, D.C., Schuchard, R.A., and Renninger, L.W. (2012). Patient
awareness of binocular central scotoma in age-related macular degen-
eration. Optom. Vis. Sci. 89, 1395–1398.

También podría gustarte