Está en la página 1de 3

Consciousness

Essay

Consciousness Isn’t About the Mind, It’s


About the Body
Thinking and Feeling Are the Products of the Brain's
Physical Architecture

Illustration from The Book of Life: The Spiritual and Physical Constitution of Man, by Dr
Alesha Sivartha, published in 1898. Image courtesy of the California Digital Library.

By Michael S. Gazzaniga | April 11, 2018

Many students of the mind have observed that consciousness—as a word or as a concept—is
a placeholder, a suitcase word for multiple processes in our brains. Those processes are
systems in our brains that are made of physical matter; they chug away, following physical
laws, to generate our felt state, our subjective sense of life. And somehow, in the entirety of
their collective actions, we are aware, we feel, we love, we sit on the porch in the evening and
enjoy the sunset.

Some say the gap—between nerve cells and life, the brain and the mind, objective reality and
the subjective—will never be understood, because such understanding is beyond our human
capacity. But I think it is possible to answer the question of how the brain becomes the mind.
We just need to change our thinking.

Get More Zócalo


Ideas journalism with a head and heart.

You may opt out or contact us anytime.

Right now, we are stuck—at a place and time in human history—where scientists and other
serious people accept that the physical brain, following all the laws of mother nature, gins up
this wonderful thing called consciousness and its particular brand of sweetness in humans.
Scientists also realize the brain has evolved over eons to have a gazillion parts, intricately
organized to manage certain capacities of life, from seeking food, drink, and shelter, to
eating, drinking, and reproducing, which we share with other animals. In addition, humanity
has more elaborate thinking and talking capacities. Each process by itself—seeking, eating,
reproducing—has been dubbed instinct, because it is in some ways unlearned. And as the
great American philosopher and psychologist William James famously observed, humans
have more instincts than any other animal.

But none of this actually answers the question of brain-to-mind. In our era, we have the work
of the cognitive psychologist Steven Pinker, who famously showed in the 1990s that
language was not the mere product of our brain or our human societies. Language was an
instinct, literally a piece of our brains, and thus part of the very human machine itself.

It is time to think of consciousness as another instinct. We know that, literally, consciousness


comes from physical places in our brain. Think about it: Consciousness comes with each
capacity we have as humans and it is the state we feel about the capacities we possess. When
we lose a capacity, we also lose its felt state. Consciousness is not an added-on capacity that
enlivens another one of our capacities.

What we call consciousness arises through the moment-to-moment expression of brain


modules with hyper-specific functions that are scattered through the brain.

To my way of thinking, each of our sub-systems, modules, or capacities is real and alive
because of the particular way that life works. And here I appeal and defer to the lifelong work
of an extraordinary scientist little known to neuroscientists and psychologists, Howard Pattee.
While Pattee was trained as a physicist at Stanford, he has spent his academic life at State
University of New York, Binghamton as a theoretical biologist. Pattee proposes a different
way to think about that pesky gap between neurons and mind. And to do it, he takes us all the
way back to a prior question: How does life arise from non-life? Hold your hat. The next bit
requires more thinking.

Pattee’s original insight began when thinking about the gap between non-living stuff and
living stuff. How did one become the other? Living stuff replicates and evolves over time. To
replicate, this living stuff has to build a copy of itself. That requires instructions on how to
build a copy and construction of the copy. It also requires the newly minted stuff to have its
own copy of instructions, so those, too, have to be copied.
But life has gone a step beyond simple replication; it is evolvable. And the mathematician
and physicist John von Newmann understood that to evolve, the process has to introduce
variation so that natural selection can begin its work, and variation had to come from a code,
an abstract, reliable representation of the instructions.

Right there is where Pattee stumbles upon a brilliant truth. While the substrate of the code is a
physical structure, the code itself is made up of abstract symbols that have been selected for
their reliability (and can change if a more reliable one pops up). Symbols are subjective, and
as such, follow no physical laws; they follow rules. Thus, the gap between non-life and life is
bridged by an abstract but physical code, a substance. There is no spook or magic in the
system; it is simply that every code requires a complementarity between its two aspects: the
physical and the symbolic.

While the original substance that seeded life is unknown, we now think of it as DNA. And
DNA is a perfect example of the phenomenon that Pattee describes: DNA follows the laws of
physics and generates proteins by following the coded recipe—and DNA exists physically.
Yet the code for the recipe, though conserved and stable, is abstract and not subject to
physical laws.

Likewise, the gap between neurons and mind is bridged by a symbolic neural code and that
code also requires a complementarity between its two aspects: the physical and the symbolic.
Subjective consciousness is not a “thing,” though physical neurons produce it. It is the result
of a process embedded in an architecture, just as a democracy is not a thing but the result of a
process.

What we call consciousness arises through the moment-to-moment expression of brain


modules with hyper-specific functions that are scattered through the brain. As those modules
each come forward through time, they create the “flow” of consciousness, which is both a
process and a sensation.

It is a rich story and the details are absorbing and mind-bending. It’s a thrilling journey to
look back on the history of the human struggle with the question of consciousness and see the
modern battle lines about the brain-versus-mind. But the act of observing that consciousness
is an instinct—that it comes with life—is easy. Appreciating that consciousness is just that is
the hard part.

Michael S. Gazzaniga is a professor of psychology at the University of California, Santa


Barbara, where he heads the new SAGE Center for the Study of the Mind. His new book is
The Consciousness Instinct: Unraveling the Mystery of How the Brain Makes the Mind.
(Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2018).
Primary Editor: Joe Mathews | Secondary Editor: Lisa Margonelli

También podría gustarte