Está en la página 1de 7

School Improvement Research Series

Close-Up #5

Classroom Questioning

r Introduction
Kathleen Cotton

Articles on the subject of classroom question-


The present review focuses on the relationship
between teachers' classroom questioning
behaviors and a variety of student outcomes,
ing often begin by invoking Socrates. Re- including achievement, retention, and level of
searchers and other writers concerned with student participation. This means that
questioning techniques seem to want to certain other subtopics within the general
remind us that questioning has a long and area of questioning are excluded from the
venerable history as an educational strategy. present analysis. It does not deal,
And indeed, the Socratic method of using for example, with the effects of textual ques-
questions and answers to challenge assump- tions or test questions, and it is only inciden-
tions, expose contradictions, and lead to new tally concerned with methods used to impart
knowledge and wisdom is an undeniably pow- study skills, including questioning strategies,
erful teaching approach. to students.

In aduition to its long history and demon- What are the purposes of teachers' classroom
strated effectiveness, questioning is also of questions? A variety of purposes emerge from
interest to researchers and practitioners be- analysis of the literature, including:
cause of its widespread use as a contemporary
teaching technique. Research indicates that To develop interest and motivate students
questioning is second only to lecturing in to become actively involved in lessons
popularity as a teaching method and that
classroom teachers spend anywhere from To evaluate students' preparation and
thirty-five to fifty percent of their instruc- check on homework or seatwork comple-
tional time conducting questioning sessions. tion

Ta develop critical thinking skills and in-


Definition quiring attitudes

A question is any sentence which has an To review and summarize previous lessons
interrogative form or function. In classroom
settings, teacher questions are defined as To nurture insights by exposing new rela-
instructional cues or stimuli that convey to tionships
students the content elements to oe learned
and directions for what they are to do and how To assess achievement of instructional
they are to do it. goals and objectives

Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory


101 S.W. Main Street, Suite 500
Portland, Oregon 97204
Telephone (503) 275-9500

4
School Improvement Program lilt
I
To stimulate students to pursue knowl- The entire K-12 range - 14
edge on their own
Not specified - 1
These purposes are generally pursued in the
context of classroom recitation, def'oed as a The research is concerned with a variety of
series of teacher questions, each eliciting a treatments. By far the largest number of
student response and sometimes a teacher documentstwenty-sixare concerned with
reaction to that response. Within these the relative effects on student learning pro-
recitations, students follow a series cr steps duced by questions at higher and lower
(consciously or unconsciously) in order to cognitive levels (discussed below). The subject
produce responses to the questions posed. of eight of the documents is the relationship
These steps include: between teacher wait-time and learning
outcomes (also discussed in a later section).
Attending to the question Other treatments include:

Deciphering the meaning of the question Manipulating the placement and timing of
questions during lessons - 2
Generating a covert response (i.e., formu-
lating a response in one's mind) Using probing, redirection and reinforce-
ment strategies - 3
Generating an overt response; and often
Training students in responding to higher
Revising the response (based on teacher cognitive questions, making inferences,
probing or other feedback) etc. - 2

Training teachers in questioning strate-


The Research on Classroom gies - 3
Questioning
The variables are sometimes investigated
CHARACTERTISTICS alone and sometimes in combination with
OF THE RESEARCH each other or with other variables unrelated
to classroom questioning.
Classroom questioning is an extensively
researched topic. The high incidence of The student outcome areas of concern in the
questioning as a teaching strategy, and its research include:
consequent potential for influencing student
learning, have led many investigators to General achievement - 18
examine relationships between questioning
methods and student achievement and behav- . Reading achievement (usually comprehen-
ior. sion) - 5

The findings reported in this summary are . Social studies achievement - 3


drawn from thirty-seven research documents.
Twenty-one of these are the reports of experi- . Science achievement - 3
mental or correlational studies, thirteen are
reviews, one reports the results of both a Mathematics achievement - 1
review and a study, and two are meta-analy-
ses. Retention, as measured by delayed tests
3
The student populations of concern in these
documents are: Level of student engagement/ participation
-9
Elementary (mostly intermediate) - 18
Cognitive level of responses produced by
Secondary - 4 students - 4

Student attitutes - 2
PAGE 2 CLOSE-UP #5

20
RESEARCH FINDINGS tial, and evaluative thinking? Some research-
ers have designed experiments which examine
General Findings the effects of questions framed at differing
levels of Bloom's Taxonomy of School Learn-
Some researchers have conducted general ing. These levels in ascending order of
investigations of the role of classroom ques- sophistication, are. (1) knowledge, (2) compre-
tioning and have drawn the following conclu- hension, (3) application, (4) analysis, (5)
sions: synthesis, and (6) evaluation. There are other
hierarchies, too, which are used as the basis
Instruction which includes posing ques- for structuring comparative studies.
tions during lessons is more effective in
producing achievement gains than in- The majority of researchers, h, sever, have
struction carried out without questioning conducted more simple comparisons: they
students. have looked at the relative effects on student
outcomes produced by what they call higher
Students perform better on test items pre- and lower cognitive questions.
viously asked as recitation questions than
on items they have not been exposed to Lower cognitive questions are those which
before. ask the student merely to recall verbatim or in
his/her own words material previously read or
Oral questions posed during classroom taught by the teacher. Lower cognitive
recitations are more effective in fostering questions are also referred to in the literature
learning than are written questions. as fact, closed, direct, recall, and knowledge
questions.
Questions which focus student attention
on salient elements in the lesson result in Higher cognitive questions are defined as
better comprehension than questions those which ask the student to mentally
which do not. manipulate bits of information previously
learned to create an answer or to support an
Placement and Timing of Questions answer with logically reasoned evidence.
Higher cognitive questions are also called
Asking questions frequently during class open-ended, interpretive, evaluative, inquiry,
discussions is positively related to inferential, and synthesis questions.
learning facts.
Research on the relationshi'- between the
Increasing the frequency of classroom cognitive level of teachers' questions and the
questions does not enhance the learning of achievement of their students has proved
more complex material. (Some research- frustrating to mcny in the field of education,
ers have found no relationship; others because it has not produced definitive results.
have found a negative relationship.) Quite a number of research studies have
found higher cognitive questions superior to
Posing questions before reading and study- lower ones, many have found the opposite, and
ing material is effective for students who still others have found no difference. The
are older, high ability, and/or known to be same is true of research examining the rela-
interested in the subject matter. tionship between the cognitive level of teach-
ers' questions and the cognitive level of
Very young children and poor readers tend students' responses. The conventional wisdom
to focus only on material that will help that says, "ask a higher level question, get a
them answer questions if these are posed higher level answer," does not seem to hold.
before the lesson is presented.
It is only when researchers look at the cogni-
Cognitive Level of Questions tive level of teachers' questions in relation to
the subject matter, the students, and the
Should we be asking questions which require teachers' intent that some meaningful conclu-
literal recall of text content and only very sions can be drawn from this body of research.
basic reasoning? Or ought we to be posing Findings include:
questions which call for speculative, inferen-
CLOSE-UP #5 PAGE 3
On the average, during classroom recita- Increases in the use of higher cognitive
tions, approximately 60 percent of the questions in recitations does not reduce
questions asked are lower cognitive student performance on lower cognitive
questions, 20 percent are higher cognitive questions on tests.
questions, and 20 percent are procedural.
For older students, increases in the use of
Higher cognitive questions are not cate- higher cognitive questions (to 50 percent
gorically better than lower cognitive or more) are positively related to increases
que.ztions in elicting higher level re- in:
sponses or in promoting learning gains.
(1) On-task behavior
Lower cognitive questions are more
effective than higher level questions with (2) Length of student responses
young (primary level) children, particu-
larly the disadvantaged. (3) The number of relevant contributions
volunteered by students
Lower cognitive questions are more
effective when the teacher's purpose is to (4) The number of student -to- student
impart factual knowledge and assist interactions
students in committing this knowledge to
memory. (5) Student use of complete sentences
In settings where a high incidence of lower (6) Speculative thinking on the part of
level questions is appropriate, greater students
frequency of questions is positively related
to student achievement. (7) Relevant questions posed by students
When predominantly lower level questions For older students, increases in the use of
are used, their level of difficulty should be higher cognitive questions (to 50 percent
such that most will elicit correct re- or more) are positively related to increased
spon ses. teacher expectations about children's
abilitiesparticularly the abilities of
In most classes above the primary grades, those students whom teachers have
a combination of higher and lower cogni- habitually regarded as slow or poor
tive questions is superior to exclusive use learners.
of one or the other.
Wait-Time
Students whom teachers perceive as slow
or poor learners are asked fewer higher Researchers on questioning strategies speak
cognitive questions than students per- of two kinds of wait-time: "wait-time 1" refers
ceived as more capable learners. to the amount of time the teacher allows to
elapse after he/she has posed a question and
Increasing the use of higher cognitive before a student begins to speak; and "wait-
questions (to considerably above the 20 time 2" refers to the amount of time a teacher
percent incidence noted in most classes) waits after a student has stopped speaking
produces superior learning gains for before saying anything The research has
students above the primary grades and focused more on wait-time 1 than wait-time 2,
particulary for secondary students. but the following findings apply to both.
Simply asking higher cognitive questions Because research has established a positive
does not necessarily lead students to relationship between the amount of instruc-
produce higher cognitive responses. tional content covered and student achieve-
ment, researchers and other educators have
Teaching students to draw inferences and recommended that teachers keep up brisk in-
giving them practice in doing so result in structional pacing. In this way, the reasoning
higher cognitive responses and greater goes, classes will cover more material, student
learning gains. interest will be maintained, and achievement
PAGE 4 CLOSE-UP #5
levels will be higher. As with the research on (9) Expansion of the variety of responses
the cognitive level of teachers' questions, this offered by students
wisdom turns out to have limited application.
Findings include: (10) Decreases in student interruptions
The average wait-time teachers allow (11) Increases in student-student interac-
after posing a question is one second or tion s
less.
(12) Increases in the number of questions
Students whom teachers perceive as slow posed by students
or poor learners are given less wait-time
than those teachers view as more capable. Increasing wait-time beyond three seconds
is positively related to the following
For lower cognitive questions, a wait-time teacher outcomes:
of three seconds is most positively related
to achievement, with less success result- (1) Increases in flexibility of teacher
ing from shorter or longer wait-times. responses, with teachers listening
more and engaging students in more
There seems to be no wait-time threshold discussions
for higher cognitive questions; students
seem to become more and more engaged (2) Increases in teacher expectations
and perform better and better the longer regarding students usually thought of
the teacher is willing to wait. as slow

Increasing wait-time beyond three seconds (3) Expansion of the variety of questions
is positively related to the following asked by teachers
student outcomes:
(4) Increases in the number of higher
(1) Improvements in the student cognitive questions asked by teach-
achievement ers.
(2) Improvements in student retention, Relationship Between Increasing the Use
as measured by delayed tests of Higher Cognitive Questions and In-
creasing Wait-Time
(3) Increases in the number of higher
cognitive responses generated by
students The list of benefits produced by increasing
higher cognitive questions and the list of
(4) Increases in the length of student benefits resulting from increased wait-time
responses are remarkably similar. In addition, research
has shown that the degree of improvement
(5) Increases in the number of unsolic- resulting from increases in both higher
ited responses cognitive questions and wait-time is greater
than an increase in either of these variables
(6) Decreases in students' failure to re- by itself. Indeed, those who have examined
spond the relationship between these factors tell us
that, in a sense, they "cause" one another.
(7) Increases in the amount and quality That is, the more complex mental operations
of evidence students offer to support required by higher cognitive questions call
their inferences forand are often found to producelonger
wait-times. And increases in wait-time seem
(8) Increases in contributions by stu- to result in teachers and students carrying out
dents who do not participate much recitations at higher cognitive levels.
when wait-time is under three
seconds

CLOSE-UP #5 PAGE 5
23
Redirection/Probing/Reinforcement The cognitive level of questions posed is
unrelated to students' attitudes toward
The research on questioning includes investi- the subject matter.
gations into the effects of redirecting ques-
tions when initial responses are unsatisfac- Those students who prefer lower cognitive
tory or incomplete, probing for more complete questions perform better in recitations
responses, and providing reinforcement of and on tests where lower cognitive ques-
responses. tions are posed.

These practices have been discussed previ- Those students whq prefer higher cogni-
ously in this School Improvement Research tive questions perform equally well with
Series. The 1988 "close-up" report entitled In- higher or lower cognitive questions in reci-
structional Reinforcement looks at the ways tations and on tests.
teachers respond to student answers and
ether student comments, and how the nature Teacher Training
of those responses relate to student outcomes.
Monitoring Student Learning in the Class- Research tells us that preservice teachers are
room, also published in 1988, discusses class- given inadequate training in developing
room questioning as one of many approaches questioning strategies and, indeed, that some
teachers can use to track student learning. receive no training at all. What happens
The findings emerging from these investiga- when teachers participate in training de-
tions are congruent with the general litera- signed to help them improve their questioning
ture on questioning, including: skills? Research indicates that:
When teachers get training
Redirection and probing (often researched Training teachers in asking higher cogni-
together) are positively related to achieve- tive questions is positively related to the
ment when they are explicitly focused, achievement of students above the pri-
e.g., on the clarity, accuracy, plausibility, mary grades.
etc. of student responses.
Training teachers in increased wait-time
Redirection and probing are unrelated to is positively related to student achieve-
achievement when they are vague or ment.
critical, e.g., "That's not right; try again";
"Where did you get an idea like that? I'm Training teachers to vary their question-
sure Suzanne has thought it through more ing behaviors and to use approaches other
carefully and can help us." than questioning during classroom discus-
sions (e.g., silence, making statements)
Acknowledging correct responses as such are positively related to student achieve-
is positively related to achievement. ment.

Praise is positively related to achievement


when it is used sparingly, is directly Guidelines for Classroom
related to the student's response, and is Questioning
sincere and credible.

Student Attitudes Based on the foregoing findings from the


research on classroom questioning, the follow-
Reports on most practices investigated by ing recommendations are offered:
educational researchers include findings about
the effects of the practice on student attitudes Incorporate questioning into classroom
as well as learning outcomes. Research on the teaching/learning practices.
relationship between questioning practices
and student attitudes is virtually nonexistent. Ask questions which focus on the salient
The only findings emerging from the litera- elements in the lesson, avoid questioning
ture reviewed in preparation for this report students about extraneous matters.
include:

PAGE 6 CLOSE-UP #5
When teaching students factual material, learn. Pearson suggests that teachers com-
keep up a brisk instructional pace, fre- plete all the steps in this process by way of
quently posing lower cognitive questions. demonstration, then gradually shift responsi-
bility for all but the first step to the students.
With older and higher ability students,
ask questions before (as well as after) 1. Ask the inference question.
material is read and studied.
2. Answer it
Question younger and lower ability
students only after material has been read 3. Find clues in the text to support the
and studied. inference.

Ask a majority of lower cognitive ques- 4. Tell how to get from the clues to the
tions when instructing younger and lower answer (i.e., give a line of reasoning).
ability students. Structure these ques-
tions so that most of them will elicit Better preservice training in the art of posing
correct responses. classroom questions, together with inservice
training to sharpen teachers' questioning
Ask a majority of higher cognitive ques- skills, have potential for increasing students'
tions when instructing older and higher classroom participation and achievement.
ability students. Increasing wait-time and the incidence of
higher cognitive questions, in particular, have
In settings where higher cognitive ques- considerable promise for improving the
tions are appropriate, teach students effectiveness of classroom instruction.
strategies for drawing inferences.

Keep wait-time to about three seconds Key References


when conducting recitations involving a
majority of lower cognitive questions. Adams, J. Refinements in Teaching Compre-
hension: Who Should Ask the Questions?
Increase wait-time beyond three seconds Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of
when asking higher cognitive questions. the Illinois Reading Council, Peoria, IL,
March 7-9, 1985. (ED 255 874).
Be particularly careful to allow generous
amounts of wait-time to students per- Investigates the effects of teaching ele-
ceived as lower ability. mentary children about the kinds of
questions used in educational settings and
Use redirection and probing as part of how to generate good comprehension
classroom questioning and keep these questions. Fourteen teachers and their
focused on salient elements of students' students in grades one through eight
responses. participated. Experimental students
outperformed controls on standardized
Avoid vague or critical responses to tests.
student answers during recitations.
Bozsik, B.E. A Study of Teacher Questioning
During recitations, use praise sparingly and Student Response Interaction During
and make certain it is sincere, credible, Pre-Story and Post-Story Portions of
and directly connected to the students' Reading Comprehension Lessons. Paper
responses. °resented at the Annual Meeting of the
American Educational Research Associa-
Detailed instructions for teaching students to tion, New York., March 19-23, 1982. (ED
draw inferences is outside the scope of this 215 294).
paper. However, the model offered by Pearson
(1985) does provide some basic steps which Compares the questioning behavior of four
can help students make connections between preservice and four inservice teachers in
what they know and what they are seeking to an inner city elementary school Also

CLOSE-UP #5 25 PAGE 7

También podría gustarte