Está en la página 1de 10

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2004), 10, 332–341.

Copyright © 2004 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA.
DOI: 10.10170S1355617704103019

Assessment of time perception: The effect of aging

MIGUEL COELHO,1 JOAQUIM JOSÉ FERREIRA,1,2 BEATRIZ DIAS,1 CRISTINA SAMPAIO,2


ISABEL PAVÃO MARTINS,1 and ALEXANDRE CASTRO-CALDAS 1
1 Laboratório de Estudos da Linguagem, Centro de Estudos Egas Moniz, Faculdade de Medicina de Lisboa, Hospital Santa Maria,
Av. Egas Muniz, 1649-028 Lisboa, Portugal
2 Instituto de Farmacologia e Terapêutica Geral, Faculdade de Medicina de Lisboa, Hospital Santa Maria, Av. Egas Muniz,

1649-028 Lisboa, Portugal


(Received June 3, 2002; Revised July 28, 2003; Accepted August 26, 2003)

Abstract
Studies concerning time perception lack a validated assessment tool and a consensual “gold-standard” measure.
Moreover, the present evidence suggests modification of timing with aging. This study aimed to develop and
validate a neuropsychological tool to measure time perception and to study temporal perception with aging.
Eighty-six healthy participants, aged 15–90 years old, were asked to verbally estimate and produce empty
intervals signaled by auditory beeps, of 7-, 32-, and 58-s duration. Two tests were used as “gold-standards”:
estimation of the duration of time necessary to draw a clock (“clock time”) and estimation of the duration of
neuropsychological evaluation (“global time”). Results showed a correlation between estimation and production
( p , .01) and a correlation between estimation or production and “global time” ( p , .01). The correlation
between either estimation or production and age ( p , .01), suggested a faster “internal-clock” in the older
participants. However, this finding lost significance when controlled for literacy. The results suggest that these
tests are potentially a useful tool to measure subjective perception of time. They also corroborate the hypothesis
of a change in subjective time perception with aging. It was not possible to conclude if this effect was a specific
result of aging or biased by the interference of literacy. (JINS, 2004, 10, 332–341.)
Keywords: Time perception, Aging, Working memory, Attention

INTRODUCTION explanation for subjective timing also assumes the exis-


tence of an “internal-clock” mechanism (Binkofski & Block,
Time perception and measure are essential components of
1996).
cognition, behavior, and motor performance, representing
Classically, time perception has been explained by two
one of the basic mechanisms of cerebral function (Artieda
different theoretical models: scalar expectancy theory (SET)
& Pastor, 1996). Accordingly, temporal processing is an (Gibbon et al., 1984) and attentional-gate theory (AG)
integral part of many everyday goal-oriented behaviors
(Block, 1990). Both models postulate the existence of a
(Mangels & Ivry, 2001), making it a crucial tool for plan- pacemaker (the “clock”) that generates neuronal pulses and
ning future actions. the existence of a counter accumulating them. This counter
Temporal perception comprises several subjective phe-
signals when the number of pulses generated reaches a tar-
nomena including the judgement of subjective duration (Pop-
get value corresponding to a given interval duration (Fortin
pel, 1997). The different aspects of psychological time are & Breton, 1995). The accumulator serves as a working mem-
interlinked with various attentional, memory, and other cog-
ory buffer and attention as the gate to working memory.
nitive processes (Binkofski & Block, 1996). However, the Concomitant nontemporal tasks interfering with working
memory and0or competing for attention will reduce the ac-
Reprint requests to: Miguel Coelho, Laboratório de Estudos da curacy in temporal perception (Fortin et al., 1993). To our
Linguagem, Centro de Estudos Egas Moniz, Faculdade de Medicina de
Lisboa, Hospital Santa Maria, Av. Egas Muniz, 1649-028 Lisboa, Portu- best knowledge there is no data regarding the effect of lit-
gal. E-mail: migcoelho2002@yahoo.es eracy on time perception.
332
Perception of time 333

Quantifying timing is difficult because there is no vali- Verbal estimation and production of a time
dated method, despite many tools being available (Bindra interval
& Waksberg, 1956), and it is still lacking a consensual
“gold-standard” test to measure temporal perception. There This experiment tested the ability to verbally estimate and
is also some evidence that timing changes with age produce the duration of empty time intervals signaled by
(Carrasco et al., 2001; Craik & Hay, 1999; Fraisse, 1963), auditory beeps. The auditory beeps were of 20-ms duration.
but to what extent and in what direction is still a matter of A specific software was developed to meet this aim. The
debate. Since timing is dependent on working memory durations of time intervals were of 7, 32, and 58 s for both
and attention, it is hypothesized that it should deteriorate estimation and production tests; every duration was re-
along neuropathological processes that interfere with those peated three times in a pseudorandom order (not allowing
cognitive functions. the repetition of the same number in sequence), so that for
The aims of our study were twofold: to develop and test both estimation and production tests nine tasks were per-
a neuropsychological tool to measure time perception and formed. Participants were not aware they would make nine
to study time perception along normal aging. trials and they did not know the test involved only three
possible interval durations.

MATERIAL AND METHODS Estimation test. Participants were told that they would
have to verbally estimate the duration of different intervals;
Research Participants one auditory beep indicated the onset of the interval while
The study was performed in healthy volunteers, defined as another indicated its end; the beeps were separated by a
participants with no known diseases and not taking drugs silent interval. Participants were instructed to start internal
known to interfere directly with the central nervous system. (“mental”) counting of seconds after listening to the first
It was intended to include ten patients per decade of life, with auditory beep and to stop counting when they heard the
an age spectrum ranging from 15–90 years old. This sample second beep. They were specifically instructed not to count
was not controlled for literacy due to the pragmatic need to aloud nor to perform any digital counting, or use any body
define normative values for the Portuguese population, which rhythm to help in the estimation. Then they were asked
is characterized by a natural higher literacy in younger indi- “how many seconds elapsed between the two beeps?”. The
viduals; moreover, we were not aware of a previous clear doc- test proper was preceded by a test run with an interval du-
umentation of a specific effect of literacy on time perception. ration of 4 s. The outcome measure was the ratio between
Exclusion criteria were a diagnosis of major depression (MINI the estimated duration and the target (7, 32, or 58 s) one.
International Neuropsychiatry Interview version 4.4 – For each participant, we calculated the mean of the nine
Portuguese version) (Sheehan et al., 1998), a diagnosis of ratios and this value was entered for the group statistical
dementia [Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) vali- analysis.
dated for the Portuguese population] (Folstein et al., 1975;
Guerreiro et al., 1994), the presence of any relevant neuro- Production test. Participants were told that they would
logical or psychiatric disease, and the consumption of drugs have to verbally produce different interval durations. In this
with central nervous system effect (benzodiazepines with an task, an auditory beep indicated the onset of the interval
equivalent dose of diazepam less than 10 mg daily were how- and its end was indicated by the participants, by telling the
ever accepted). Participants were also excluded if they abused examiner when they thought they had reached the target
alcohol, consumed more than four “espresso coffee” cups a duration. Participants were asked to start internal counting
day, and if they were illiterate or unable to understand the of seconds after they heard the first beep and to tell the
tests. Participants could only be evaluated if they had had a examiner when they thought they had reached the target
good night sleep according to their usual sleep pattern. All duration. The examiner wore a chronometer to know how
participants gave informed consent. many seconds had the participant really produced. The test
proper was preceded by a test run with an interval duration
Experimental Tasks of 4 s. The outcome measure was the ratio between the
produced duration (the value on the examiner’s chronom-
Temporal estimation and production tests were carried out eter) and the target (7, 32, or 58 s) one. For each partici-
with a standard Toshiba laptop personal computer and par- pant, we calculated the mean of the nine ratios and this
ticipants wore headphones to listen to the auditory stimuli. value was entered for the group statistical analysis.
The digits on the Digit Span Forward (DSF) and Digit Span
Reverse (DSR) (Garcia, 1984; Wechsler, 1969) tests were
“Gold-standards”
read by the examiner. Participants sat in a comfortable chair,
in a quiet and empty room without any rhythmic sound We assumed as “gold-standards” two tests we accepted as
(e.g., wall clock) and they were asked to remove their watch ecological, which replicate real-life time measurements. In
during the test. All participants were evaluated in the after- both cases, the participants were not aware they would be
noon due to logistical constraints. asked to retrospectively estimate the time elapsed during a
The experimental tasks were performed as follows: certain task.
334 M. Coelho et al.

Clock time. Participants were asked to draw a clock on calculated the mean of the nine ratios. If the ratio was su-
a blank sheet of paper. After finishing the drawing, the ex- perior to 1, the participant was said to overproduce, indi-
aminer asked the participant how much time had elapsed cating that the hypothetical “internal-clock” was going
between the moment he held the pen and finished the draw- slower. If the ratio was inferior to 1, the participant was
ing. The examiner used a chronometer to measure the du- said to underproduce, indicating that the “internal-clock”
ration of the task. The chronometer was started when the was going faster.
participant picked up the pen and stopped when the partici-
pant placed it on the paper. The outcome measure was the Clock time
ratio between the estimated time and the “real” duration
measured by the examiner. The outcome measure was the ratio between the estimated
time and the duration measured by the examiner.
Global time. At the very end of the neuropsychological
evaluation, the examiner asked the participant how much time Global time
had elapsed since they started the current evaluation. The on-
The outcome measure was the ratio between the estimated
set of the evaluation had been indicated out loud by the ex-
time and the duration measured by the examiner.
aminer with the sentence: “we are going to start the tests now”
and the examiner specifically referred to that moment when
Digit Span
asking the participant about the elapsed time. The examiner
wore a chronometer to know how much time the evaluation For each participant, we recorded the highest number of
had lasted. The chronometer was started when the examiner digits the participant could repeat, both in DSF and DSR.
said the mentioned sentence and stopped just before the ex- We studied the relation between estimation and produc-
aminer asked the participant about the elapsed time. The out- tion tests results and the two “gold-standards”. A sub-
come measure was the ratio between the estimated time and analysis was also performed, studying the relation between
the duration measured by the examiner. the different time intervals (7, 32, and 58 s) and both clock
and global time.
Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Reverse The age effect on estimation, production, clock time, glo-
bal time, and digit span were studied. The relation between
These tests were used to measure the attention0short-term the digit span and estimation, production, clock time, and
and working-memory capacity of the participants. In the global time was also investigated.
Digit Span Forward, the examiner told the participant a A comparison of estimation, production, clock time, and
maximum of seven sequences of digits, each with an in- global time was made between three predefined age groups
creasing number of digits (up to a maximum of nine digits) (group A: 15– 40 years old; group B: 41– 64 years old;
and at the end of each sequence the participants were asked group C: 65–90 years old). These age intervals were chosen
to repeat it in the same order. The Digit Span Reverse was according to what we assumed represents different stages
identical but the participant had to repeat each sequence of of aging: young, middle-age, and elderly people, respec-
digits in the reverse order. The outcome was the highest tively. We studied the effect of education and gender in
number of digits the participants could repeat correctly. The estimation, production, clock time, and global time. Educa-
digit span was the last test to be performed at the end of the tion (number of years in school) was compared between the
evaluation. three aforementioned age groups. The effect of literacy on
time perception was analyzed as a covariate. The analysis
Statistical Analysis of metric properties of estimation and production variables
showed they had a near-normal distribution. Although the
Estimation test size of the sample allowed for parametric tests, we chose to
make use of nonparametric tests in statistical analysis. Re-
The outcome measure was the ratio between the estimated
lations between variables were studied using nonparamet-
duration and the target (7, 32, or 58 s) duration. For each
ric tests for correlations (Spearman test), and comparisons
participant, we calculated the mean of the nine ratios.
of variables between the three age groups were made using
If the ratio was superior to 1, the participant was said to
nonparametric tests of variance (Kruskal-Wallis test).
overestimate, indicating that the hypothetical “internal-
clock” was going faster. If the ratio was inferior to 1, the
participant was said to underestimate, indicating that the RESULTS
“internal-clock” was going slower.
Ninety-seven participants were screened to participate in
this study. Seven participants were excluded due to diagno-
Production test
sis of dementia according to MMSE (4 participants) and
The outcome measure was the ratio between the produced diagnosis of major depression according to the MINI Inter-
duration (the value on the examiner’s chronometer) and the national Neuropsychiatry Interview (3 participants). One
target (7, 32, or 58 s) duration. For each participant, we participant was excluded because she was not able to un-
Perception of time 335

Table 1. Demographics

Age groups (years) 15–20 21–30 31– 40 41–50 51– 60 61–70 71–80 81–90
N (total 5 58) 10 12 10 10 11 10 14 9
F0M 109 1101 802 703 1001 505 905 702
Education (years) mean 11.8 17.3 14 12.2 8.8 7.1 7.0 7.5

Note. N 5 number of participants; F 5 female; M 5 male.

derstand the test instructions. Three individuals did not com- A positive correlation was found between estimation and
plete the evaluation due to tiredness, and their results were age, suggesting that older participants had faster “internal-
not included in the analysis. The final sample was of 86 clocks” ( p , .01; r 5 .2978) (Figure 1a). Conversely, a
participants and the data of these participants were the ba- negative correlation was found between production and age,
sis for the analysis. Their age ranged from 15–90 years old. also indicating an association between older participants
There were 58 female and 28 male participants. There were and faster “internal-clocks” ( p , .01; r 5 2.2731) (Fig-
roughly ten participants per decade of life (Table 1). ure 1b). We could not find a correlation between age and
Overall, the verbal estimation and production tests were either clock time or global time. All previous analysis lost
easy to explain and perform. statistical significance when controlled for literacy. Age was
Concerning the metric properties of estimation and pro- negatively correlated with education ( p , .01; r 5 2.6).
duction, the median value for estimation was 1.081 (Min The results showed a negative correlation between educa-
.2128 and Max 6.3988) and the median value for produc- tion and estimation ( p , .01; r 5 2.3951) and global time
tion was .8224 (Min .3382 and Max 1.788), with a statisti- ( p , .05; r 5 2.2521), and a positive correlation between
cal significant negative correlation between estimation and education and production ( p , .01; r 5 .3612), suggesting
production ( p , .01; r 5 2.5509). that participants with lower level of education had faster
A positive correlation was found between estimation and “internal-clocks”.
global time ( p , .01; r 5 .0712), and a negative correlation We compared the difference in performance between three
between production and global time ( p , .01; r 5 2.1591). predefined age groups (group A: 15– 40 years old, mean
A correlation of estimation or production and clock time 26.84, SD (standard deviation) 7.21; group B: 41– 64 years
was not demonstrated, as well as a correlation between clock old, mean 50.72, SD 4.93; group C: 65–90 years old, mean
and global time. The different time intervals (7, 32, and 75.84, SD 7.18; 95% confidence interval). When estimation
58 s), in both estimation and production tests, were all cor- was compared between the age groups there was a trend
related with global time but not with clock time, and the toward overestimation with older age, suggesting accelera-
power of correlation was similar between them. tion of “internal-clocks” with age (Figure 2a). The same

Fig. 1a. Correlation of age with time es-


timation. Age was positively correlated
[ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 .29)] with
the estimation of time, suggesting an ac-
celeration of the “internal-clock” with
aging.
336 M. Coelho et al.

Fig. 1b. Correlation of age with time pro-


duction. Age was negatively correlated
[ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 2.27)] with
the production of time, suggesting an ac-
celeration of the “internal-clock” with
aging.

analysis about production showed a trend toward underpro- .05; r 5 2.2434 for DSR) (Figure 4a). There was a positive
duction with age, also suggesting acceleration of “internal- correlation between production and DSF0 DSR ( p , .01;
clock” with age (Figure 2b). r 5 .3286, for DSF; p , .01; r 5 .3902, for DSR) (Figure 4b).
The performance on estimation, production, clock time,
and global time tasks was not significantly different be-
DISCUSSION
tween female and male participants.
As expected from the literature, we found a negative cor- The results of this study showed an internal consistency
relation between DSF0DSR and age ( p , .01; r 5 2.4715, between the performance on estimation and production tests,
for DSF; p , .01; r 5 2.4658, for DSR) (Figure 3). and found a correlation between those tasks and one of the
The results showed a negative correlation between DSF0 chosen “gold-standards” tests, suggesting that our assess-
DSR and estimation ( p , .01; r 5 2.3398, for DSF; p , ment tool is a potentially good measure of time duration.

Fig. 2a. Comparison of mean values of


time estimation between three age groups.
There was a trend towards overestima-
tion of time with aging.
Perception of time 337

Fig. 2b. Comparison of mean values of


time production between three age groups.
There was a trend towards underproduc-
tion of time with aging.

The results also showed a change of estimation and produc- method can claim a consistent superiority over the others
tion of time intervals with aging, suggesting an association and the correlation between them is either poor or absent.
of aging with a faster “internal-clock”. The aim of this study was to develop a neuropsycholog-
The extensive literature on time perception often differ ical tool to measure subjective duration and, secondly, to
on the methods used to measure temporal perception. How- study temporal perception along normal aging. Firstly, we
ever, none of these methods has yet been properly validated tried to overcome the lack of a “gold-standard” test, that is,
in a normal population, casting doubt on the theoretical a test that reliably measures what we want it to measure–
models based on their results. Also, as recognized by Allan subjective time perception. Thus, the first step was to iden-
(1979) and McConchie and Rutschmann (1971), no single tify a real-life measure against which our tests could be

Fig. 3. Correlation of Digit Span Forward with age [ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 2.47), left figure] and Digit Span
Reverse with age [ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 2.46), right figure] showing a decrease performance with aging.
338 M. Coelho et al.
Perception of time 339

compared. The “gold-standard”, which is in any case rela- timation, production, “global time”, and “clock time” be-
tive, should be as feasible and as close as possible a mea- tween the three age groups, we found the same trend towards
sure of temporal perception of everyday life, that is to say, acceleration of the “internal-clock” in older participants,
a pragmatic measure of the processing of temporal infor- suggesting that prospective and retrospective timing may
mation by the brain in real-life conditions. We assumed as a share common cognitive mechanisms.
“gold-standard” for subjective duration the “clock time” We found an association of estimation with production,
test and the “global time” test. In both tests, the participants which gives internal consistency to our results, and allows
performed tasks without knowing they would be asked to speculating about a common “internal-clock” subserving
estimate the time elapsed during their performance. They estimation and production (Ivry & Hazeltine, 1995; Keele
made retrospective evaluations, not paying attention to the et al., 1985).
passage of time, instead allocating attention towards the This study showed an age effect on Digit Span, and a
tasks themselves. We believe they inferred duration from change of estimation and production with aging when com-
contents of memory, memory for events, number of transi- pared between the three predefined age groups. This trend
tions between events, and expectation for duration of events was towards an acceleration of “internal-clock” in older
(Block & Zakay, 1997; Ornstein, 1969). Thus, retrospec- participants. A smaller study performed by Carrasco and
tive timing seems to depend mostly on long-term memory coworkers (2001) found similar results when comparing
and contextual events (Mangels & Ivry, 2001; Zakay, 1990). time estimation between young and elderly healthy partici-
On the other hand, during the tests of verbal estimation and pants. The study consisted of 25 adults, and the mean age of
production, participants knew they should actively monitor the young (26.15 years) and the elderly groups (79.1 years)
the passage of time. Their prospective measurement was were similar to that of groups A and C in our study. The
mostly dependent on attention, mostly attention to the vari- methodology and the time-interval duration (10 s) were very
able “time”, and working memory (Mangels & Ivry, 2001; similar to ours. Surprisingly, both results are in contrast
Zakay, 1990). with a previous study that found an association of older age
The evidence that retrospective and prospective timing with slower “internal-clock” (Craik & Hay, 1999). In that
depend upon different cognitive processes may explain the study, the interval durations were of the same range (30, 60,
weak association we found of estimation and production and 120 s) as ours, but the paradigms of estimation and
with “global time” and the lacking association of estima- production differed slightly, namely, participants per-
tion and production with “clock time”. Besides, an ecolog- formed the estimation and production tests while perform-
ical measure of time perception should have some utility ing other tasks at different levels of complexity, and this
for the participant, acting as a motivational drive. While the discrepancy in methodology can make comparison diffi-
“global time” measure was surely being subjectively as- cult. Data concerning age and short time intervals is still
sessed for its duration, the “clock time” measure had little scanty and inconclusive though (Nichelli, 1993). A study
usefulness or meaning for the participants of the experi- from Fraisse (1963) showed an association of aging with
ment. This may explain its lack of correlation with more acceleration of “internal-clock” regarding large units of time
conscious measures of time perception. Possibly, this dif- such as days or years; however, people estimates of inter-
ference in usefulness between the “global time” and the vals in range of hours may be related to sleep–wake cycles
“clock time” measures also explains the lack of association (Artieda & Pastor, 1996).
between each other. An alternative explanation could be The effect of education on cognition is well established,
their difference in magnitude duration (the “global time” in although we are not aware of any clear documentation of a
the range of 20–25 min while the “clock time” in the range specific effect of literacy on time perception. We found a
of a few seconds). In future experiments, we plan to com- correlation of education with estimation, production and
pare the estimation and production measures with ecologi- “global time”, suggesting faster “internal-clocks” in partici-
cal “gold-standards” that require prospective timing and pants with lower education. However, as it was anticipated
more meaningful time intervals, eventually increasing the from the characteristics of the Portuguese population and
strength of their statistical association; moreover, if more as our sample was not purposefully controlled for literacy,
than one “gold-standard” is used, they should be of same older participants were less literate. The correlation be-
magnitude and duration. Interestingly, when comparing es- tween either estimation or production and aging lost signif-

Fig. 4a. Correlation of time estimation with Digit Span Forward [ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 2.34), left figure] and
time estimation with Digit Span Reverse [ p , .05 (Spearman test r 5 2.24), right figure] showing that overestimation
of time is associated with a decrease performance in tests of attention and short-term memory0working memory.

Fig. 4b. Correlation of time production with digit span forward [ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 .39), left figure] and time
production with Digit Span Reverse [ p , .01 (Spearman test r 5 .33), right figure] showing that underproduction of
time is associated with a decrease performance in tests of attention and short-term memory0working memory.
340 M. Coelho et al.

icance when education was analyzed as a co-variate. The ings are confirmed, they must be explained in the light of
absence of a statistical difference of years of education be- current theoretical models for time perception.
tween the three predefined age groups argues against a pri-
mary effect of education on our results, suggesting that the ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
association of older participants with faster “internal-
clocks” derives mainly from aging. Nevertheless, it is not This study was supported by Grant 59098 from Fundação Bial,
Portugal.
possible to firmly conclude if the association of older par-
ticipants with faster “internal-clocks” is a specific result of
aging or biased by the interference of literacy. As the de- REFERENCES
sign of the present study was not intended to look for an Allan, L.G. (1979). The perception of time. Perception and Psy-
effect of education on timing, future studies must be con- chophysics, 26, 340–354.
ducted specifically to address this interesting question. Artieda, J. & Pastor, M.A. (1996). Time, internal clocks and move-
Our results demonstrated an association between estima- ment. In G.E. Stelmach & P.A. Vroon (Eds.), Advances in psy-
tion and production and both DSF and DSR, while there was chology, Vol. 115 (pp. 1–27). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
no association between DSF and DSR, and either both “clock Bindra, D. & Waksberg, H. (1956). Methods and terminology in the
time” or “global time”. This adds further evidence regarding studies of time estimation. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 155–159.
the role of attention0short-term and working memory in pro- Binkofski, F. & Block, R.A. (1996). Accelerated time experience
after left frontal cortex lesion. Neurocase, 2, 485– 493.
spective timing but not in retrospective timing (Block, 1992;
Block, R.A. (1990). Models of psychological time. In R.A. Block
Block & Zakay, 1997; Brown, 1997; Mangels & Ivry, 2001). (Ed.), Cognitive models of psychological time (pp. 1–35). Hills-
Nevertheless, the association of estimation and production dale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
with Digit Span may result from an effect of age on both tasks. Block, R.A. (1992). Prospective and retrospective duration judg-
The performance was similar between female and male ment: The role of information processing and memory. In F.
participants in all tasks, though a disproportionate number Macar & V. Pouthas (Eds.), Time, action and cognition: To-
of females were present in our sample. wards bridging the gap (pp. 141–152). Dordrecht, Nether-
Participants were explicitly asked to perform the estima- lands: Kluwer Academic.
tion and production tests by internal counting of seconds Block, R.A. & Zakay, D. (1997). Prospective and retrospective
and the intention was to have them producing their idea of duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bul-
1 s. The strategy of internal counting has been considered letin and Review, 4, 184–197.
Brown, S.W. (1997). Attentional resources in timing: Interference
to increase the accuracy of estimation (Davis, 1962; Fraisse,
affects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working mem-
1984; Wearden, 1991). Counting was also aimed at giving a ory tasks. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 1118–1140.
homogeneous activity for all participants during estimation Carrasco, M.C., Guillem, M.J., & Redolat, R. (2000). Estimation
and production tasks (Rubia et al., 1997). of short temporal intervals in Alzheimer’s disease. Experimen-
Physiopathologic speculations may be too premature, be- tal Aging Research, 26, 139–151.
cause replication of our results is still needed. Nevertheless, Carrasco, M.C., Bernal, M.C., & Redolat, R. (2001). Time esti-
and according to present theoretical models (Block, 1990; mation and aging: A comparison between young and elderly
Gibbon et al., 1984), the observed deficit in time estimation adults. International Journal of Aging and Human Develop-
could derive from a change in the speed of the “internal- ment, 52, 91–101.
clock”, which has been associated with several conditions Craik, F.I.M. & Hay, J.F. (1999). Aging and judgments of dura-
(Binkoski, 1996; Carrasco et al., 2000; Meck, 1996; Pastor tion: Effects of task complexity and method of estimation. Per-
ception and Psychophysics, 61, 549–560.
et al., 1992). Alternatively, a deficit in attention and working
Davis, R. (1962). L’estimation d’intervalles temporels en comp-
memory associated with aging could disrupt the counter sys- tant plus ou moins rapidement. Anné Psychologique, 62, 29– 44.
tem that accumulates the pulses generated by the “internal- Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-
clock”. Studies demonstrating deficits in time perception in Mental State. A practical method for grading the cognitive state
Alzheimer and Parkinson disease (Carrasco et al., 2000; Pas- of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research,
tor et al., 1992; Riesen & Schnider, 2001) suggest the possi- 12, 189–198.
ble involvement of neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine Fortin, C. & Breton, R. (1995). Temporal interval production and
and dopamine in time perception. processing in working memory. Perception and Psychophys-
Our results showed an acceleration of “internal-clock” ics, 57, 203–215.
with aging, and also suggested a role for attention and short- Fortin, C., Rousseau, R., Bourque, P., & Kirouac, E. (1993). Time
term0working memory on prospective timing. The assess- estimation and concurrent nontemporal processing: Specific
interference from short-term memory demands. Perception and
ment tool presented was easy to perform and was related to
Psychophysics, 53, 536–548.
one ecological measure of time perception. Nevertheless, Fraisse, P. (1963). The psychology of time. New York: Harper.
and due to its moderate association with the “gold-standards” Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Re-
and possible bias of results due to literacy, future well- view of Psychology, 35, 1–36.
designed studies are needed to replicate the present find- Garcia, C. (1984). A Doença de Alzheimer. Problemas de diagnós-
ings, to analyze its interobserver and intrasubject validity, tico clínico. Dissertação de Doutoramento. Lisboa: Faculdade
and to confirm its potential usefulness. If the present find- de Medicina de Lisboa.
Perception of time 341

Gibbon, J., Church, R.M., & Meck, W. (1984). Scalar timing in Ornstein, R.E. (1969). On the experience of time. Harmond-
memory. In J. Gibbon & L. Allan (Eds.), Annals of the New sworth, England: Penguin Books.
York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 423: Timing and time percep- Pastor, M.A., Artieda, J., Jahanshahi, M., & Obeso, J. (1992). Time
tion (pp. 52–77). New York: New York Academy of Sciences. estimation and reproduction is abnormal in Parkinson’s dis-
Guerreiro, M., Silva, A.P., Botelho, M.A., Leitão, O., Castro- ease. Brain, 115, 211–225.
Caldas, A., & Garcia, C. (1994). Adaptação à População Por- Poppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception.
tuguesa da tradução do “Mini Mental State Examination” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 56– 61.
(MMSE). Revista Portuguesa de Neurologia, 1, 9–10. Riesen, J.M. & Schnider, A. (2001). Time estimation in Parkin-
Ivry, R. & Hazeltine, R.E. (1995). Perception and production of son’s disease: Normal long duration estimation despite im-
temporal intervals across a range of duration: Evidence for a paired short duration discrimination. Journal of Neurology, 248,
common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychol- 27–35.
ogy: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 3–18. Rubia, K., Schuri, U., Cramon, D.Y.V., & Poppel, E. (1997). Time
Keele, S.W., Pokorny, R.A., Corcos, D.M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do estimation as a neuronal network property: A lesion study. Neu-
perception and motor production share common timing mech- roReport, 8, 1273–1276.
anisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs,
173–191. J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G.C. (1998).
Mangels, J.A. & Ivry, R.B. (2001). Time perception. In B. Rapp The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I):
(Ed.), Handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psy-
recall about the human mind, Chapter 19 (pp. 467– 493). Hove: chiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clini-
Psychological Press. cal Psychiatry, 59, 22–33; quiz 34–57. Adapted to Portuguese
McConchie, R.D. & Rutschmann, J. (1971). Human time estima- (Portugal) by T. Guterres.
tion: On difference between methods. Perceptual and Motor Wearden, J.H. (1991). Do humans possess an interval clock with
Skills, 32, 319–336. scalar timing properties? Learning and Motivation, 22, 59–83.
Meck, W.H. (1996). Neuropharmacology of timing and time per- Wechsler, D.A. (1969). Manuel de I’èchelle clinique de mémoire.
ception. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 227–242. Paris, France: Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.
Nichelli, P. (1993). The neuropsychology of human temporal in- Zakay, D. (1990). The evasive art of subjective time measure-
formation processing. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Hand- ment. In R.A. Block (Ed.), Cognitive models of psychological
book of neuropsychology, Vol. 8 (pp. 339–371). Amsterdam: time (pp. 59–84). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Elsevier. Associates.

También podría gustarte