Está en la página 1de 6

1040-5488/12/8909-1276/0 VOL. 89, NO. 9, PP.

1276–1281 OPTOMETRY
AND VISION SCIENCE Copyright © 2012 American Academy of Optometry

ARTÍCULO ORIGINAL

Accesibilidad de texto por personas con reducida


La sensibilidad de contraste

Michael D. Crossland * † y Gary S. Rubin *


Downloaded from https://journals.lww.com/optvissci by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3PxsYRkX7FpNejnD9kELdAGVWPhYVU/W9HKcF24waT2E=

ABSTRACTO

Propósito. sensibilidad al contraste se reduce en las personas con enfermedades de los ojos, y también en los adultos mayores sin enfermedad ocular. En este artículo, se compara el contraste
del texto presentado en formatos impresos y digitales con valores de sensibilidad de contraste para una gran cohorte de sujetos en un estudio basado en la población de adultos mayores (la
evaluación Salisbury Eye).

Métodos. los valores de sensibilidad de contraste se registraron para 2520 adultos de 65 a 84 años que viven en Salisbury, Maryland. La proporción de la muestra probable que sea incapaz de
leer el texto de diferentes formatos (libros electrónicos, papel de periódico, libros de bolsillo, impresión láser, y los monitores de ordenador LED) se calculó utilizando los niveles de reserva de
contraste publicada necesarios para realizar lectura puntual, a leer con fluidez, alta fluidez, y en condiciones óptimas.

Resultados. El uno por ciento de esta muestra tenía la sensibilidad al contraste menor que la requerida para leer el periódico con fluidez. Texto presentado en un monitor de
ordenador LED tuvo el mayor contraste. El noventa y ocho por ciento de la muestra tenía la sensibilidad al contraste suficiente para alta lectura fluida de texto (por lo menos 160
palabras / min) en un monitor. Sin embargo, el 29,6% eran todavía poco probable que sea capaz de leer este texto con fluidez óptima.

Conclusiones. contraste reducido de impresión accesibilidad límites de texto para muchas personas en el mundo desarrollado. La presentación de texto en un formato de alto
contraste, como el negro de impresión láser en una página blanca, aumentaría el número de personas que pueden acceder a dicha información. Además, por lo que el texto
disponible en un formato que puede ser presentado en un monitor de ordenador LED aumentará el acceso a los documentos escritos. (Optom Vis Sci 2012; 89: 1276-1281)

Palabras clave: la lectura, la sensibilidad al contraste, baja visión, la accesibilidad


on 08/30/2018

UN istradas por ser legalmente ciego, y una 2.4million aún tienen baja visión. 1 Sin embargo, se
artículo, Whittaker y Lovie-Kitchin revisión de la bibliografía sobre la lectura y
presentaron la “reserva de contraste” que se necesita para leer el texto en
proximadamesabequeunnúmerote900.mucho000personasmayor deen personlosEstadosqueUnidoshanreducidosonREGla
absoluto, y para leer con fluidez, con una alta fluidez, y en condiciones óptimas
visión sin estar registrado, 2 y que el error de refracción no corregidos y el envejecimiento normal pueden reducir la visión de
(Tabla 1). 15 The contrast reserve is the ratio of the text contrast to the contrast
las personas sin ningún tipo de enfermedad ocular identificado. 3,4
sensitivity. In this article, we analyze the contrast sensitivity data from a large
epidemiological study investigating visual function in a population of older
adults living in Maryland (the Salisbury Eye Evaluation [SEE]). The SEE study was
La sensibilidad de contraste es un aspecto de la función visual que disminuye con la edad 3 así
a population-based evaluation of adults aged between 65 and 84 years living in
como con enfermedades de los ojos. 5,6 Reducción de la sensibilidad al contraste es conocido por
the community. 16
estar asociado con dificultades en la lectura, 6-8 Reconocimiento facial, 9 reconocimiento de objetos,
10 movilidad, 11,12 y la comunicación en personas con audición reducida. 13 Poorer sensibilidad al
contraste también aumenta el riesgo de estar involucrado en una colisión del vehículo de motor
We use these data to determine what proportion of the population would be able
“en-defecto”. 14
to read text presented in different formats, assuming that the visual acuity
criterion is met (i.e., the text is large enough to be comfortably resolvedwith the
Para leer con fluidez, el texto debe ser presentado en varias veces mayor current spectacle prescription).
contraste que el umbral de sensibilidad al contraste. 7 En una clave
METHODS
*PhD
† MCOptom
The SEE identified adults aged between 65 and 84 years living in the
Salisbury, Maryland, metropolitan area, using the Health Care Financing
Visual Neuroscience, UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom, andBiomedical
ResearchCentre forOphthalmology, London, UnitedKingdom (both authors). Administration Medicare eligibility lists. In total, 4624 subjects were identified, of
whom 2520 received a compre-

Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 89, No. 9, September 2012


Text Accessibility by People with Reduced Contrast Sensitivity —Crossland and Rubin 1277

hensive visual assessment. As part of this assessment, contrast sensitivity was high fluency, and with optimal performance was calculated using the values
published by Whitaker and Lovie-Kitchin. 15
measured using the Pelli–Robson chart. 17 This test was performed monocularly on
each eye, at a distance of 1 m, in a well-lit room (chart luminance: approximately
100 cd/m 2), using the optimal refractive correction. Credit was given for each letter RESULTS
read correctly. Data from the better eye are presented in this article. Participants
Contrast sensitivity was recorded for 2507 subjects. Data were missing for 0.5%
were also asked a visual function questionnaire (the Activities of Daily Vision Scale).
of the sample (14 of 2520). Mean age of participants was 71 years (interquartile
18 One question on the Activities of Daily Vision Scale specifically addresses reading
range: 68–76 years), and the median best corrected distance visual acuity was 0.02
newsprint: “Would you say that you read the ordinary print in newspapers with: (a)
logMAR (20/20 1, 6/6 1, interquartile range:
no difficulty at all; (b) little difficulty; (c) moderate difficulty; (d) extreme difficulty; or
0.1 logMAR to 0.06
(e) you have not read a newspaper recently because of your vision?”
logMAR). Visual acuity and contrast sensitivity were moderately linearly related (r
0.69).
Median contrast sensitivity was 1.60 log units (interquartile range: 1.50–1.70 log
Ethical approval was obtained for the study, informed consent was received from
units). Fig. 1 shows the cumulative percentage of contrast sensitivity values. This
all participants, and the study conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki.
figure shows the proportion of the population whose contrast sensitivity is at, or
is poorer than, the value shown on the horizontal axis. Table 2 indicates the
Weber contrast values for different types of text were obtained by measuring
Weber contrast values for the different formats of text.
the luminance of the background (a blank area of white) and of print (a filled area
of black ink or print) using a photometer (CS-100, Minolta, Tokyo, Japan). This
measurement was performed in a bright office (room luminance: 940 lux [Testo
Fig. 2 andTable 3 show the proportion of the population unable to read text
presented in different formats at different levels of
540, Testo, Lenzkirch, Germany]), although Weber contrast is invariant to
changes in background luminance. The contrast sensitivity required to read
each text format at all, with fluency, with TABLE 2.
Weber contrast of different text formats

TABLE 1. Background Print


Contrast reserve required for different reading tasks Task luminance luminance Weber
Task (cd/m 2) (cd/m 2) contrast
Contrast reserve
Electronic book (Sony 70 16 77%
Spot reading 3:1 PRS-505)
Fluent reading (80 words/min) 4:1 Newspaper 97.2 18.2 81.2%
High-fluent reading (160 words/min) 10:1 Paperback book 109 15 86.2%
Optimal reading 30:1 Laser print 144 19.7 86.3%
Computer monitor (Apple 177 4.06 97.7%
Data from Whittaker and Lovie-Kitchin. 15
Cinema display)

FIGURE 1.
Cumulative percentage of contrast sensitivity values for this population.

Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 89, No. 9, September 2012


People with Reduced Contrast Sensitivity—Crossland and Rubin

FIGURE 2.
Proportion of population able to read different text with different levels of fluency for (A) electronic book, (B) newsprint and (C) paperback book. 1278 Text Accessibility by

Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 89, No. 9, September 2012


Text Accessibility by People with Reduced Contrast Sensitivity —Crossland and Rubin 1279

FIGURE 2.
Proportion of population able to read different text with different levels of fluency for (D) laser print and (E) computer monitor.

TABLE 3.
Contrast sensitivity value required for each task and each fluency value, and the percentage of the population with this contrast sensitivity value

Percentage of population with this


Contrast sensitivity required (log units) contrast sensitivity

Spot Fluent High fluent Optimum Spot Fluent High fluent Optimum

Electronic book 0.59 0.72 1.11 1.59 99.08 98.88 96.93 44.71
Newsprint 0.57 0.69 1.09 1.57 99.08 99.00 97.16 44.71
Paperback 0.54 0.67 1.06 1.54 99.16 99.00 97.16 60.51
Laser print 0.54 0.67 1.06 1.54 99.16 99.00 97.16 60.51
Computer monitor 0.49 0.61 1.01 1.49 99.24 99.02 97.73 70.40

Note: contrast values are rounded up to the nearest 0.05 log units when calculating the percentage of the population with this contrast sensitivity.

Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 89, No. 9, September 2012


1280 Text Accessibility by People with Reduced Contrast Sensitivity —Crossland and Rubin

TABLE 4. Use of self-reported visual function data indicated that declining contrast
Self-reported reading difficulty and mean contrast sensitivity value sensitivity was related to an increased probability of reporting difficulty in reading
newsprint. Approximately one-quarter of our population reported difficulty in
reading newsprint. This is greater than the proportion we predicted to read the
Mean (SD) contrast
newspaper with less than high fluency based on our contrast sensitivity model. Of
No. sensitivity value
course, many other visual and nonvisual properties will affect newspaper reading
Self-reported reading difficulty responses (log units)
performance, including visual acuity, the presence of visual field loss, and cognitive
No difficulty at all 1836 1.60 (0.10) and neurological factors. However, the relationship between self-reported reading
Little difficulty 372 1.55 (0.15) performance and contrast sensitivity confirms the importance of contrast
Moderate difficulty 86 1.50 (0.20) sensitivity on reading ability.
Extreme difficulty 17 1.40 (0.25)
Have not read because of vision 191 1.30 (0.45)

SD, standard deviation. Our Weber contrast values are higher than those previously reported by, for
example, Brilliant. 19 We suggest that this is because of advances in printing
technology, which has enabled print to be darker and paper to be brighter. It may
fluency. The poorest performance was found for electronic books (Fig. 2A): only
be that British newspapers and paperback books (which we have assessed in our
44.71% of the population was expected to be able to read electronic books or
study) have higher contrast than the North American publications described in
newsprint optimally. One percent of this population was unlikely to be able to
many low vision textbooks. The population of the SEEmay not be truly
read newsprint with any level of fluency. LED computer monitors had the highest
contrast text: 99% of the population could read this fluently, yet more than one representative. This population is older (minimumage: 65 years; median: 71 years)

person in four (29.6%) could not read this with optimal fluency. and includes subjects of predominantly white and African American origin.
Furthermore, these data are from a population with Medicare insurance and
access to health care. As contrast sensitivity is significantly reduced in cataract,
lower values may be expected in a population with poorer access to health care
One thousand eight hundred thirty-six participants (72.9%) did not report any
and without health insurance. These data are now several years old, but we have
difficulty in reading newsprint, and 666 (26.4%) reported some level of difficulty or
no reason to doubt their applicability at present. The population is aging, and the
had not read newsprint because of their vision. Data were missing for 18
mean age of a random sample of older adults would be expected to be older,
participants (0.7% of the sample). Mean contrast sensitivity in those reporting no
increasing the prevalence of age-related eye disease. However, this aging will
difficulty was 1.6 log units, whereas in those reporting any level of difficulty, it was
bemitigated by recent advances in ophthalmology, such as anti–vascular
1.45 log units. Table 4 shows the mean contrast sensivity values for different levels
endothelial growth factor treatment for age-related macular disease. The data
of self-reported difficulty in reading newsprint.
presented in this article examine only the effect of text contrast on reading
performance, and have not considered visual acuity. As there is a moderate
relationship between visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, it would be expected
that many of the people unable to read print because of poorer contrast
DISCUSSION
sensitivity would also struggle with text size. There will be other individuals who
Reduced contrast sensitivity creates difficulty in reading text. Although the have sufficient contrast sensitivity to access text but who will not be able to
proportion of the population who are unlikely to be able read text with resolve text because of its size. These people are likely to respond well to
fluency because of low contrast sensitivity is relatively small, this translates to magnification. Reduced contrast sensitivity can only be ameliorated by electronic
large numbers of people. For example, 1% of our population was unlikely to means, such as the use of an electronic magnifier or by manipulating text
read newsprint with any fluency because of reduced contrast sensitivity. If this presented on a computer monitor.
population is representative, this indicates that 395,000 older people in the
United States and 768,000 older people in the European Union are unable to
read newsprint fluently because of its text contrast.

Laser print is relatively easy to read, although 0.8% of the population (316,000
older Americans, 614,000 Europeans aged 65 years) is unable to read laser- We have only considered the eye with better contrast sensitivity in this
printed text even with the slowest “spot reading” fluency. If laser-printed investigation. As there is some binocular summation of contrast in people with
documents were instead sent electronically and viewed on a computer monitor, good vision, 20,21 one might expect true binocular contrast sensitivity to be better
an additional 0.08% of the older population would be able to read them. This than the values presented here. However, as binocular contrast sensitivity was not
number may sound small, but it equates to 31,600 more people in the Unites measured in the present study, and contrast summation is limited or reduced in
States and 61,400 more people in the EuropeanUnion being able to read them. some people with eye disease, 22 we felt more comfortable using a measured
This is of clear importance for banks, utility companies, and government agencies response than an inferred binocular contrast sensitivity value.
that send communication by post. Of course, presenting text on a computer
monitor is no guarantee that it will be easily readable by the end user: color
contrast, the boldness of text, and the ability to manipulate the format and A final limitation of our work is that we have compared contrast sensitivity
spacing of text are also important. values measured at 1 m using large characters to a reading task, typically
performed with smaller characters viewed from a distance of about 40 cm. This
approach has been used before, for

Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 89, No. 9, September 2012


Text Accessibility by People with Reduced Contrast Sensitivity—Crossland and Rubin 1281

example, byMohammed andDickinson. 23 By extending the viewing distance of a 10. Owsley C, Sloane ME. Contrast sensitivity, acuity, and the perception of ‘real-world ’
Pelli–Robson chart, these authors found that contrast sensitivity did not vary targets. Br J Ophthalmol 1987;71:791–6.

significantly with angular size of the letters on the chart. 11. West SK, Rubin GS, Broman AT, Munoz B, Bandeen-Roche K, Turano K. How does
visual impairment affect performance on tasks of everyday life? The SEE Project.
Salisbury Eye Evaluation. Arch Ophthalmol 2002;120:774–80.

CONCLUSIONS
12. Turano KA, Broman AT, Bandeen-Roche K, Munoz B, Rubin GS, West S. Association of
Reduced text contrast restricts accessibility to the written word for a large visual field loss and mobility performance in older adults: Salisbury eye evaluation

number of people in the developed world. Making text available in a format study. OptomVis Sci 2004;81: 298–307.

that can be presented on an LED computer monitor will increase access to


written documents. 13. Dickinson CM, Taylor J. The effect of simulated visual impairment on
speech-reading ability. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2011;31: 249–57.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
14. Owsley C, Stalvey BT, Wells J, Sloane ME, McGwin G, Jr. Visual risk factors for crash
The authors wish to thank GF Mueden for providing the motivation for this analysis. MDC is involvement in older drivers with cataract. Arch Ophthalmol 2001;119:881–7.
supported by the National Institute for Health Research grant PDF/01/2008/011. This report
describes independent research arising from a grant supported by the National Institute for
15. Whittaker SG, Lovie-Kitchin J. Visual requirements for reading. Optom Vis Sci
Health Research. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not
1993;70:54–65.
necessarily those of the NHS, the National Institute for Health Research or the Depart- ment of
Health. 16. West SK, Munoz B, Rubin GS, Schein OD, Bandeen-Roche K, Zeger S, German S,
Fried LP. Function and visual impairment in a population-based study of older
Received January 5, 2012; accepted April 11, 2012. adults. The SEE project. Salisbury eye evaluation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci
1997;38:72–82.

REFERENCES 17. Pelli DG, Robson JG, Wilkins AJ. The design of a new letter chart for measuring
contrast sensitivity. Clin Vision Sci 1988;2:187–99.
1. CongdonN, O’Colmain B, Klaver CC, KleinR,Munoz B, Friedman DS, Kempen J, 18. Mangione CM, Phillips RS, Seddon JM, Lawrence MG, Cook EF, Dailey R, Goldman L.
Taylor HR, Mitchell P. Causes and prevalence of visual impairment among adults in Development of the ‘Activities of Daily Vision Scale’. A measure of visual functional
the United States. Arch Ophthalmol 2004;122:477–85. status. Med Care 1992;30: 1111–26.

2. Ryan B, Margrain TH. Registration for people with sight impairment: fit for 19. Brilliant RL, ed. Essentials of Low Vision Practice. Boston, MA:
purpose? Br J Ophthalmol 2010;94:1692–3.
Butterworth-Heinemann; 1999.
3. Owsley C. Aging and vision. Vision Res 2011;51:1610–22.
20. Legge GE. Binocular contrast summation. II. Quadratic summation. Vision Res
4. Sjostrand J, Laatikainen L, Hirvela H, Popovic Z, Jonsson R. The decline in visual acuity in
1984;24:385–94.
elderly people with healthy eyes or eyes with early age-related maculopathy in two
21. Legge GE. Binocular contrast summation. I. Detection and discrimination. Vision
Scandinavian population samples. Acta Ophthalmol 2011;89:116–23.
Res 1984;24:373–83.
22. Valberg A, Fosse P. Binocular contrast inhibition in subjects with age-relatedmacular
5. Elliott DB, Situ P. Visual acuity versus letter contrast sensitivity in early cataract.
degeneration. J Opt Soc Am (A) 2002;19:223–8.
Vision Res 1998;38:2047–52.
23. Mohammed Z, DickinsonCM. The inter-relationship betweenmagnification, field of
6. Rubin GS, Legge GE. Psychophysics of reading. VI. The role of contrast in low
view and contrast reserve: the effect on reading performance. Ophthalmic Physiol
vision. Vision Res 1989;29:79–91.
Opt 2000;20:464–72.
7. Legge GE, Rubin GS, Luebker A. Psychophysics of reading. V. The role of contrast in
normal vision. Vision Res 1987;27:1165–77.
8. Leat SJ, Woo GC. The validity of current clinical tests of contrast sensitivity and their
Michael Crossland
ability to predict reading speed in low vision. Eye 1997;11:893–9.
UCL Institute of Ophthalmology
11–43 Bath Street
9. Tejeria L, Harper RA, Artes PH, Dickinson CM. Face recognition in age related
macular degeneration: perceived disability, measured disability, and performance London EC1V 9EL
with a bioptic device. Br J Ophthalmol 2002;86:1019–26. United Kingdom
e-mail: m.crossland@ucl.ac.uk

Optometry and Vision Science, Vol. 89, No. 9, September 2012

También podría gustarte