Está en la página 1de 12

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, MAY–JUNE 2012,

VOL. 47, NO. 3, 233–244

Research Report
The Social Communication Intervention Project: a randomized controlled
trial of the effectiveness of speech and language therapy for school-age
children who have pragmatic and social communication problems with
or without autism spectrum disorder
Catherine Adams†, Elaine Lockton†, Jenny Freed†, Jacqueline Gaile†, Gillian Earl‡, Kirsty McBean†,
Marysia Nash§, Jonathan Green¶, Andy Vail¶ and James Law∗

Human Communication and Deafness Division, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK

NHS Lothian, Edinburgh, UK
§
Speech and Language Therapy Department, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh, UK

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK

School of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, University of Newcastle, Newcastle, UK

(Received August 2011; accepted December 2011)

Abstract
Background: Children who show disproportionate difficulty with the pragmatic as compared with the structural
aspects of language are described as having pragmatic language impairment (PLI) or social communication disorder
(SCD). Some children who have PLI also show mild social impairments associated with high-functioning autism or
autism spectrum disorder (ASD). There is little robust evidence of effectiveness of speech–language interventions
which target the language, pragmatic or social communication needs of these children.
Aims: To evaluate the effectiveness of an intensive manualized social communication intervention (SCIP) for
children who have PLI with or without features of ASD.
Methods & Procedures: In a single-blind RCT design, 88 children with pragmatic and social communication needs
aged 5;11–10;8, recruited from UK speech and language therapy services, were randomly assigned in a 2:1 ratio
to SCIP or to treatment-as-usual. Children in the SCIP condition received up to 20 sessions of direct intervention
from a specialist research speech and language therapist working with supervised assistants. All therapy content
and methodology was derived from an intervention manual. A primary outcome measure of structural language
and secondary outcome measures of narrative, parent-reported pragmatic functioning and social communication,
blind-rated perceptions of conversational competence and teacher-reported ratings of classroom learning skills
were taken pre-intervention, immediately post-intervention and at 6-month follow-up. Analysis was by intention
to treat.
Outcomes & Results: No significant treatment effect was found for the primary outcome measure of structural
language ability or for a measure of narrative ability. Significant treatment effects were found for blind-rated
perceptions of conversational competence, for parent-reported measures of pragmatic functioning and social
communication, and for teacher-reported ratings of classroom learning skills.
Conclusions & Implications: There is some evidence of an intervention effect on blind and parent/teacher-reported
communication outcomes, but not standardized language assessment outcomes, for 6–11-year-old children who
have pragmatic and social communication needs. These findings are discussed in the context of the increasingly
central role of service user outcomes in providing evidence for an intervention. The substantial overlap between
the presence of PLI and ASD (75%) across the whole cohort suggests that the intervention may also be applicable
to some verbally able children with ASD who have pragmatic communication needs.

Keywords: language impairment, pragmatics, social communication disorder, treatment, randomized controlled
trial

Address correspondence to: Catherine Adams, Human Communication and Deafness Group, Ellen Wilkinson Building, University of
Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PL, UK; email: catherine.adams@manchester.ac.uk
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  c 2012 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists
DOI: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00146.x
234 Catherine Adams et al.
What is already known?
Children who have pragmatic and social communication needs are at risk of long term behavioural and social
disadvantage. These children often have additional autism spectrum conditions and/or high-level language
impairments. There are no randomized controlled trials of speech-language intervention for these children.
What this paper adds:
This paper reports evidence of an effect of intensive, specialist supervised speech and language therapy in improving
overall conversational competence and functional social communication skills for school-age children who have
persistent pragmatic and social communication needs. There was no effect of treatment on structural language skills.
The majority of children with pragmatic difficulties also had a history of autism spectrum disorder or pervasive
developmental disorder. Parent and teacher outcomes were important measures in the context of the implementation
of a complex intervention where measurement of pragmatic and social communication skills remain challenging.

Background disorder (SCD)’ may be a more appropriate descriptor.


In proposals for the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and
Pragmatic language impairment (PLI) is present when
Statistical Manual of the American Psychiatric Associa-
children have disproportionate difficulty with the
tion (n.d.), SCD is categorized as a subtype of Language
pragmatic domain of language in relation to relative
Impairment with features that closely match the descrip-
strength in grammar and phonology (Bishop 2000).
tion of PLI provided by Bishop and colleagues in earlier
Pragmatic features of PLI include verbosity, excessive
work. The diagnostic overlaps remain unresolved. In
topic switching, a tendency to dominate verbal interac-
the current study, the term ‘children who have PLI
tions, poor adjustment to listeners’ prior knowledge and
(CwPLI)’ will be used to describe children with a pattern
limited application of inference in naturalistic interac-
of pragmatic and social communication needs similar to
tion (Bishop and Adams 1989, Adams 2001). Symptoms
those described by Bishop (2000) and the SCD profile in
characteristic of specific language impairment (SLI)
DSM-5.
are also present in PLI with some children presenting
The number of children who have significant needs
semantic errors, word-finding difficulties and persistent
in pragmatics and social communication is rising in line
difficulty with receptive language. High-level language
with increased identification of broader ASD conditions
deficits such as poor comprehension of non-literal
(Baird et al. 2006). Botting and Conti-Ramsden (1999)
language and narratives and stories are also evident
identified 22% of children attending UK special-
(Botting and Adams 2005). As a group, children who
ist language units as having PLI. Emerging evidence
have PLI (CwPLI) present considerable heterogeneity
suggests that pragmatic difficulties in the primary school
in social communication and language skills, in both
years may be related to emotional and behaviour difficul-
pattern and severity. The full profile of PLI emerges in
ties, high referral rates to educational psychology services
the early school years as the disparity between structural
(Mackie and Law 2010), longer-than-usual time spent
and social language functioning becomes clear.
in special provision (Botting et al. 1998), and specific
The pragmatic characteristics of CwPLI are similar
difficulty in forming adult relationships in later life
to those reported in high-functioning children who
(Whitehouse et al. 2009).
have autism spectrum disorder (HF-ASD) or pervasive
The need for adequate communication interven-
developmental disorder (PDD1 ) (Landa 2000). PLI and
tions as preventative measures is therefore consider-
HF-ASD may share other abnormalities of communica-
able. Single case studies of CwPLI (Adams 2001) and
tion such as use of stereotyped phrases, intonation and
SCD (Timler et al. 2005, Brinton et al. 2005) have
non-verbal interaction. As with CwPLI, a significant
shown positive effects of intervention on specific aspects
proportion of children who have HF-ASD also have a
of communication such as conversational skills; a case
profile of language impairment similar to SLI (Kjelgaard
study series (Adams et al. 2006) reported a signal of
and Tager-Flusberg 2001). There is consequently
change as a result of intensive speech–language therapy
controversy surrounding the use of PLI as a diagnosis
for six CwPLI. There have been no trials of speech–
distinct from HF-ASD, since the two groups may not
language interventions for CwPLI (Law et al. 2003). The
be distinguishable in terms of social communication
aims of the present study were to examine the effective-
and social interaction deficits. It has been suggested
ness of an intensive manualized social communica-
that CwPLI are typically differentiated from CwHF-
tion speech and language intervention in improving
ASD by the absence of repetitive/restricted behaviours
(1) language skills and (2) observed functional
(Bishop 1998, Leyfer et al. 2008). In addition, since the
pragmatic ability and broader social communication
pattern of communication deficits seen in PLI clearly
of CwPLI within a small-scale randomized controlled
extends beyond the pragmatics domain, there is increas-
trial.
ing consensus that the term ‘social communication
RCT of speech/language therapy for children with pragmatic language impairment 235
Methods • A score in the communication impaired
range (≤ 58) on the General Communication
The two-arm parallel-group randomized controlled trial
Composite (GCC) of the Children’s Communica-
was carried out in accordance with the NHS Research
tion Checklist—Second Edition (CCC-2; Bishop
Governance Framework for Health and Social Care
2003), a measure functional communication
(NHS 2005) and was approved by the Northern
including pragmatics completed by parents.
and Yorkshire NHS Research Ethics Committee (REC
• A score ≥ 5th centile on Raven’s Coloured
No. 07/MRE03/3). All parents gave written informed
Progressive Matrices (RCPM; Raven 1979), a
consent for their child’s participation. Informed consent
test of non-verbal perceptual/analogical reasoning
was also gained from the child (where able), their school,
skills.
class teacher and learning support assistant (LSA) and
the relevant local authorities.
Screening assessments were carried out by a
researcher in the child’s school with one or both parents
Participants present.
The Social Interaction Deviance Composite (SIDC)
The study was powered on the basis of a pilot study
of the CCC-2 and the Social Communication Question-
of the intervention (n = 6; Adams et al. 2006). This
naire Lifetime version (SCQ) (Rutter et al. 2003), a
demonstrated a clinically significant standardized effect
parent questionnaire which asks about the history of
size for the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamen-
diagnostic features of autism, were also completed to
tals (CELF-4; Semel et al. 2006) of 0.62 (difference
provide information on group characteristics but were
in means of 3.1 with an SD of 5, based on the sum
not used as part of the inclusion criteria. An SCQ cut-
of two subtests only). Therefore, target recruitment for
off score of ≥15 is considered indicative of a history of
the current study was set at 99 participants, providing
PDD, and a cut-off score of ≥22 is considered indicative
> 80% power to detect a standardized effect size of 0.6
of a history of ASD (Berument et al. 1999).2
in CELF-4 as the primary outcome.
Speech and language therapists (SLTs) across the
North West of England and South East Scotland referred Randomization, blinding and schedule of
to the trial any children on their caseloads aged 6 years assessments
to 10 years 11 months who met the following inclusion
criteria: Following screening and baseline (T1) assessments, a
researcher at the University of Manchester, who was
not independent of the study, randomly assigned each
• Pragmatic communication problems as observed child to SCIP intervention or treatment-as-usual (TAU)
by the child’s SLT (including a minimum of two in a 2:1 ratio, stratified by age group (6;00–8;11 or
out of five pragmatic behaviours from a social 9;00–10;11). A 2:1 ratio was used to improve power
communication behaviour checklist (SCBC, see for later moderator and mediator exploratory analyses.
appendix A). Allocation used three possible permuted blocks of size
• Attending mainstream primary education three:
provision and identified as having Special
Educational Needs (England) or Additional
Support Needs (Scotland). 1: SCIP TAU SCIP
2: SCIP SCIP TAU
• English as the primary language of communica- 3: TAU SCIP SCIP
tion and learning.
• Agreement with the child’s school to accommo-
date intervention and assessment visits. A new block was selected after every three children in
• No current diagnosis of core autism. the same age band.
• Currently receiving regular, on-going attention All assessments (delivery, coding and scoring) were
from SLT services and able to cooperate with direct completed by a research assistant (RA) blind to
intervention. treatment allocation; however, families, schools and
• No evidence of severe difficulties in emotional those delivering intervention could not be blind to
development, behaviour needs, unintelligibility or treatment allocation. At assessment points, children,
hearing. parents and schools were reminded that the RA was
blind to treatment allocation and that anything related
All referred children were assessed for eligibility and to this should not be discussed with them. Question-
included in the study if they met the following screening naires completed by parents or teachers (CCC-2, PRO
criteria: and TRO—see the outcome measures) were returned
236 Catherine Adams et al.
by post to a researcher who was not involved with for LSAs; three had provided training to LSAs aimed
their child’s intervention. Reassessments were carried at the management of an individual child. Children in
out in the child’s school within 2 weeks following both arms continued to receive support from their LSAs
the completion of intervention/TAU (T2) and at (and/or classroom support for learning) throughout the
6 months following the completion of interven- study.
tion/TAU (T3). SCIP intervention began within 4 Post-intervention, children in the SCIP intervention
weeks of pre-intervention (T1) assessment. arm were referred back to their local SLT services for
on-going care. Provision post-intervention to 6-month
follow-up was documented through telephone liaison
Interventions
with local SLT services obtained for 21/29 children in
Participants in the SCIP group ceased their usual the TAU arm and for 47/59 children in the SCIP arm.
treatment as provided by their local SLT services for Of these children 17 (81%) in the TAU arm, and 30
the experimental intervention period. The experimental (63%) in the SCIP arm, received SLT contact. Of these
treatment was an intensive manualized social communi- children, 12 (57% of TAU; 26% of SCIP) children
cation intervention with content within a principled from each arm were reported to have received direct
framework aimed at remediation of impairments in individual or group specialist intervention.
semantics and high-level language skills, pragmatic After 6-month follow-up (T3) measures, all SLTs of
difficulties, and social interaction and social cue children allocated to TAU received a manual interven-
interpretation (Adams and Gaile 2012). The research tion plan based on what the child would have received
intervention manual provided procedures for planning had they been in the SCIP intervention arm.
intervention and establishing appropriate goals as well
as all intervention activities. For each child, between
Outcome measures
16 and 20 individual face-to-face one hour sessions
of intervention (up to three sessions per week) were The primary outcome measure was predefined as the
delivered in school over the course of one school CELF-4 (Semel et al. 2006) Core Language Standard
term. Parent/teacher input was solicited throughout the Score (CLSS) as a recognized, reliable and standard-
setting of goals and intervention period. Each child ized measure of general language ability, which can be
therefore received an individualized intervention derived administered blind to treatment allocation. The relevant
from the manual, but within a specified framework, subtests were completed with each participant (taking
as outlined above, so as to ensure that intervention into account age at that time point) to allow the calcula-
choices were consistent. Two specialist research speech tion of composite core language scores (see appendix
and language therapists (RSLT) (one at each of the two A).
locations) and five specially trained therapy assistants Secondary outcome measures were:
(ThAs) (England n = 4, Scotland n = 1) delivered the
experimental treatment. Treatment fidelity, measured by • Targeted Observation of Pragmatics in Children’s
audit of planned intervention sessions versus received Conversation (TOPICC; Adams et al. 2011): a
sessions and adherence to written activity procedure semi-structured task that allows for the rating of
as stated in the manual (both across 10% of sample) overall quality of interaction in conversation (the
was at ≥ 80% for both measures. A detailed and same task was used in Bishop and Adams 1989).
precise account of the experimental SCIP interven- Each participant was video recorded discussing a
tion, including rationale, the process of manualization, standard series of three photographs of events with
implementation, intervention components, individu- an RA who used a series of question prompts.
alization procedures, required level of practitioner The aim of the task was to obtain a sample of
expertise and treatment fidelity measures, is provided natural conversation with the child. Three sets of
in Adams et al. (2012). photographs were used in rotation for each partici-
Children allocated to TAU continued with the pant at T1, T2 and T3. Following completion
treatment being provided by their local SLT services. of all outcome measurement, the video recorded
This provision was documented through telephone conversations were rated by a trained, indepen-
liaison with these services (data obtained for 27/29 dent assessor, blind to treatment allocation, at
children). Thirteen of these children (48%) received the University of Manchester. The assessor was
Learning Support Assistant (LSA) support provided by asked to make a judgement of overall conversa-
the Education Authority plus SLT contact or contact tional skills of the child, rating them as improved,
from another communication professional. Ten of stayed the same or got worse from T1 to T3.
these children received direct individual or group-based A proportion of T1 and T3 videos (37% of total
intervention. Seventeen SLTs reported general training available; n = 29, TAU n = 10, SCIP n = 19) were
RCT of speech/language therapy for children with pragmatic language impairment 237
rated independently by a second assessor also blind • Teacher-reported outcome (TRO): teachers were
to group status. Raters were asked to consider asked, via a questionnaire, to state their judgments
the overall impact of any pragmatic behaviours about the current status of the child’s classroom
on the overall quality of interaction. The inter- learning skills (TRO-CLS) and to rate whether
rater reliability for the raters was Cohen’s these have improved, stayed the same or got worse
κ = 0.68, indicating a substantial inter-rater since pre-intervention (T1). Teachers completed
agreement for overall impression of conversation TRO questionnaires at school at 6 months
change. following the completion of intervention (T3)
• A pragmatics rating scale (CCC-PRAG), derived, only.
a priori, from the CCC-2. Two highly special-
ist RSLTs provided face validity for this scale by Statistical analysis
identifying a list of 18 items which in their view Analysis was by intention to treat3 and was undertaken
was closely related to the content of SCIP interven- after completion of all T3 assessments. For the primary
tion. An autism-type communication problems outcome, and secondary outcomes CCC-2 PRAG/AUT
scale (CCC-AUT), consisting of 18 items not and ERRNI, analysis was by linear regression with
targeted by SCIP intervention directly, was also adjustment for age and the corresponding T1 measure.
identified as a control for predictable bias by Analysis for TOPICC, PRO and TRO was by logistic
parents in the experimental intervention arm. regression with similar adjustment for age and the
Parents completing the CCC-2 would be unaware corresponding T1 measure.
of separate PRAG/AUT lists embedded within
it. CCC-PRAG and AUT lists are shown in
appendix A. Scores are sums of rated items, Results
converted from CCC-2 scoring to a single polarity,
with higher scores indicating greater impairment. Participants were recruited between April 2007 and
Split-half reliability (internal consistency) of both April 2008 in England and between October 2007 and
CCC-PRAG and CCC-AUT scales were in the September 2008 in Scotland. All assessments occurred
acceptable range (Cronbach’s α for CCC-PRAG within schedule.
= 0.82; for CCC-AUT α = 0.80). Parents Figure 1 shows the trial profile. The study under-
completed CCC-PRAG/AUT lists at home within recruited participants (final n = 88) compared with the
2 weeks following the completion of intervention target. Fifty-nine participants were randomly assigned
(T2) and at 6 months following the completion to the SCIP group and 29 participants were randomly
of intervention (T3). Completed CCCs were assigned to the TAU group. In the SCIP group
returned by post to a researcher who was not 57 participants received intended treatment and were
involved in the child’s intervention. analysed for the primary outcome. In the TAU group
• The Expression, Reception and Recall of Narrative 28 participants received the intended treatment and
Instrument (ERRNI; Bishop 2004) was used as a were analysed for the primary outcome. Attrition
measure of ability to interpret, remember and tell was therefore 3/88 (3%) for the primary endpoint.
a pictured narrative. Standardized scores are given One participant who withdrew from the study during
for initial telling of the story (ERRNI-I), story intervention also withdrew consent for their T1 data to
recall (ERRNI-R) and comprehension (ERRNI- be used.
C). The ERRNI story was alternated at each time Table 1 shows baseline demographic and clinical
point (therefore the same story was used at T1 characteristics for each group. The groups were well
and T3). matched on age, demographics, CELF-4 CLSS and
• Parent-reported outcome (PRO): parents were CCC-2 GCC. Mean Raven’s Progressive Coloured
asked, via a questionnaire, to state their judgments Matrices mid-point percentile score were slightly higher
about the current status of their child’s Language in the SCIP than in the TAU group. Scores on the SCQ
Skills (PRO-LS), Social Communication (PRO- showed that 75% of participants scores (76% of SCIP
SC), Social Situations (PRO-SS) and Peer and 73% of TAU) fell within the range indicative of
Relationships (PRO-PR) and to rate whether these the child having PDD or ASD features at some stage in
have improved, stayed the same or got worse development.
since pre-intervention (T1). Parents completed
Primary outcome
PRO questionnaires at home within 2 weeks
following the completion of intervention (T2) The primary outcome measure was CELF-4 CLSS.
and at 6 months following the completion Figure 2 shows the spread of CELF-4 CLSS scores at
of intervention (T3). T1, T2 and T3 for SCIP and TAU groups.
238 Catherine Adams et al.

Figure 1. Trial profile. SCIP, Social Communication Intervention Project.

Figure 2. CELF-4 Core Language Standard Score at T1, T2 and T3 for TAU and SCIP intervention groups. The dotted line is at the CELF-4
CLSS population mean of 100; and the dashed line is at the cut-off between normal range (≥ 80) and language impaired (< 80).
RCT of speech/language therapy for children with pragmatic language impairment 239
Table 1. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of each groupc

Whole group SCIP TAU


n = 87 n = 59 n = 28
75 boys, 12 girls 52 boys, 7 girls 23 boys, 5 girls
Mean SD Range Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
Age (months) 100.5 15.0 71–128 100.9 15.1 71–128 99.6 14.8 74–125
NRS ‘ABC1’ demographicsb
Middle to high areaa 30 (34%) 18 (31%) 12 (43%)
Middle to low areaa 49 (56%) 34 (58%) 15 (54%)
Mixed areaa 8 (9%) 7 (12%) 1 (4%)
SCBC (n = 80) 4.4 0.7 3–5 4.4 0.7 3–5 4.4 0.7 3–5
CCC-2 GCC 29.3 12.0 6–57 29.5 12.1 6–57 28.8 12.0 7–56
RCPM percentilee 53.5 28.6 5–96 49.2 28.9 5–96 62.6 26.2 17.5–96
CELF-4 CLSSd 72.6 18.3 40–114 71.3 16.8 40–112 75.3 21.2 40–114
Language impaired CELF-4 61.6 11.2 40–79 62.3 10.5 40–79 59.7 13.2 40–79
CLSSd < 80 (n = 56)
Not language impaired 92.4 9.9 81–114 91.7 8.0 81–112 93.3 12.4 81–114
CELF-4 CLSSd ≥ 80 (n = 31)
CCC-PRAG (n = 86) 34.5 10.0 10–54 34.2 9.9 15–54 35.2 10.3 10–50
CCC-AUT 28.1 10.2 6–49 27.9 10.6 8–49 28.4 9.6 6–44
ERRNI-Id 90.9 17.4 64–135 90.3 17.4 65–135 92.1 17.6 64–122
ERRNI-R d 86.4 18.2 64–136 85.8 17.2 64–131 87.7 20.3 64–136
ERRNI-Cd 87.7 16.0 64–125 87.2 16.6 64–125 88.7 14.8 64–115
SCQ (n = 84)∗ 20.3 7.9 2–37 20.3 7.7 3–37 20.2 8.3 2–33
Non-ASD SCQ ≤ 14a 21 (25%) 14 (24%) 7 (27%)
PDD-NOS SCQ 15–22a 23 (27%) 17 (29%) 6 (23%)
ASD SCQ ≥ 22a 40 (48%) 27 (47%) 13 (50%)
Notes: a n (%).
b
Area based on postcode. Middle to high areas include ≥ C2 only; middle to low areas include ≤ C1 only; and mixed areas included a range from A/B to D/E.
c
n = 87 as one child withdrew without giving permission to use the data.
d
Standard scores have a population mean = 100.
e
RCPM percentile ranges transformed into percentile midpoints, e.g. 5th–10th percentile becomes 7.5th percentile

SCQ scores ≥ 15 are considered indicative of PDD-NOS; and scores ≥ 22 are considered indicative of ASD.
NRS, National Readership Survey (NRS Ltd); SCBC, Social Communication Behaviour Checklist; CCC-2 GCC, Children’s Communication Checklist-2 General Communication
Composite; RCPM, Ravens Coloured Progressive Matrices; CELF-4 CLSS, Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Core Language Standard Score; CCC-PRAG, Pragmatics
rating scale; CCC-AUT, Autism-Communication Rating Scale; ERRNI-I/R/C, Expression, Reception and Recall of Narrative Instrument Initial Telling/Story Recall/Comprehension
Standard Score; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire.

Using linear regression, comparison was made PRAG/AUT or ERRNI at T2. However, parent ratings
between the TAU and SCIP groups on the CELF-4 (PRO) showed significant differences by treatment
CLSS at T2 and T3 with adjustment for age and the group in favour of SCIP at T2.
CELF-4 CLSS at T1. No significant treatment effect Table 3 shows the results of secondary outcomes
was found at T2 (p = 0.78) or T3 (p = 0.87). The at T3. TOPICC, CCC-PRAG, PRO-SC and PRO-
estimated group difference at T2 was 0.5 (95% CI = SS, and TRO-CLS showed significant differences by
–3.1 to 4.1) and at T3 was 0.3 (95% CI = –3.7 to 4.4). treatment group in favour of SCIP at T3. However,
Given that a large proportion (36%) of participants no significant treatment effects were found for CCC-
were found to have CELF-4 CLSS well within the AUT, ERRNI, PRO-LS or PRO-PR at T3. Note that
normal range (CELF-4 CLSS > 80) at baseline, a further for PRO-PR numbers in the control group were too
exploratory comparison was made for the subgroup of small for analysis by logistic regression.
children who scored in the low ability/language impaired
range (CELF-4 CLSS < 80) at baseline. Conclusions
were similar: the estimated group differences (95% CI) Discussion
at T2 and T3 were 3.9 (–1.3 to 8.9) and 1.4 (–4.8 to This is the first randomized controlled trial investigating
7.6) respectively. the effectiveness of intervention for children who have
PLI. The first aim was to establish the effects of the
SCIP intervention compared with TAU on standardized
Secondary outcomes
language assessment.
Table 2 shows the results of secondary outcomes at T2. The primary outcome, a standardized measure of
No significant treatment effects were found for CCC- overall language performance (CELF-4 CLSS), did
240 Catherine Adams et al.
Table 2. Secondary outcomes at Time 2

SCIP TAU Effecta


n Mean (SD) n Mean (SD) Mean difference (95% CI) p
CCC-PRAG 49 30.9 (10.2) 21 29.4 (11.4) 0.7 (–3.3 to 4.6) 0.74
CCC-AUT 50 26.3 (10.8) 21 23.3 (8.9) 1.6 (–2.1 to 5.3) 0.39
ERRNI-I 57 90.9 (17.8) 28 90.9 (17.3) 0.31 (–6.9 to 7.5) 0.93
ERRNI-R 57 91.2 (20.2) 28 88.3 (21.2) 3.7 (–4.7 to 12) 0.38
ERRNI-C 57 89.1 (13.9) 28 87.1 (12.9) 2.4 (–3.3 to 8.1) 0.41
n (%) improved n (%) improved Odds ratio (95% CI)
PRO-LS 50 38 (76) 19 4 (21.1) 11.9 (3.3–42.7) < 0.001∗∗
PRO-SC 48 35 (72.9) 20 7 (35) 5 (1.6–15.2) 0.005∗∗
PRO-SS 47 27 (57.4) 21 3 (14.3) 8.7 (2.2–34.2) 0.002∗∗
PRO-PR b 40 20 (48.8) 18 1 (5.6)
Notes: a Mean difference by linear regression or odds ratio by logistic regression.
b
Numbers improved in the control group too small to compute odds ratio.
∗∗
p < 0.01.
Abbreviations are as given in table 1, except for PRO-LS/SC/SS/PR, Parent Reported Outcome—Language Skills/Social Communication/Behaviour in Social Situations/Peer Relation-
ships.

not show a significant intervention effect for SCIP plausible differences between groups. Inspection of the
compared with TAU, nor did a secondary standard- standard score increments and confidence intervals on
ized measure of narrative ability (ERRNI). The identifi- CELF-4 CLSS also indicate that very large changes in
cation of a single, predefined standardized outcome raw scores (larger than those considered clinically signifi-
measure, which was sensitive enough to capture change cant) would be required to show shift in standard scores.
in pragmatic impairment, although highly desirable, Despite CELF-4 CLSS being a recognized, reliable
was not easily achievable for this complex group. Over and objective test of general language function, some
one-third of participants recruited were found to be CwPLI in this study had functional difficulties with
performing well within the normal range on CELF-4 language processing that were evident to teachers and
CLSS (and ERRNI) at pre-intervention assessment. parents but which were not detected by standardized
A further exploratory comparison was therefore made language tests. Further, some CwPLI functioned in the
for the subgroup of children who scored in the low normal range across all language tasks and did not
ability/language impaired range (CELF-4 CLSS < 80) present concern to teachers or parents, except in the
at baseline. This comparison showed a trend in favour pragmatics and social communication domain. Further
of intervention, but had insufficient power to detect characterization of the PLI ‘population’ may therefore be

Table 3. Secondary outcomes at Time 3

SCIP TAU Effecta


n Mean (SD) n Mean (SD) Mean difference (95% CI) p
CCC-PRAG 39 27.9 (12.9) 16 33.5 (9.0) 5.5 (0.04–10.9) 0.049∗
CCC-AUT 41 24.3 (11.4) 16 24.1 (9.6) 0.13 (–4.8 to 5.1) 0.96
ERRNI-I 57 97.2 (14.8) 28 100.8 (13.8) 3.3 (–2.5 to 9.1) 0.27
ERRNI-R 57 93.7 (20.9) 28 93.5 (23.2) 0.58 (–8.7 to 9.9) 0.90
ERRNI-C 57 91.6 (14.5) 28 93.4 (15.5) 1.4 (–4.7 to 7.5) 0.64
n (%) improved n (%) improved Odds ratio (95% CI)
TOPICC 51 22 (43.1) 27 5 (18.5) 0.3 (0.1–0.9) 0.04∗
PRO-LS 38 26 (68.4) 16 7 (43.8) 2.9 (0.9–9.9) 0.09
PRO-SC 38 28 (73.7) 16 4 (25.0) 8.0 (2.1–31.1) 0.003∗∗
PRO-SS 38 24 (63.2) 15 3 (20.0) 7.4 (1.7–31.8) 0.007∗∗
PRO-PR 38 24 (63.2) 14 7 (50.0) 1.6 (0.5–5.7) 0.46
TRO-CLS 40 30 (75.0) 18 8 (44.4) 3.7 (1.2–12.1) 0.03∗
Notes: a Mean difference by linear regression or odds ratio by logistic regression.

p < 0.05; ∗∗ p < 0.01.
Abbreviations are as given in tables 1 and 2, except TRO-CLS, Teacher Reported Outcome—Classroom Learning Skills.
RCT of speech/language therapy for children with pragmatic language impairment 241
required to understand the true nature and occurrence measures were subject to bias and therefore need to
of underlying language impairments as well as develop- be interpreted with great caution. In this study we
ment of sensitive, valid and objective measures of high aimed to mitigate bias by extracting two sets of items,
level language outcomes. The content of CELF-4 CLSS CCC-PRAG (behaviours targeted in the intervention)
was also ultimately relatively far removed from the and CCC-AUT (not targeted in the intervention) from
intervention goals in SCIP which emerged for individu- CCC-2 assessments. The hypothesis that intervention
als over time. These tended to be at the levels of activity which targeted pragmatics should show an effect on
and participation, reflecting the priorities of parents CCC-PRAG only was confirmed. Parent report was able
and teachers whose input was solicited throughout the to differentiate effects for these two aspects of communi-
setting of goals and intervention period. cation.
The second aim was to explore any notable effects Differential effects across categories were also
of the SCIP intervention compared with TAU, on demonstrated within Parent Reported Outcomes, where
observed functional pragmatic ability and broader social findings were not universally in favour of interven-
communication. tion across individual items. Although careful attention
Significant intervention effects were found for SCIP was given to the method of reporting to attempt
intervention compared with TAU for overall conversa- to control halo effects, it is possible that parents in
tional quality (TOPICC) between baseline and 6- the intervention group were biased to report positive
month follow-up. This finding is important given that changes in aspects of intervention which they perceived
it was achieved using blind rater perceptions. It is as central to therapy, but, in practice, intervention
carefully noted, however, that approximately half of contained multiple integrated components and these
the intervention group were rated as not improved on were not explicitly labelled by therapists. Further,
this measure, indicating that only some CwPLI are exploration of outcome measures indicates that there
likely to be able to adapt conversational style with a is convergence in the types of functional communica-
short period of intervention. It would be important to tion changes observed by parents across measures (CCC-
identify, in further research, variables associated with PRAG/PRO) and good agreement between parent
potential for change in order to adopt differential CCC raters and blind TOPICC raters (Adams et al.
management strategies. TOPICC ratings were based on 2011).
blind perceptions of change between baseline and 6- Delivering a complex individualized speech–
month follow-up suggesting the intervention effect is language intervention to a relatively heterogeneous
maintained well beyond the end of intensive therapy for population, in the context of all the variables of real-
these children who did show change. life school environments was exceptionally challeng-
Significant intervention effects were also found for ing and there are several potential limitations and
SCIP intervention compared with TAU for non-blind possibilities for bias which have to be controlled. The
parent-reported social communication, social behaviour TAU group received less direct specialist intervention
and language skills immediately following intervention. than participants receiving SCIP (Adams et al. 2012)
These effects were maintained at 6-month follow-up for making it difficult to attribute intervention effects to the
social communication and behaviour in social situations. specific content of SCIP rather than increased specialist
Significant intervention effects were also found for non- attention. However, the TAU group continued to receive
blind teacher ratings of classroom learning skills at 6- SLT support via LSAs in a training/consultancy model
month follow-up (measured at T3 only). which is widely used in UK mainstream SLT services.
Non-blind parent-reported pragmatic functioning Thus, this was a fair comparison of the experimental
(CCC-PRAG) showed a significant intervention effect treatment to the care that would have been received
compared with TAU at 6-month follow-up, but not were the trial not to have taken place and therefore has
immediately following intervention. It might therefore ecological validity.
be that skills learned in an intensive period of therapy In this study, the developer of the SCIP intervention
take time to be consolidated in broader contexts, which and the TOPICC outcome measure is also the evaluator
would contribute to a possible ‘sleeper effect’ where the of the intervention in this trial. As such, a potential for
effects of treatment may be undetectable on immediate systematic bias related to conflict of interest may exist
post-test but become apparent on later testing. CCC- (Petrosino and Soydan 2005). However, the trial was
PRAG was composed of a diverse list of pragmatic rigorous with respect to blinding and pre-definition of
skills, not all of which could have been addressed in any outcome measures.
single individual’s intervention. It was not unexpected These findings highlight important implications for
therefore that this finding just achieved significance. future studies of interventions for complex developmen-
It is important to note that between-condition tal constructs such as PLI and SCD. This complexity
effects on non-blind parent and teacher-reported means that it is unlikely that there will be uniform
242 Catherine Adams et al.
outcome effects across participants due to differential indirect support). In future work it will be important to
profiles of impairment and rates of development (Koenig include estimates of cost-effectiveness and the resources
et al. 2009). Obtaining a balance in outcome measures required to support alternative models of delivery. It
between sensitivity to change in performance increments would be essential to look at the balance between a more
(where potential measureable outcomes are many) and intensive, mixed direct/indirect, specialist-led provision
meaningful functional communication change is a real which has the potential to produce outcomes desired by
challenge in intervention trials such as these. The service users and the more indirect SLT models which
TOPICC method of using blind rater perception of are generally provided over longer periods and whose
overall conversational quality holds promise as a way of potential for achieving desired outcomes is unknown.
capturing change across a complex population present- Although resources prevented the use of gold
ing with wide ranging impairments that impact on the standard autism diagnostic procedures such as the
quality of conversation in different ways. However, there Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord
is a need to develop measures which are specific and et al. 2000), there is an indication that our PLI group
sensitive to the perceived needs of the children from showed a substantial overlap with children with HF-
the service users’ perspective too. Therefore, parent or ASD/PDD and that SCIP may therefore also be a
teacher-reported outcomes may ultimately be a more relevant intervention in addressing social communi-
powerful primary outcome measure in a study such as cation skills in this group. We emphasize though
this. that SCIP intervention is aimed at optimizing social
communication, it does not aim to ‘cure’ communica-
tion impairments or change autistic symptomatology.
Conclusions and clinical implications
The findings are suggestive of changes in children’s
The overall conclusions are that it is likely that communication skills which are perceived as meaning-
the intervention provided in SCIP is effective at ful to those living and working with the children daily.
improving overall conversational quality (but not Such indicators are now seen as central to the aspiration
structural language skills) in 6–11 year olds who have of delivering services which are designed around the
significant pragmatic and social communication needs family (Department for Children, Schools and Families
compared with TAU. (DCSF) 2008).
SCIP is perceived by parents and teachers as effective
at improving some functional pragmatic and social
communication skills at home, and classroom learning Acknowledgements
skills, for these children. This study was funded by the Nuffield Foundation (Grant Reference:
The implications are that, with carefully targeted EDU/32953) and sponsored by the University of Manchester.
The authors gratefully thank the participating children, parents,
specialist intervention, there is potential for some change schools, and referring speech and language therapy services in the
in some school-aged children who have persistent North West of England and in the South East Scotland area. They
pragmatic and social communication needs, even with a acknowledge the cooperation of the Centre for Integrated Healthcare
brief period of speech and language therapy. The amount Research, Queen Margaret University, Edinburgh; and the support
of therapy offered was constrained by the experimen- and guidance from their study advisory committee (Sue Roulstone,
Bonnie Brinton, Martin Fujiki and Geoff Lindsay). Declaration of
tal model; in reality, some children may require longer interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors
periods of intervention to consolidate gains made in alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper. The
the intensive period of therapy. Changes in individual intervention described in this paper is currently in preparation for
children varied widely and a further stage of enquiry publication in book form by two of the authors (Adams and Gaile).
would be to investigate factors which contribute to this
finding.
These findings may provide some support for the
Notes
conclusions of a recently reported trial of interven-
tion for children with SLI (McCartney et al. 2011), 1. The term ‘PDD’ is likely to be subsumed under ASD in DSM-5;
which concluded that the effects of a specialist interven- therefore, it will not be used from this point. Note that it does
appear in the SCQ assessment used in this study. The proportion
tion can be lost whilst attempting to embed it within of children with historical ASD and PDD on this assessment will
a consultancy training model. In the present study’s be interpreted as all falling into a broader ASD definition (see
TAU condition, children received less direct interven- the Results).
tion but continued to receive SLT support from LSAs via 2. SCQ Lifetime scores are based on parent report of autism features
a consultancy model. Many children had received this which have occurred in the child’s lifetime; it is probable that the
behavioural and social profile of the child in the current study
support for a long period of time and continued to do will have changed to some degree over time, so no definitive
so. In the SCIP condition, direct therapy was intensively diagnosis of current autism functioning can be derived.
provided using a model of specialist SLT provision 3. Outcome data for all randomized participants were included in
supported by specially trained assistants (with additional analysis where available.
RCT of speech/language therapy for children with pragmatic language impairment 243
References BRINTON, B., FUJIKI, M. and ROBINSON, L., 2005, Life on a tricycle:
a case study of language impairment from 4 to 19. Topics in
ADAMS, C., 2001, Clinical diagnostic studies of children with Language Disorders, 25, 338–352.
semantic–pragmatic language disorder. International Journal DEPARTMENT FOR CHILDREN, SCHOOLS AND FAMILIES (DCSF),
of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 289–306. 2008, The Bercow Report: A Review of Services for Children and.
ADAMS, C. and GAILE, J., Forthcoming 2012, Managing Children’s Young People (0–19) with Speech, Language and Communica-
Pragmatic and Social Communication Needs in the Early School tion Needs (London: DCSF).
Years (Manchester: Roundway Centre Publ.) (available at: KJELGAARD, M. and TAGER-FLUSBERG, H., 2001, An investigation
http://www.roundwaycentre.org.uk/publications). of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic
ADAMS, C., LLOYD, J., ALDRED, C. and BAXENDALE, J., 2006, subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308.
Exploring the effects of communication intervention for KOENIG, K., DE LOS REYES, A., CHICHETTI, D., SCAHILL, L.
developmental pragmatic language impairments: a signal- and KLIN, A., 2009, Group interventions to promote social
generation study. International Journal of Language and skills in school-age children with Pervasive Developmen-
Communication Disorders, 41, 41–66. tal Disorder: reconsidering efficacy. Journal of Autism and
ADAMS, C., LOCKTON, E., GAILE, J. and FREED, J., 2011, Developmental Disorders, 39, 1163–1172.
TOPICCAL applications: assessment of children’s conversa- LANDA, R., 2000, Social language use in Asperger’s syndrome and
tion skills. Speech and Language Therapy in Practice, Spring, high-functioning autism. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar and S. S.
7–9. Sparrow (eds), Asperger Syndrome (New York, NY: Guildford),
ADAMS, C., LOCKTON, E., GAILE, J., EARL, G. and FREED, J., pp. 125–158.
Forthcoming 2012, The Social Communication Interven- LAW, J., GARRETT, Z. and NYE, C., 2003, Speech and language
tion Project: implementation of a manualised communica- therapy interventions for children with primary speech and
tion intervention for school-aged children with pragmatic language delay or disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic
and social communication needs in a randomised controlled Reviews, Issue 3, Art. No.: CD004110 (updated 2010
trial. International Journal of Language and Communication without revision).
Disorders. LEYFER, O. T., TAGER-FLUSBERG, H., DOWD, M., TOMBLIN, B. and
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION, n.d., Proposed Draft Revisions FOLSTEIN, S. E., 2008, Overlap between autism and specific
to DSM Criteria and Disorders—APA DSM-5 (available at: language impairment: comparison of Autism Diagnostic
http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions) (accessed on 20 Interview and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
June 2011). Scores. Autism Research, 1, 284–296.
BAIRD, G., SIMONOFF, E., PICKLES, A., CHANDLER, S., LOUCAS, LORD, C., RISI, S., LAMBRECHT, L., COOK, E. H., LEVENTHAL, B.
T., MELDRUM, D. and CHARMAN, T., 2006, Prevalence of L., DILAVORE, P. C., PICKLES, A. and RUTTER, M., 2000,
disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of ADOS—The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—
children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Generic: a standard measure of social and communication
Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368, 210–215. deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of
BERUMENT, S. K., RUTTER, M., LORD, C., PICKLES, A. and BAILEY, Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.
A., 1999, Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic validity. MACKIE, L. and LAW, J., 2010, Pragmatics and the child with
British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451. Emotional/Behavioural Difficulties (EBD): a pilot study
BISHOP, D. V. M., 1998, Development of the Children’s Communi- exploring the interaction between behaviour and communi-
cation Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative cation disability. International Journal of Language and
aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 45, 397–410.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–891. MCCARTNEY, E., BOYLE, J., ELLIS, S., BANNATYNE, S. and TURNBULL,
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2000, Pragmatic language impairment: a correlate M., 2011, Indirect language therapy for children with
of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part of the autistic continuum? persistent language impairment in mainstream primary
In D. V. M. Bishop and L. B. Leonard (eds), Speech and schools: outcomes from a cohort intervention. International
Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 46, 74–82.
Intervention and Outcomes (Hove: Psychology Press), pp. 99– PETROSINO, A. and SOYDAN, H., 2005, The impact of program
113. developers as evaluators on criminal recidivism: results
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2003, The Children’s Communication Checklist— from meta-analyses of experimental and quasi-experimental
Second Edition (Hove: Harcourt Assessment). research. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 1, 435–450.
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2004, Expression, Reception and Recall of Narrative RAVEN, J. C., 1979, Coloured Progressive Matrices (Oxford: Oxford
Instrument (ERRNI) (Hove: Harcourt Assessment). Psychologists Press).
BISHOP, D. V. M. and ADAMS, C., 1989, Conversational character- RUTTER, M., BAILEY, A. and LORD, C., 2003, The Social Communi-
istics of children with semantic–pragmatic disorder. II: What cation Questionnaire (Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
features lead to a judgement of inappropriacy? British Journal Services).
of Disorders of Communication, 24, 241–263. SEMEL, E., WIIG, E. H. and SECORD, W., 2006, Clinical Evaluation
BOTTING, N. and ADAMS, C., 2005, Inferential and semantic of Language Fundamentals—Fourth Edition UK (CELF-4 UK)
abilities in children with communication disorders. Interna- (Hove: Harcourt Assessment).
tional Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, TIMLER, G., OLSWANG, L. B. and COGGINS, T. E., 2005, ‘Do I know
49–66. what I need to do?’ A social communication intervention for
BOTTING, N. and CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 1999, Pragmatic language children with complex clinical profiles. Language, Speech, and
impairment without autism: the children in question. Autism, Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 73–85.
3, 371–396. WHITEHOUSE, A. J. O., WATT H. J., LINE, E. A. and BISHOP,
BOTTING, N., CRUTCHLEY A. and CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 1998, D. V. M., 2009, Adult psychosocial outcomes of children
Educational transitions of 7-year-old children with SLI in with specific language impairment, pragmatic language
language units: a longitudinal study. International Journal of impairment and autism. International Journal of Language
Language and Communication Disorders, 33, 177–197. and Communication Disorders, 44, 511–528.
244 Catherine Adams et al.
Appendix A
Table A1. Social Communication Behaviour Checklist (SCBC)
used for recruitment purposes only

Yes/no
The child has trouble understanding and interpreting the social context and friendship, e.g. social roles, emotions
The child has trouble understanding and/or using non-verbal aspects of communication, e.g. facial expression, intonation
The child has trouble with aspects of conversation, e.g. beginning and ending, taking turns, giving relevant and sufficient information
The child makes bizarre, tangential or inappropriate comments
The child has difficulty using and understanding non-literal language

Table A2. CELF-4 Core Language Scale subtests by age

Ages 5–8;11 Ages 9–12


Concepts and following directions Concepts and following directions
Word structure Recalling sentences
Recalling sentences Formulating sentences
Formulating sentences Word classes 2 (receptive and expressive)

Table A3. Children’s Communication Checklist-2 Pragmatics


(PRAG) and Autism-communication (AUT) subscales (derived
from Bishop 2003)

(a) CCC-2 PRAG


1 Gets confused when a word used with a different meaning
2 Includes over-precise information
3 It’s hard to make sense of what he is saying even though the words are clear
4 It’s difficult to stop him or her from talking
5 Tells people things they know already
6 Gets the sequence of events muddled up when trying to tell a story
7 Doesn’t explain what he is talking about to someone who doesn’t share their experiences
8 Can be hard to tell if she is talking about something real or make believe
9 Talks to people too readily
10 Takes in just one or two words in a sentence and so misinterprets
11 Moves the conversation to a favourite topic, even if others not interested
12 Talks repetitively about things that no one is interested in
13 Uses terms like ‘he’ or ‘it’ without making it clear what s/he is talking about
14a Talks to others about their interests rather than his own
15 When answering a question, provides enough information without being over precise
16 Realises the need to be polite
17 Talks about his friends, shows an interest in what they say and do
18 Keeps quiet in situations where someone else is trying to talk or concentrate
19 You can have an enjoyable, interesting conversation with her
20 Explains a past event clearly
Note: a Reversed polarity from this point.

También podría gustarte