Está en la página 1de 153

The Relationship between Irrational Beliefs and

Marital Satisfaction

By Karen L. Nickl

A Dissertation

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

O f the Requirements for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

In Educational Psychology

Northern Arizona University

May, 2006

Approved

/ j %aaax> ^ j i m
Ramona N. Mellott, PhD ., Chair

William E. Martin, Jr. Ed.D

. Moan, Ed.D

j
Frances J. Ridm6r,

O-C- (
Bob Tures, Ed.D

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
UMI Number: 3228561

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy
submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and
photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized
copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
UMI Microform 3228561
Copyright 2006 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest Information and Learning Company


300 North Zeeb Road
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Abstract

The Relationship between Irrational Beliefs and Marital Satisfaction

Karen L. Nickl

The purpose of this study was three fold: 1) Determine the reliability and factor

validity of two instruments designed to assess irrational relationship beliefs, The

Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI) and the Relationship Belief Scale (RBS). 2) Further

delineate the relationship between irrational relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction.

3) Investigate possible demographic factors that predict endorsing irrational relationship

beliefs.

The 297 non-clinical, married participants, from the Salt Lake City, Utah area

completed the RBI and RBS plus the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). Principle

Component Analysis was used to test the five factor structure of both instruments. An

exploratory factor analysis was also performed on the RBS. While the RBS was more

predictive o f Marital Satisfaction than the RBI, loadings achieved indicate variability in

the factor structure o f both instruments. The RBI subscales Disagreement is Destructive

and Mindreading is Expected combined to form a new component. The RBS subscales

all evidenced variability when 15 test items failed to load significantly on the target

subscales.

Both the RBI and RBS were found to predict endorsement of irrational

relationship beliefs for this sample.

A series o f stepwise multiple regression analysis were performed on the subscales

of the RBI and RBS plus the demographic factors of gender, age, years married, times

married, education, household income, position in family life cycle and parent’s divorce.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The shared variance in the significant models was minimal and findings from the

multiple regression analysis should be generalized to other populations with caution.

Demographic factors were not found to predict endorsement of irrational relationship

beliefs.

The sample proved to be a highly homogenous group, and was characterized as

religious, committed to marriage, well educated, upper middle class, and mature. The

majority (74%) of respondents identified themselves as members of The Church of Jesus

Christ o f Latter Day Saints (LDS). Although subscale and overall test score means

indicated no statistical differences between these participants and other samples, the

relevance and application of this study may be limited.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Acknowledgments

This project has exposed all my weaknesses so I heartily thank the members of

my committee for their efforts on my behalf and their contribution to this work. To

Dr. Ramona N. Mellott, I am fortunate to have been your student. I appreciate your

qualities o f wisdom, restraint, and resourcefulness. The example you provide me in both

personal and professional arenas is inspiring. Your guidance in the development and

execution of this project has been critical to its completion. Dr. William E. Martin, you

have taught with clarity and expertise. Your knowledge of statistics and research design

has been invaluable to me as I lean towards practitioner and away from scientist.

Dr. Eugene R. Moan, I recognize in you a shared interest in spirituality and healthy

families. Thank you for assigning the paper that first sparked my interest in this subject

matter. Dr. Frances J. Riemer, you have been an effective teacher and have brought a

fresh perspective to this work. I appreciate your willingness to invest in my

development. Dr. Bob Tures, you lead me with your optimism, expertise, and creative

deployment of our craft. I thank you all.

I would also like to thank my family. To my parents Richard and Jeanne, thank

you for sacrifices you made for my education and your example in the continuing pursuit

of knowledge. To my brothers and sister Doug, Brad, Richard, Steven and Barbara your

love, encouragement, and faith have been sustaining.

Thanks to my husband Mario for the investments you continue to make in my

growth and our marriage. For my children Tyler, Gina, Reagan, and Lauren, you simply

delight me.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table o f Contents

Abstract.....................................................................................................................................2

Acknowledgments....................................................................................................................4

Table of Contents.....................................................................................................................5

List of Tables............................................................................................................................ 9

List of Figures......................................................................................................................... 10

C hapter 1.................................................................................................................................11
Introduction..............................................................................................................................12
Predicting Marital Satisfaction.................................................................................. 12
Background and Contextual Factors............................................................. 12
The Couple’s Interaction Process................................................................. 13
Individual Traits and Behavior..................................................................... 13
Theoretical Orientation............................................................................................... 13
Instruments that Measure Irrational Beliefs............................................................. 14
Statement of the Problem...........................................................................................15
Purpose of the Study................................................................................................... 15
Significance of this Study.........................................................................................15
Research Questions................................................................................................... 17
Research Question One................................................................................ 17
Research Question Two............................................................................... 17
Research Question Three............................................................................. 17
Research Question Four...............................................................................18
Research Question Five............................................................................... 18
Research Question Six................................................................................. 18
Research Question Seven (a) and (b).........................................................19
Variables and Definitions of Terms....................................................................... 20
Marriage....................................................................................................... 20
Irrational Relationship Beliefs................................................................... 20
Marital Satisfaction..................................................................................... 21
Gender.......................................................................................................... 21
Education Level.......................................................................................... 21
Household Income...................................................................................... 21
Years Married..............................................................................................22
Position in Family Life Cycle.................................................................... 22
Parents’ Divorce.......................................................................................... 23
Limitations and Delimitations................................................................................23
Delimitations...............................................................................................23
Limitations..................................................................................................23
Summary..................................................................................................................23

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
C hapter II ........................................................................................................................... 25
Review of the Literature.....................................................................................................25
Theoretical Orientation.......................................................................................... 25
Irrational Thoughts and Mental Health.................................................... 25
Irrational Beliefs in the Context of Intimate Relationships................... 25
What Make a Belief “Irrational”? ............................................................25
Types of Irrational Beliefs and Unrealistic Expectations.......................27
The Relationship between Irrational Beliefs and Marital Satisfaction.............. 30
People Who Endorse Irrational Beliefs................................................................. 33
Gender......................................................................................................... 33
Age.............................................................................................................. 34
Education.....................................................................................................35
Position in Family Life Cycle.................................................................... 35
Household Income...................................................................................... 36
Years Married............................................................................................. 36
Times Married............................................................................................ 37
Parents’ Divorce......................................................................................... 37
Religion....................................................................................................... 37
Measuring Irrational Relationship Beliefs............................................................37
The Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI)................................................. 39
The Relationship Belief Scale (RBS)....................................................... 40
Summary o f Chapter Two...................................................................................... 40

C hapter I I I .......................................................................................................................... 42
Methodology.......................................................................................................................... 42
Research Design....................................................................................................... 42
Target Population and Accessible Population....................................................... 42
Selecting the Sample.................................................................................................43
Description o f the Sample........................................................................................ 45
Sample Source.............................................................................................. 45
Age and Gender............................................................................................ 45
Education....................................................................................................... 43
Position in Family Life Cycle...................................................................... 46
Household Income........................................................................................ 47
Years Married................................................................................................47
Times Married.............................................................................................. 47
Parents’ Divorce.......................................................................................... 47
Religion......................................................................................................... 47
Instrumentation.........................................................................................................47
Demographic Item Questionnaire (DIQ)................................................... 48
Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS)................................................................48
Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI)..........................................................50
Relationship Belief Scale (RBS)................................................................53
Statistical Procedures...............................................................................................53
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question One........................53

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Two...................... 54
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Three.................... 54
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Four.......................55
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Five.......................55
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Six.........................56
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Seven.................... 56
Summary o f Chapter Three........................................................................................ 57

C hapter I V ............................................................................................................................. 58
Results......................................................................................................................................58
Data Screening Procedures........................................................................................ 58
Data Entry...................................................................................................... 58
Missing Data..................................................................................................58
Univariate Underlying Assumptions........................................................... 60
Multivariate Outliers.....................................................................................62
Summary o f Data Screening.....................................................................................63
Restatement of the Problem...................................................................................... 63
Analysis of the Statistical Results............................................................................ 64
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question One...........................64
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Two..........................65
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Three........................81
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Four......................... 93
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Five..........................98
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Six (a).....................100
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Six (b).....................102
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Seven (a)................ 104
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Seven (b)................105
Summary of Results..................................................................................................107
Results of Research Question One.............................................................. 107
Results of Research Question Two.............................................................108
Results o f Research Question Three...........................................................108
Results o f Research Question Four............................................................. 108
Results of Research Question Five............................................................. 109
Results o f Research Question Six (a)..........................................................109
Results o f Research Question Six (b).........................................................110
Results of Research Question Seven (a).................................................... 110
Results o f Research Question Seven (b).................................................... I l l

C hapter V ............................................................................................................................ 112


Discussion...............................................................................................................................112
Introduction................................................................................................................112
Population and Sample.............................................................................................113
Research Question One.............................................................................................115
Research Question Two............................................................................................116
Research Question Three..........................................................................................119
Research Question Four............................................................................................121

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Research Question Five............................................................................................122
Research Question Six..............................................................................................123
Research Question Seven (a)................................................................................... 124
Research Question Seven (b)................................................................................... 125
Strengths.................................................................................................................... 125
Demographic Factors................................................................................... 126
Large Sample Size....................................................................................... 126
Limitations.................................................................................................................126
Assumptions of Normality..........................................................................126
The Unique and Highly Homogenous Sample.......................................... 127
The LDS Population.................................................................................... 128
Non-Random Sample.................................................................................. 128
Possible Influence of Treatment................................................................ 128
Implications for Practice...........................................................................................129
Clinical Use of the RBI and RBS............................................................... 129
Irrational Relationship Beliefs and Marital Satisfaction.......................... 129
Demographic Factors.................................................................................. 129
Specific Value for Therapists serving LDS Couples................................129
Implications for Further Research..........................................................................130
Summary o f Chapter Five........................................................................................131

R eferences............................................................................................................................ 133

Appendix A NAU Letter from Institutional Review Board........................................... 141

Appendix B Letter of Informed Consent.........................................................................143

Appendix C Letter from The Institute of Marriage and Family Counseling.............. 145
Expressing support and permission to poll lecture audiences.

Appendix D Email Letter from Norman Epstein, Ph.D............................................... 147


Granting permission to use the Relationship Belief Inventory

Appendix E Email Letter from Bryce F. Sullivan Ph.D.............................................. 149


Granting permission to use the Relationship Belief Scale

Appendix F Demographic Information Questionnaire (DIQ)....................................151

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
List of Tables

Table 1 Participant Source Breakdown by Gender...................................................46

Table 2 RBI Mean, Standard Deviation and Pearson r with RBS Total.................. 66

Table 3 RBS Mean, Standard Deviation and Pearson r with RBI Total.................. 68

Table 4 Comparison o fM and SD o f RBI, RBS, and DAS Scores.............................70

Table 5 Correlation Matrix o f the Subscale and Total Scores o f the....................... 71


RBI and RBS

Table 6 RBI Principle Component Analyses - Pattern Matrix................................. 76

Table 7 Principle Component Analysis, Loadings o f RBI Subscale Items...............79

Table 8 Results o f the RBS Principle Component Analysis - Pattern M atrix 86

Table 9 Results o f the RBS Factor Analysis - Pattern Matrix.................................. 90

T able 10 RBS Loadings from Sullivan’s Study, Factor Analysis and.........................94


Principle Component Analysis.

Table 11 Correlation Matrix fo r RBI Subscales and DAS Total Score...................... 97

Table 12 Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis fo r .............................. 98


RBI subscales and Dyadic Adjustment

Table 13 Pearson r Correlation Matrix fo r RBS Subscales........................................ 99


and DAS Total Score

Table 14 Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis,................................. 101


RBS subscales and Dyadic Adjustment.

Table 15 Demographic Factors and Pearson r Correlations................................... 102


fo r RBI and RBS Total Score

Table 16 Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis.................................. 103


RBI Subscales, Demographic Factors and Dyadic Adjustment

Table 17 Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis................................. 106


RBS Subscales, Demographic Factors and Dyadic Adjustment

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
List of Figures

Figure One - Scree Plot fo r Principle Component Analysis o f RBI.............................. 75

Figure Two - Scree Plot o f Principle Component Analysis o f RBS.............................. 84

Figure Three - Scree Plot fo r Exploratory Factor Analysis o f RBS.............................. 89

10

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER I

Introduction

The marriage relationship is one of the most fundamental of all human

relationships. It is the primary means of establishing familial connection and is the best

means of rearing children (Stahmann & Hiebert, 1987). In 2004, 2,211,000 couples

married (Department of Health and Human Services, 2005). It is predicted that 43% of

these marriages will break up within 15 years (National Center of Health Statistics,

2005). Tremendous emotional and monetary expense is attached to these statistics. On

the Social Readjustment Scale (Holmes & Rahe, 1967) divorce and marital separation

rank second and third, respectively, of the most stressful of all life events. Marital

distress has been linked with decreased work productivity (Forthofer, Markman, Cox,

Stanley & Kessler, 1996) and increased mental and physical problems (Coie et al., 1993).

Children whose parents divorce are seriously affected too. These children are

more likely to drop out of school, become pregnant in their teens, abuse drugs, and break

the law. In addition, they have more trouble getting along with their parents and seek

more psychological services (Harvey & Fine, 2004). Like their parents, they suffer from

increased mental and physical problems (Fincham, Grych, & Osborne, 1993).

O f the marriages that remain intact, many of them are unhappy and characterized

by high levels o f negative interaction (Weiss & Heyman, 1990) or abuse (Vivian &

O’Leary, 1990). Unhappy marriages have been linked to mental health risks.

Specifically, marital distress is linked to higher occurrences o f depression in adults and to

conduct disorders for children who live in these families (Coie et al., 1991). Therefore

there is great interest in predicting the quality of marriages.

11

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Predicting Marital Satisfaction

Historically, researchers and marriage therapists have looked at factors that

predict marriage success and those that predict divorce (Cate & Loyd, 1992; Gottman,

1994; Larson & Holman, 1994; Lewis & Spanier, 1979; Wambolt & Reiss, 1989). These

factors fall into the following three categories.

Background and Contextual Factors

There are many factors about one’s background that may predict marital

satisfaction. For instance, a person is more likely to be happily married if their parents

did not divorce (McLanahan & Bumpass, 1988). There are other sociocultural factors

that predict marital satisfaction. Getting married when you are still a teenager is highly

predictive o f poor marital quality (Booth & Edwards, 1985; Martin & Bumpass, 1989).

Other socioeconomic factors like income and level of education at marriage have been

found to join together to predict marital stability (Kurdek, 1991, 1993; Martin &

Bumpass, 1989). Men, who attended college prior to marriage and never experienced

unemployment, tend to stay in their marriages longer than men who did not (Bahr &

Galligan, 1984). Having the support and influence of friends who approve of your choice

to marry also can predict satisfaction (Kurdek, 1991).

The Couple’s Interaction Process

This prediction category includes such factors as homogamy, interpersonal

similarity, and interactional history. Homogamy refers to the theory that marriage

satisfaction tends to occur in marriages where the spouses have similar cultural, religious,

education, intellectual and socio-economic backgrounds (Burr, 1973). Interpersonal

similarity is also predictive o f marital success. People who share core values, beliefs, and

12

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
attitudes tend to have more marital quality and more stability (Fowers & Olsen, 1986;

Kurdek, 1993).

Interactional history and process includes factors such a length of courtship,

cohabitation, premarital pregnancy, and communication styles. Longer courtships tend to

predict more satisfaction partly because major differences can be ferreted out and leave

fewer surprises for the marriage (Kurdek, 1991, 1993). Living together before marriage

is linked to poor marital satisfaction (Janus & Janus, 1993; Stanley & Markman, 1997).

Premarital pregnancy is highly predictive of poor marital satisfaction because of the

added stress it puts on the relationship (Manning, 1993). Other studies have concentrated

on a couple’s communication skills. Those couples with more skills tend to have better

marital satisfaction (Gottman, 1994, Wamboldt & Reiss 1989).

Individual Traits and Behavior

There are several intrapersonal characteristics that are highly predictive of poor

marital quality. Poor mental health (Beach & O ’Leary, 1993), impulsivity (Kelly &

Conley, 1987), and poor self-concept (Lewis & Spanier, 1979) are all highly predictive of

poor marital quality. The final factor and focus of this study, is that of dysfunctional or

irrational beliefs about marriage and the impact these beliefs have on marriage

satisfaction. (Baucom & Epstein, 1990; Eidelson & Epstein, 1982; Larson, 1992;

Kurdek, 1993; Schwebel & Sullivan, 1996).

Theoretical Orientation

Cognitive behavioral psychology arose from the observation that people can have

faulty, erroneous, or irrational beliefs and that these beliefs can negatively affect the way

in which people function (Beck & Emery, 1985; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979;

13

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Ellis, 1994; Meichenbaum, 1977). Specifically, in the context of marriage, there are

irrational beliefs about intimate relationships that contribute to disappointment and

distress in marriages and may cause people to interact in non-productive ways (Baucom

& Epstein, 1990; Eidelson & Epstein, 1982; Epstein & Eidelson, 1981, Larson 1992;

Schwebel & Fine, 1994; Schwebel & Sullivan, 1996). Chapter Two contains a detailed

explanation o f irrational relationship beliefs. The term “irrational belief’ will be further

defined and several irrational relationship beliefs will be identified.

Instruments that Measure Irrational Beliefs

The topic of irrational relationship beliefs has generated much interest especially

among scientists and practitioners that follow a cognitive behavioral therapeutic approach

(Baucom et al., 1989; Doherty, 1997; Eidelson & Epstein 1982; Schwebel & Fine, 1994;

Sullivan, 1997). In turn, this interest generated the need to accurately assess these beliefs

and the development of instruments to do so.

The first instrument designed to assess irrational thoughts was the Irrational

Beliefs Test (IBT, Jones, 1968). It was designed in accordance with Albert Ellis’ work

and the irrational beliefs he noticed in his clients. It was general in nature and not

specific to marriage or relationships.

The Relationship Belief Inventory was the first instrument created specifically to

assess those irrational beliefs that exist within committed intimate relationships (Eidelson

& Epstein, 1982). According to the test authors, the original development study found it

to have good psychometric properties. It has been researched, written about, and used as

an assessment instrument in many studies that have investigated relationship beliefs.

Although the RBI is the most commonly used test used to assess marital beliefs, some

14

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
authors have cast doubt upon the internal consistency and divergent validity of this

measure (Emmelkamp et al., 1987; James et al, 2002; Sullivan, 1997).

For his doctoral dissertation, Sullivan (1997) designed a new instrument that

measures irrational relationship beliefs—the Relationship Belief Scale (RBS). Initial

study results show the RBS to have good psychometric properties and much potential for

use in research. However, this new instrument has not been used in any published study.

Further investigation is needed regarding the internal consistency and scale validity of the

RBS.

Statement o f the Problem

Millions of couples divorce every year. Most divorces are characterized by

disappointment and marital distress, some of which is due to unrealistic or irrational

expectations of the marriage relationship. Some of this distress may be minimized by

normalizing the conflict in marriage and helping clients to adopt more realistic or rational

expectations regarding this intimate human relationship.

Some studies have looked at dispelling irrational beliefs as an intervention for

premarital counseling (Doherty 1997; Holman, Larson, & Harmer, 1994; Larson, 1992;

Larson & Holman, 1994). Still others have used the assessment o f irrational beliefs as a

prediction tool for marital distress and divorce (Crohan, 1992; Kurdek, 1993; Markman

& Hahlweg, 1993). However, very little research exists regarding irrational beliefs in

intact marriages. This study investigated irrational beliefs in the context o f non-clinical

married people.

Furthermore, this study sought to determine significant demographic features of

married individuals that endorse irrational beliefs. Previous studies have been

15

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
inconclusive regarding the demographic factors that predict endorsing irrational

relationship beliefs.

Clinicians have traditionally used the Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI,

Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) to assess irrational relationship thoughts. Recently, a new

instrument has become available —The Relationship Belief Scale (RBS, Sullivan, 1997).

Both instruments need further study to determine the psychometric strengths of each.

The problems addressed in this study are threefold, (a) A new instrument that

assesses irrational beliefs requires further investigation and comparison to the currently

existing instrument, (b) The relationship between irrational relationship beliefs and

marital satisfaction needs to be determined, (c) Little is known concerning the

demographic make up of people who endorse irrational relationship beliefs.

Purpose o f this Study

The purpose of this study was to explore the psychometric indices of a new

instrument designed to measure irrational beliefs, and to compare this to an existing

instrument. Furthermore, this study seeks to establish the connection between irrational

relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction with a new sample. Also, the relationship

between the demographic factors of age, gender, years married, times married, household

income, education, religion, position in the family life cycle and parent’s divorce will be

investigated to determine their relationship with endorsing irrational relationship beliefs.

Significance o f this Study

This study explored the validity and reliability of an existing instrument, the RBI,

that measures irrational relationship beliefs and a new instrument, the RBS. This study

provided insight into the people who endorse irrational relationship beliefs and certain

16

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
demographic factors were examined for their connection to these people. It builds on

previous research that has indicated that irrational beliefs predict poor marital adjustment.

Information from this study may be used to guide future research of irrational

relationship beliefs. Psychologists, counselors and educators may use the results to help

design interventions that increase marital satisfaction and decrease the rate of divorce.

Research Questions

Research Question One

What will be the relationships comparing the scales of the RBI and the RBS?

Null hypothesis. There will be no significant relationships comparing the RBI and

RBS scales.

Alternative hypothesis. There will be moderate and significant relationships

comparing the RBI and the RBS scales.

Research Question Two

What will be the factor structure and internal consistency o f the Relationship

Belief Inventory?

Null Hypothesis. There will not be a five factor structure that emerges from the

factor analysis performed on the Relationship Belief Inventory.

Alternative hypothesis. There will a five factor structure that emerges from the

factor analysis performed on the Relationship Belief Inventory.

Research Question Three

What will be the factor structure and internal consistency o f the Relationship

Belief Scale?

Null hypothesis. There will not be a five factor structure that emerges from the

17

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
factor analysis performed on the Relationship Belief Scale.

Alternative hypothesis. There will a five factor structure that emerges from the

factor analysis performed on the Relationship Belief Scale.

Research Question Four

To what extent do irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the Relationship

Belief Inventory, predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment

Scale?

Null hypothesis. Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the RBI, will not

predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale.

Alternative hypothesis. Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the RBI,

will predict marriage satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale.

Research Question Five

To what extent do irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the Relationship

Belief Scale, predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale?

Null hypothesis. Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the RBS, will not

predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale.

Alternative hypothesis. Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the RBS,

will predict marriage satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale.

Research Question Six

To what extent do the demographic variables of gender, age, education level,

years married, times married, household income, religion, position in family life cycle,

and parent’s divorce predict endorsement of irrational beliefs, as measured by the total

scores of a) the Relationship Belief Inventory and b) the Relationship Belief Scale?

18

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Null hypothesis. The demographic variables of gender, age, education level,

years married, times married, household income, religion, position in the family life

cycle, and parent’s divorce will not predict endorsement of irrational relationship beliefs,

as measured by the total scores of the a) Relationship Belief Inventory and b) the

Relationship Belief Scale.

Alternative hypothesis. The demographic variables of gender, age, years married,

times married, education level, household income, religion, position in the family life

cycle, and parent’s divorce will predict endorsement of irrational relationship beliefs, as

measured by the total scores of a) the Relationship Belief Inventory and b) the

Relationship Belief Scale.

Research Question Seven (a) and (b)

Which combinations o f demographic factors and irrational beliefs, as measured

by a) the Relationship Belief Inventory and b) the Relationship Belief Scale are

predictive of marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale?

Null hypothesis. There are no combinations of demographic factors and

irrational beliefs, as measured by the subscales on the Relationship Belief Inventory and

the Relationship Belief Scale that predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic

Adjustment Scale.

Alternative hypothesis. There are combinations of demographic factors and

irrational beliefs, as measured by the subscales on the Relationship Belief Inventory and

the Relationship Belief Scale, that predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic

Adjustment Scale.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Variables and Definitions o f Terms

The following terms are used in the study and are defined as follows.

Marriage

For the purposes of this study, marriage is defined as a legal union between a man

and a woman. It does not include same-sex unions, unmarried, or cohabitating couples.

Irrational Relationship Beliefs

Attitudes about marriage, that are not empirically proven or which are

maladaptive, are considered irrational. Specific to this study were the five irrational

belief subscale variables obtained by responses to the Relationship Belief Inventory

(Eidelson & Epstein, 1982).

Disagreement is Destructive', An attitude that contention will threaten the

relationship.

Mindreading is Expected; An attitude that good spouses know the mind of their

partners without it being communicated vocally.

Partners Cannot Change; An attitude that partners have no capacity for growth or

change.

Sexual Perfectionism; An attitude that good marriages are characterized by perfect

sexual performance.

The Sexes are Different; An attitude that ascribes a partner’s negative

characteristics to their gender.

There were another five irrational belief subscales obtained by responses to the

Relationship Belief Scale (Sullivan, 1997).

Vigilance; An attitude o f fear which requires constantly monitoring the partner’s

20

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
thoughts and actions to keep the partner in line.

Blame and Punishment, An attitude that partners are, or have the potential to be

bad and cause trouble in the relationship.

All or Nothing Thinking; An attitude that events can be described in black and

white terms.

Perfectionistic Expectations', An attitude that relationships and partners be perfect.

Demandfor Attention', An attitude that a partner is expected to fulfill all o f one’s

needs.

Marital Satisfaction

Individuals experience their marriage relationship as providing them with levels

of pleasure or reward. For this study, marital satisfaction was measured by the composite

score on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976).

Gender

Participants indicated their gender by checking the appropriate box of a

demographic questionnaire, male or female.

Education Level

Education level was determined by participants indicating how many years of

schooling they have attended. Four checkmark categories were provided, one each for

high school, some college, college graduate, and post graduate college.

Household Income

Household income was determined by participants indicating by checkmark an

income category that included their household income.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Years Married

A blank space was provided for the participant to write the number of years they

have been married.

Religion

Participants indicated by checking off one of several identified religious

categories, including LDS, Catholic, Jewish, Protestant, No Religion and one blank for

“other” where participants wrote in their religion.

Position in Family Life Cycle

There are six developmental stages to the life of a family as identified by Carter &

McGoldrick (1989). The six stages are; (a) Beginning Family, characterized by a couple

being married but not having had their first child yet, (b) Infant Preschool Family,

characterized by a married couple whose oldest child is under 5, (c) School-age Family,

where a couple’s oldest child is under 13, (d) Adolescent Family, where the oldest child

is under 18, (e) Launching Family in which the oldest child is over 18 but under 25 and

(f) Post Parental Family wherein all the children have left home.

Participants indicated which family life cycle stage they are in by filling in a

blank with the age o f their or their spouse’s oldest child or indicating that they had no

children. An additional category was added to this study of Mature—No kids, to

differentiate these participants from young couples in the Beginning Family category

who had not yet started their families. For the purposes of this study people who

indicated that their oldest child was over 25 were considered to be in the Post Parental

Family even though they may have other children still at home.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Parents' Divorce

Participants indicated if their own parents’ marriage had ended in divorce by

answering a question and checking the appropriate box. They were also asked to write in

at what age they were when the divorce occurred.

Limitations and Delimitations

The following factors may influence the external validity (delimitations) or

internal validity (limitations) o f the study.

Delimitations

Marital status. This study considers only heterosexual couples in legal marriages.

Results of this study may not generalize to engaged couples, couples in same-sex unions,

or to heterosexual couples in cohabitating situations.

Geographic Location. Participants for this study were drawn from the greater

Salt Lake City, Utah area. In the sample, 219 participants (74%) were affiliated with the

dominant LDS religion. Those adhering to this conservative religion may have unique

attitudes regarding marriage and divorce that would prevent findings from this study to

generalize outside of Utah.

Limitations

Subject selection. Most of the people participating in this study attended psycho

educational lectures offered in the metropolitan Salt Lake City area. People attending

these lectures may not represent the population at large.

Summary

Marital distress and the effects of divorce are a significant societal problem. This

chapter introduced the general topic of predicting marital distress. The discussion then

23

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
narrowed to one of many factors that has been shown to predict marital distress—that of

endorsing irrational relationship beliefs. A theoretical orientation was provided for the

topic of irrational relationship beliefs.

There was also a discussion of the two significant instruments used to measure

irrational relationship beliefs. The purpose and significance of the study was stated and

the research questions were identified. All o f the variables in the study were defined and

the limitations and delimitations were discussed.

24

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER II

Review of the Literature

The following section will present the underlying themes of this study—irrational

relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction. Seminal theoretical pieces will be presented,

as well as study results from the literature. Rationale for inclusion in the study will be

presented for each o f the nine demographic factors; gender, age, education, years

married, times married, family life cycle position, household income, religion and

parent’s divorce. Finally, the development, and psychometric strengths of each of the

research instruments will be addressed.

Theoretical Orientation

Irrational Thoughts and Mental Health

Albert Ellis (1994) was the first to identify thoughts as “irrational”. As a

psychoanalytic therapist he noticed that his clients held irrational or maladaptive beliefs

regarding how life ought to be. His clients were unhappy and anxious because their

experiences didn’t align with these irrational beliefs. Ellis identified eleven irrational

beliefs that were common to his clients. In therapy, he helped his clients see the

irrational nature of their thoughts and to substitute a rational alternative in its place. He

found that when his clients changed to the rational belief, they experienced a decrease in

anxiety and other negative symptoms.

Beck (Beck & Emery, 1985; Beck, Rush, Shaw & Emery, 1979) also made a

connection with his depressed clients and their illogical thoughts. He discovered that his

clients’ schema was a product o f early learning and that stressful life events brought

about dependency upon the illogical schemata. This led his clients to jump to irrational

25

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
conclusions and to make faulty choices based on the illogical beliefs.

Meichenbaum (1977), another cognitive behavioral scientist, developed Self

Instructional Training. This form of therapy taught clients about the power of their

cognitions. It helped them to identify metacognitive patterns and to recognize the part

they played in their problems. Once identified, the client was taught to replace the

problematic thoughts and patterns with new ones and behaviors that were more health

promoting.

Irrational Beliefs in the Context o f Intimate Relationships

Authors and clinicians began applying cognitive behavioral therapy to the context

of relationships (Ellis & Harper, 1961). Albert Ellis said, “Disturbed marital and family

relationships stem not so much from what happens among family members as from the

perceptions that these members have and the views they take of these happenings.”

What Makes a Belief ‘‘Irrational”?

Cognitions regarding marriage and relationships vary regarding their

appropriateness, validity, and reasonableness (Baucom et al., 1989). Irrationality refers

to thoughts that prevent people from accomplishing their basic human goals and are

inconsistent with empirical reality (Dryden & Digiuseppe, 1990). For instance, many

people believe in the notion of a soulmate (Larson, 1992). This cognition maintains that

for every person there is one other person best suited to provide their maximum joy,

happiness, and fulfillment. There is little empirical evidence to suggest that soulmates

exist. Therefore, belief in a soulmate is irrational (Larson, 1992).

In contrast, rationality is defined as that which helps people to achieve their goals

(Kurdek, 1993). Rational thought is logical and is consistent with empirical evidence.

26

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
With regard to the theory of a soulmate it is rational to believe that there are many people

with whom you might develop an intimate and rewarding relationship (Larson, 1992).

Another term that is often used to describe these cognitions in literature is

dysfunctional beliefs. “Dysfunctional beliefs...can be regarded as relatively enduring

predispositions that bias a person toward filtering, processing, and appraising marital

events in a dysfunctional manner” (Kurdek, 1993 p.238).

Some authors refer to these cognitions as unrealistic beliefs (Larson, 1992).

Authors Frank and Frank (1990) have made the differentiation between high but realistic

expectations and high but unrealistic expectations. An example o f a high expectation

would be —wanting your spouse to anticipate your mood from your non-verbal behavior.

After many years o f interacting, discourse and sufficient motivation, your spouse may

achieve this skill. This is a high, but realistic expectation. To expect this within months

o f the marriage date is a high and unrealistic expectation that may lead to disappointment.

Types o f Irrational Beliefs and Unrealistic Expectations

A recent survey (Schwebel, 1992) asked 100 young adults what it would take for

them to stay in a one year old marriage. The results were characterized by extremely high

expectations of marriage. On a scale that ranged from 0 (lowest) and 100 (highest) the

responses had the following means: “ amount of joy and laughter 81.7; decision-making

skills as a couple, 85.7; conflict resolution skills as a couple 86.0; communication quality,

86.5; level o f physical intimacy, 88.4; level o f psychological intimacy, 83.9; and level of

love, 90.2.” The author concluded that students with unrealistic, high expectations would

meet marriage with disappointment and distress (Schwebel, 1992). Similar results from

another study (Kudek, 1991), suggest that the level o f expectation in marriage is directly

27

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
proportional to the level o f disappointment.

In the development o f the Relationship Belief Inventory, Eidelson and Epstein

(1982) polled 20 marriage therapists to identify beliefs about intimate relationships that

seemed to cause the most difficulty for their clients. This generated 128 items that

appeared to fall into 5 categories or themes. 1) Disagreement is Destructive; An attitude

that keeps couples from discussing their differences and learning problem solving

strategies. 2) Mindreading is Expected; A belief that keeps couples from using dialogue

as a means of communication and prevents them from improving their skills in this area.

3) Partners cannot change; An attitude that encourages people to leave relationships

prematurely or to not engage in behavior that might resolve conflicts. 4) Sexual

Perfectionism; A belief characterized by a preoccupation with performance during sex. 5)

The Sexes are Different; Despite the popularity o f this belief, adhering to it perpetrates

the notion that men are from Mars and women are from Venus (Grey, 1992). It prevents

people from seeing the unique qualities of their partner and making changes that enhance

marital satisfaction.

By searching the existing literature, Larson (1992) identified several unrealistic

beliefs prevalent in young adults when they anticipated their future marriages. Eight of

them apply to this discussion. They are: 1) The One and Only Belief; A belief that there

is one right person for each person to marry. 2) The Perfect Partner Belief; A belief that

places unrealistic expectations on one’s partner. 3) The Perfect Self Belief; A belief that

one must be perfect to marry and engage in romantic relationships. In reality, imposed

perfection can be a roadblock to intimacy. 4) The Perfect Relationship Belief; this is the

attitude that flaws or weaknesses in the relationship are indications that the relationship

28

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
should be ended. It keeps individuals from working on relationships or on individual

change. 5) The Try Harder Belief; A belief opposite of the Perfect Partner Belief. It is

an attitude that one individual can build a quality marriage by themselves. Realistically,

it requires two committed and invested partners. 6) The Love is Enough Belief; A belief

that attachment, erotic interest, or infatuation is sufficient to bind a couple together. 7)

The Cohabitation Belief; A belief that requires couples to test the relationship by living

together prior to marriage. 8) The Opposites Complement Each Other Belief; A belief

that prevents people from changing behaviors. Realistically, the most satisfaction is

found in couples where great similarities exist.

When Sullivan (1997) performed a factor analysis on the 42 irrational belief items

for the Relationship Belief Scale, five dominant factors emerged. They were: 1)

Vigilance; An attitude o f constantly monitoring one’s partner’s thoughts and actions to

keep the partner in line. 2) Blame and Punishment; An attitude that partners are, or have

the potential to be bad and cause trouble in the relationship. 3) All or Nothing Thinking;

An attitude o f describing events in extreme, black and white terms. 4) Perfectionistic

Expectations; An attitude that relationships and partners should be perfect. 5) Demand

for Attention; An attitude that your partner should fulfill your needs.

Several authors have identified thoughts or attitudes about relationships that

appear to be irrational and therefore at odds with the reality o f relationships. Holding

these attitudes is not consistent with the goal of having a satisfying relationship.

Therefore the next item to examine is the link between endorsing irrational relationship

beliefs and marital satisfaction.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The Relationship between Irrational Beliefs and Marital Satisfaction

Schwebel and Sullivan (1996) state that people come to marriage with a

“marriage dream”. This is a collection of cognitions regarding a successful marriage.

Smith and Schwebel refer to this as the fam ily schema (1998). This collection of

thoughts and attitudes about marriage is socially constructed (Bandura, 1977) over the

course of one’s life and represent all of the assumptions, standards, attributions, and

expectancies that one has regarding marriage (Baucom et al. 1989). They also dictate

how couples will react to one another and to conflict and disappointment in the

relationship (Schwebel & Fine, 1994). Schwebel and Sullivan (1996) point out that all

marriages will eventually experience a loss of the marriage dream as the dream is

tempered by real life, but for couples with unrealistic or irrational cognitions, the loss

leads to higher levels o f marital distress.

Several studies have indicated a negative correlation between irrational

relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction. In a study used to established the norms and

validity o f the Relationship Belief Inventory, Eidelson and Epstein, (1982) found a

significant correlation between people’s endorsement of irrational relationship beliefs and

marital adjustment. In this study 100 couples responded to an instrument that measures

marital satisfaction and another that measures irrational belief. There was significant

negative correlation between the endorsement of irrational beliefs and their expressed

marital adjustment. Those in the sample that were taken from a clinical population were

also given another measure designed to measure one’s attitude about the outcome of

therapy. In this clinical population, those who endorsed irrational relationship beliefs

were more likely to predict a poor outcome for therapy with their spouse and expressed a

30

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
desire for individual therapy instead of couple’s therapy.

A study done in Holland (Emmelkamp, Krol, Sanderman & Ruphan,1987)

consisted of two samples. The first was from a random sample of Dutch couples (414

couples) and the other was from a clinical population of couples seeking marital therapy

(179 couples). All participants were given the RBI and two measures of marital

satisfaction, the Maudsley Marital Questionnaire (Aringdell, Emmelkamp, and Bast,

1983) and the Communication Questionnaire (CQ, Buunk & Nijskens, 1980). Results

indicated that irrational relationship beliefs negatively correlated to marital satisfaction

and quality o f communication.

Sullivan and Schwebel (1995) had similar results with their unmarried sample of

474 college students. Participants that endorsed irrational relationship beliefs also

experienced their current relationship to be less satisfying than those that held more

realistic beliefs.

In their study to investigate the effect that popular media had upon creating

irrational relationship beliefs, Shapiro & Kroeger (1991) had similar results. The 109

participants in this study took the RBI and the satisfaction subscale of the DAS as well as

a media questionnaire. Correlation results of this study (r =-.61 /?<001), indicated that

those people who endorsed irrational beliefs also reported less satisfaction in their current

intimate relationship.

In another study, 61 married couples took part in a study that sought to compare

irrational beliefs with marital satisfaction (Addis & Bernard, 2002). In this study 61

couples were divided into two groups based on whether or not they were involved in

marriage therapy. It was found that the irrational belief factors were a significant

31

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
predictor of the clinical and non-clinical sample. Specifically, the irrational beliefs of

“self-downing” and “need for comfort” showed the strongest relationship to marital

dissatisfaction.

In 1991, a study from Russia (Moller & Van Zyl, 1991) had 46 married couples

take the Relationship Belief Inventory and the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. They found

correlation between extreme levels of irrational belief and unhappy adjustment in

marriage. Specifically, the subscales, Disagreement is Destructive and Sexual

Perfectionism were significantly correlated with marital adjustment, while the other

subscales were not.

A study from Turkey (Hamamci, 2005) had 190 married men and women take a

measure of marital adjustment and another test designed to measure irrational beliefs in

marriage, The Interpersonal Cognitive Distortions Scale, developed by the study’s author.

Again, irrational beliefs were shown to be significantly negatively correlated with marital

adjustment.

Not all the research is conclusive. DeBord and Romans (1996) wanted to

determine what contribution irrational relationship beliefs made to marital adjustment and

compare that contribution to that of irrational beliefs in general. Unlike previous studies

cited, this study measured irrational relationship beliefs with an instrument designed by

the authors, called the Relationship Belief Questionnaire, (RBQ, Romans & DeBord,

1995). Results o f this study indicated that relationship-specific irrational beliefs were

significantly related to marital satisfaction but in the opposite direction than expected.

People that endorsed the irrational beliefs regarding intimate relationships were better

adjusted in their marriages. The fact that these findings are opposite to most of the

32

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
others, (Emmelkamp et al., 1987; Sullivan, 1997) may be attributed to the instrument

used to assess irrational relationship beliefs, the RBQ instead of the more established and

researched, RBI (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982).

People Who Endorse Irrational Beliefs

To date, there are very few published studies that have examined the demographic

factors that predispose or predict irrational relationship beliefs. Knowledge of these

factors could prove helpful in targeting, designing and implementing intervention and

relationship education. The demographic factors included in this study are presented as

well as a review from the current literature.

Gender

The connection between gender and irrational relationship beliefs appears to be

weak and the results o f several studies are mixed. Some studies have been unable to

show significant differences between the scores of men and woman (Fitzpatrick & Sollie,

1999; Hamamci, 2005; Shapiro & Kroeger, 1991; Sullivan & Schwebel, 1995).

However, one study did find that men are more likely to endorse irrational relationship

views than woman, (DeBord & Romans, 1996). Although this study used the

Relationship Belief Questionnaire to measure endorsement and not the RBI or RBS.

Another study (Sharp & Ganong, 2000) found that men’s irrational beliefs were more

resistant to change than women’s after an integrative training designed to help dispel the

beliefs.

In his dissertation, Bushman (1999) examined problem-solving techniques,

endorsement of irrational relationship beliefs, and marital satisfaction. He used the

popular RBI to measure endorsement of irrational relationship beliefs. His sample

33

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
consisted of 150 dating couples. There were some significant gender differences. The

men that reported poor satisfaction and destructive problem solving techniques

significantly endorsed the irrational belief subscales of Disagreement is Destructive and

People Cannot Change. Women that reported poor relationship satisfaction and

destructive problem solving techniques endorsed the irrational belief subscale of M ind

Reading is Expected.

In another study that used the RBI to measure endorsement, some slight

differences between men and woman were found (Bradbury & Fincham, 1993).

Specifically these differences were in the subscale endorsement. Men were more apt to

endorse the belief sub scale o f Sexual Perfectionism and the belief subscale The Sexes are

Different. However, this study did not detect gender differences in the RBI total score.

In another study that looked at gender and subscription to Ellis’ irrational beliefs

(Lichtenberg, 1992) it was found that women were more likely to endorse irrational

beliefs in general.

Age

O f the several studies that have looked at irrational relationship beliefs only one

o f them addressed age as a demographic factor. Shapiro and Kroeger (1991) designed a

study that examined media’s effect on endorsement of irrational relationship beliefs. Age

was found not to predict irrational relationship beliefs.

There was one other study that used age as a demographic factor but the authors

were not looking specifically at irrational relationship beliefs but irrational beliefs in

general. This study was designed to test a Slovakian version of the Irrational Beliefs

Scale (Kordacova & Kondas, 1998). In this study, respondents took a translated and

34

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
modified version of The Irrational Belief Test (Jones, 1968) to measure endorsement of

irrational beliefs. It was found that teenagers endorsed irrational beliefs significantly

more than adults and young adults endorsed them significantly more than older adults.

Education

Only one published study to date has investigated education level and its

relationship to irrational relationship beliefs (Hamamci, 2005). Although this study did

not use the RBI to measure the irrational beliefs, this author found that education did help

to temper the endorsing o f irrational relationship beliefs.

Shapiro and Kroeger (1991) examined the factor of occupation, which could be a

function of education. Their demographic questionnaire included a checkmark response

to either “professional” or “nonprofessional”. Results of this study found a correlation

between occupation and the RBI sub scales of Disagreement is Destructive and M ind

Reading is Expected. Participants categorized as “nonprofessional” had significantly

higher scores on these RBI subscales than their “professional” counterparts. These

results suggest that education tends to decrease the irrational belief that disagreement is

destructive and to decrease the expectation that partners be able to read each other’s

minds.

Position in Family Life Cycle

There is no published study to date that has investigated people’s position in the

family life cycle and irrational relationship beliefs. But one study that looked specifically

at parents of preschool age children found correlation between irrational parenting

beliefs, parenting stress, and marital satisfaction (McDonald, 1995). Parents who

reported high levels o f parenting stress were more apt to endorse irrational parenting

35

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
beliefs and also report lower levels of marital satisfaction. Consistent with previous

results, are studies that have looked at the family life cycle and marital satisfaction

(Rollins & Feldman, 1970; Spanier, Lewis, & Cole, 1975). There is significant

curvilinear U correlation indicating that the most satisfying periods of marital

relationships occur during early and late marriage, and the most dissatisfying period

during the “middle years”.

In order to establish a pattern of change in marital satisfaction, Kurdek (1999)

followed 522 couples who were married 1 year and 93 couples married 10 years.

Measurements were taken of each participant’s level of marital satisfaction over time.

Most participants reported very high levels of satisfaction in the beginning of their

marriages. But, generally, marital quality declined rapidly over the first few years,

stabilized and then fell again. Couples living with children showed the steepest rates of

decline in satisfaction.

Household Income

No published studies have examined the relationship between income and

endorsement o f irrational relationship beliefs. However, one study explored

socioeconomic levels and marital satisfaction (Aub & Linden, 1991). In their study of

180 couples they found no significant correlation between socioeconomic level and

marital satisfaction.

Years Married

There have been no published studies that investigated years married as a variable

in endorsement o f irrational relationship beliefs. It is hypothesized though, that length of

marriage would correlate positively to marital satisfaction and that people who may have

36

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
endorsed irrational beliefs when first married would have moderated their irrational

beliefs over time and experience.

Times Married

There have been no published studies to date that have used number of times

married as a demographic factor affecting endorsing of irrational beliefs.

Parents ’Divorce

Children whose parents divorce experience increased distress and seek more

psychological services than children in intact families (Harvey & Fine, 2004). They may

also have subtle predispositions about intimate relationships as suggested by Marquardt

(2005). Although there have been no published studies that have investigated the

relationship between divorced parents and irrational relationship beliefs, it was

hypothesized that participants whose parents’ had divorced would endorse more irrational

relationship beliefs.

Religion

There have been no published studies that have used religious affiliation as a

factor in endorsing irrational relationship beliefs. Because the dominant culture in Salt

Lake City, Utah is LDS, it seems appropriate that this factor be included. It was

anticipated that the LDS culture with its emphasis on traditional families and marital

success (The Church o f Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints, 2005) may support some of the

irrational belief scales. It was hypothesized that LDS respondents would endorse more

irrational beliefs.

Measuring Irrational Relationship Beliefs

The topic o f irrational relationship beliefs has generated interest among scientists

37

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
and practitioners who follow a cognitive behavioral therapeutic approach (Baucom et al.,

1989; Doherty, 1997; Eidelson & Epstein 1982; Schwebel & Fine, 1994; Sullivan, 1997).

In turn, this interest generated the need to accurately assess these beliefs and the

development of instruments to do so.

One of the most important aspects of this study is the investigation of the validity

of the instruments designed to measure irrational relationship beliefs. The validity of an

instrument is not established by one or two studies but rather by repeated studies, under

new conditions, and with different samples (Gall, Borg, & Gall, 1996). The Standards

for Educational and Psychological Testing is a reference work that seeks to provide

guidelines for the creation and use of instruments (American Educational Research

Association, 1999). It stipulates that validity o f an instrument is determined by test

content, response processes, and the internal structure of the test. This study seeks

further evidence that the RBI and the RBS are actually measuring the endorsement of

irrational beliefs and secondly, that the instruments predict marital relationship quality.

The other important aspect of this study concerns the reliability and internal

consistency of the individual items on each instrument. Reliability refers to the integrity

o f the items in relationship to each other. Internal consistency is an element of reliability.

All items in a subscale should load similarly toward the score. In this study, the RBI and

RBS purport to measure five subscales each (Eidelson & Epstein 1982 , Sullivan, 1997).

Cronbach’s coefficient alphas were performed on all the items that contribute to a

subscale score, further establishing the reliability of the individual items. For example, if

a subject scored high on an item on the sub scale M ind Reading is Expected, they should

have scored similarly on other items that contribute to that subscale.

38

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
In order to further establish the internal consistency of each instrument a principle

component analysis (PCA) was performed. PCA is a statistical procedure applied to a set

of variables to determine the strength of their relationship to one another. Results of the

PCA could help determine if the items in each instrument naturally fall into distinct and

exclusive groups or components (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). Although it wasn’t

originally planned, an exploratory factor analysis was also performed on the RBS to

attempt to replicate the study by Sullivan (1997).

The Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI)

Several studies have attempted to establish the reliability and validity o f the RBI

(Bradbury & Fincham, 1993; Eidelson & Epstein, 1982; Emmelkamp et al., 1987; James,

Hunsley & Hemsworth, 2002; Jones & Stanton, 1988). The RBI appears to have good

test retest reliability (Emmelkamp, et al, 1987) at both the 2 week and 12 week intervals.

Split-half reliability was also established.

Some studies have pointed to weaknesses in the validity of the RBI. In a study

conducted by Emmelkamp et al. (1987) only two of the subscales were predictive of poor

marital satisfaction. Higher scores in three subscales, Mindreading is Expected, Partners

Cannot Change, and The Sexes are Different actually predicted the random sample and

not the clinical one. “Clinically distressed couples scored less irrationally than their

normal counterparts, (p 779).” Since these discrepancies exist, the discriminant validity

of the RBI is in question.

Authors James, Hunsley, and Hemsworth, (2002), performed exploratory factor

analysis and confirmatory factor analysis on the RBI and found that support for a 5 factor

structure was weak. They identified problems with the internal consistency o f the

39

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
sub scales The Sexes are Different and Partners Cannot Change. In their study they

proposed a six factor model and cautioned against the use of the RBI until its reliability

could be established.

The Bradbury (1993) study also found problems with the internal validity of the

RBI. Unlike the initial study (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) and similar to the Emmelkamp

study (1987) they found that not all subscales were indicative of poor marital satisfaction.

The subscales Mindreading is Expected and Sexual Perfectionism did not correlate with

marital satisfaction. The Bradbury study suggested that only Disagreement is Destructive

and the total score be considered valid measures. The Moller & Van Zyl (1991) study

from Russia had similar results in which only two subscales of the RBI were found to

negatively correlate with marital satisfaction; Disagreement is Destructive and Sexual

Perfectionism.

These findings cast doubt upon the construct validity, and content validity of the

RBI and suggest the need for a superior instrument.

The Relationship Belief Scale (RBS)

Sullivan initially designed the RBS as a master’s thesis (1994). At the time it was

a thirty-two-item test designed to assess irrational relationship beliefs. Later in his

dissertation (1997) it was expanded to include ten more items to be a forty-two item

questionnaire. The RBS was designed to address the weaknesses inherent in the RBI and

to provide an alternative instrument. Rather than fitting several items around five distinct

themes or subscales, Sullivan’s items were unrelated items selected from the literature.

His subscale titles were derived after performing exploratory factor analysis on the items

and selecting titles that fit the emerging factors.

40

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
One of the weaknesses of Sullivan’s research is the relatively small sample of

married participants. The initial master’s study (1994) included 474 unmarried college

student participants. The dissertation study (1997) included 856 unmarried college

students and a community sample of only 100 married people.

Sullivan’s instrument is very promising because his subscales were developed by

factor analysis in contrast to the intuitive method used for the RBI (Eidelson & Epstein,

1982). However, this relatively new instrument has been used in only two studies and the

normative sample of 100 married people is relatively low.

Chapter Two Summary

A theoretical orientation was provided showing that irrational thought patterns are

often the source of depression and frustration in life and that endorsing irrational

relationship beliefs can be a source of discord for many married people. The term

“irrational relationship belief’ was defined and several irrational beliefs were identified

from the literature. Studies that have examined the relationship between irrational beliefs

and marital satisfaction were also cited. It was identified that very little is known about

the demographic qualities o f people who endorse these irrational beliefs. Rationale was

provided for each demographic factor in this study.

There was also a discussion regarding the RBI and RBS, two of the instruments

used in this study and some of the controversy over the strengths and weaknesses of each

was presented.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER III

Methodology

Research Design

This study employs four different statistics characteristic of correlational research

design; Pearson product moment bivariate correlation, stepwise multiple regression,

principal components analysis, and exploratory factor analysis. Correlational designs are

“used to make predictions and to study relationships between variables” (Gall, Borg &

Gall, 1996, p. 409). In this study, participants were asked to respond to demographic

questions, items concerning their attitudes regarding marriage relationships and their

satisfaction in their marriage. Each participant responded at only one point in time and

there was no manipulation o f variables or treatment.

The advantage to using a correlational design is that it allows researchers to look

at many variables within one study. It also lends itself to looking at pairs or

combinations o f multiple variables (Gall, Borg & Gall, 1996). There is a caution inherent

in correlational design regarding correlation and causation. Finding significant

correlation or predictive validity amongst variables is not conclusive evidence that one

has caused the other. Correlational design is best for establishing strength and direction

of the relationship (Gall, Borg & Gall, 1996).

Target Population and Accessible Population

The target population considered for this study was adults in legal marriages. The

accessible population was adults assembled in audiences to hear an instructional speaker.

The Institute of Marriage and Family Counseling, in Ogden, Utah provides speakers to

church, community, and businesses as a way to promote their counseling services. These

42

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
audiences were informed o f the opportunity to participate in the study. Interested

audience members took a response packet, completed it at home and returned it

anonymously to the researcher. Responses were culled from these audiences over the

course of a year until the 307 respondents were acquired.

A convenient sample o f people known personally to the researcher was also

included in the study. These respondents also mailed their completed packets to the

researcher anonymously. See Table 1 for a breakdown on sample sources and the

number of respondents from each one.

Selecting the Sample

It was originally hoped that audience members would complete their surveys prior

to the speech or presentation they were attending. However, time constraints of the

sponsoring organizations made this unrealistic so the data was collected using the

following method. The investigator informed the audience that they had an opportunity to

participate in a scientific study regarding attitudes about marriage. Audiences were told

that participants needed to be married and not have sought marital therapy in the last 12

months. This was done to help insure that the sample would represent a non-clinical

population. Interested parties were instructed to take a response packet at the end of the

lecture. Respondents took their surveys home and returned them to the investigator

anonymously via mail.

Each packet contained an informed consent sheet attached outside of the

envelope that explained the purpose of the study, length of time anticipated to complete

the forms and the risks and benefits of participating. The benefits included feeling good

that one has done a part to further science. The other benefit was the entertainment value

43

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
created by the questions themselves.

Inside the envelope was a ten item Demographic Questionnaire, followed by the

32 item Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) that measured adjustment to their own

marriage. The following two instruments, the RBI (40 items) and RBS (42 items) were

presented next, counterbalanced to offset instrument order effects. The RBI and RBS are

similar in nature and presentation. It was anticipated that test-takers may become fatigued

or disinterested after answering several items. If one of the tests was always second in

presentation, results of that test may not be valid. By deliberately ordering half of the

sets to have the RBI first, and the other half to have the RBS, first, the fatigue factor was

spread equally over the two instruments.

When respondents completed their packets they mailed them to the researcher.

None of the recipients received a reward or recompense for their participation. The

Catholic school that participated did receive a donation in lieu of their participation but

the money was paid to the administration and not to individual people. All envelopes

were coded discreetly so that when they were returned the investigator knew from which

audience they came. Five hundred envelopes were prepared and disseminated until the

307 responses were received. This represents a 61% rate of return and indicates that

some people intended to participate, took a packet but did not complete the

questionnaires or return them.

There were many married couples that responded to the study but their responses

were not paired, identified or linked in any way with their spouse’s. All participants,

except those known to the researcher, were gathered from audiences assembled to hear a

public speaker. These events were held in the greater metropolitan area of Salt Lake City,

44

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Utah. This includes Provo, Utah as the further southerly limit and Ogden, Utah as the

furthest northern limit.

Description o f the Sample

Sample Source

The participants were drawn from 12 different sources. Table 1 presents the

sources and the number of respondents from each. Seven of the twelve sources were

from audiences at lectures sponsored by the LDS church. Two of the sources were

private family reunions, where most of the respondents were LDS. One source comprised

people affiliated with a private Catholic school while another was from a personnel

training course for the State of Utah. The remaining source was a convenient sample of

the investigator’s acquaintances, and co-workers.

Age and Gender

A total o f 307 married people participated in the study. After deleting some cases

with missing information (described in Chapter Four, in data screening procedures) the

remaining number o f participants was 297. There were 114 men and 183 women, 38%

and 62% respectively of the sample. Female participants ranged in age from 21 to 79 with

a mean age of 40.64, while males ranged in age from 23 to 81 with a mean age of 42.77.

Education

There were only 2 respondents who indicated that they had not completed high

school. Another 19 had just high school, while 97 others had completed some college.

Those without a college degree represented 40% of all those polled. For the 179

respondents with college degrees, 44% of the total, 47 of them had obtained postgraduate

degrees.

45

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 1

Participant Source Breakdown by Gender

Source No. Sample Source male female Total %

1 Convenient Sample 33 39 72 24.2

2 LDS lecture 22 28 50 16.8

J Family Group 3 4 7 2.4

4 LDS lecture 0 2 2 .7

5 LDS lecture 5 11 16 5.4

6 LDS lecture 0 7 7 2.4

7 LDS lecture 0 0 0 0

8 State of Utah 15 1 16 5.4

9 Family Group 12 16 28 9.4

10 LDS lecture 0 4 4 1.3

11 Catholic School 11 55 66 22.2

12 LDS lecture 'y


6 9 3.0

13 LDS lecture 10 10 20 6.7

Total 114 183 297 100.0

Position in Family Life Cycle

In order to determine the position in the family life cycle, each respondent

identified the age of their oldest living child or the oldest living child of their spouse.

Twenty-three respondents (8%) were identified as Beginning Family. Forty-four people

(15%) were categorized in the Infant Preschool Family. Eighty-four people (28%) were

in the School Age category. Thirty-one people (10%) were identified in the Adolescent

46

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
family while another 37 people (13%) were considered to be in the Launching stage.

Seventy-three (25%) individuals fell into the category of Post Parental Family. Only 5

people (2%) identified as being in the category of Mature - No kids.

Household Income

The sample was predominantly upper middle class and only 7 participants

indicated household earnings under $24,000 annually. Almost half (49%) of those

participating reported household earnings over $75,000 per year. Two people failed to

indicate their household income.

Years Married

The number of years married ranged from 1 year to 56 years married.

Times Married

A total o f 262 people (88%) indicated that they were in their first marriage.

Another 30 people (10%) had been married once before. Five participants (2%) indicated

that they had been married 3 or more times.

Parents ’ Divorce

Only 56 (19%) participants indicated that their parents had divorced.

Religion

O f all the respondents, 219 (74%) were identified as LDS, 54 respondents (18%)

reported being Catholic, 9 (3%) responded Protestant, 7 (2%) indicated “No Religion”,

and 8 people (3%) reported a religion other than the options offered in the survey.

Instrumentation

Three different instruments were used to assess variables in this study as well as a

demographic questionnaire.

47

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Demographic Item Questionnaire (DIQ)

As described in Chapter II, there were nine demographic factors assessed by this

study; age, gender, education level, household income, years married, religion, position

in family life cycle, number of times married, and parent’s divorce (See the DIQ in

Appendix F). Two of the items on the DIQ were used as screening devices. The

question “Are you currently married?” helped control that only married people entered

the sample. The question “Have you sought professional counseling services for marital

distress in the last 12 months?” helped determine that the study represented a non-clinical

population.

The DIQ contained 4 sentences of short instructions and 10 items. Three of the

items, concerning age, number of years married, and age of oldest child, were “fill in the

blank”. The other seven items were arranged either as “yes/no” or in a multiple choice

fashion, both requiring the respondent to circle the correct option. This DIQ required

approximately one or two minutes to complete.

Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS)

The Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976) has been widely used since 1976 to

assess the quality of marriages. This test was developed from a large pool of items that

had appeared in other instruments. Many items were eliminated on the basis of

redundancy and lack o f clarity. A factor analysis was performed on the remaining 40

items. These 40 items were administered to a married sample, and a divorced sample.

Eight items were removed because of low factor loadings. The thirty-two remaining

items measure four subscale constructs; Dyadic Satisfaction, Dyadic Consensus, Dyadic

Cohesion, and Affectional Expression.

48

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reliability and Validity.

A panel o f three judges established content validity for each item. Criterion

validity was determined by comparing the group responses on each item. On each item,

the divorced group significantly differed from the married group. Construct validity was

proven by comparing the results of the DAS with another widely used instrument that

measures dyadic adjustment-the Locke Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale (Locke &

Wallace, 1959). The correlation between these measures was .86 for the married group

and .88 for the divorced group. Both of them significant at p = .001. The factor analysis

was a further indicator of construct validity. Results showed four distinct factors, three of

which had been hypothesized prior to the study, to be components of marital adjustment.

Reliability for the DAS was determined by performing Cronbach’s Coefficient

alpha’s on each subscale. The alpha level for each subscale is as follows: Dyadic

Consensus, .90; Dyadic Satisfaction, .94; Dyadic Cohesion, .86; and Affectional

Expression .73. The total scale reliability is .96. The alpha levels achieved with this

sample were similar. For the scales listed above the range was .86, .83, .77, and .70 with

an overall alpha on the DAS total score of .91. These alpha levels indicate that the DAS

is a reliable instrument to measure Dyadic Adjustment. The DAS has test-retest reliability

coefficient o f .96 at eleven weeks. Convergent validity studies have shown individuals

with low DAS scores have an increased probability of domestic violence, greater

depression, more family dysfunction and poor communication. Convergent validity

studies have shown high correlation between the DAS and other measures o f marital

satisfaction like the Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale (Locke & Wallace, 1959).

49

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Many other studies have arrived at similar measures of validity and reliability on

the DAS on a variety of samples (Antill & Cotton, 1982; Filsinger & Wilson, 1983;

Johnson & Greenberg, 1985; Kurdek & Schmitt, 1986; Sullivan, 1997).

The Dyadic Adjustment Scale is a well-researched tool effectively used to quickly

assess the marital satisfaction of people in any committed relationship. For instance, one

o f the items asks “Do you and your mate engage in outside interests together?” There is

a 5 point likert gradient between options “all of them” and “none of them” in which the

test-taker circles the one most closely matching their relationship. This instrument can be

completed in about 5 minutes. For purposes of this study, only a total score, indicative of

global satisfaction was used.

Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI)

The Relationship Belief Inventory (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) was designed to

assess beliefs about intimate relationships that contribute to relationship distress. Test

authors contacted twenty-three marriage counselors and asked them to identify beliefs

about marriage that they thought caused their clients marital difficulty. The original pool

of 128 items were divided into 5 preconceived categories and administered to a sample of

47 clinical couples. From these findings the list was reduced to 60 items.

A second sample o f 100 couples took the instrument containing the 60 items.

Based upon statistical results 18 items were removed and 8 items were left for each of the

subscales represented. The finished RBI consists of 40 items. Answers to items are

indicated on a 6 point likert type scale. Results of the test are expressed as an overall

score and scores on each of the five sub scales; Disagreement is Destructive, Mindreading

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
is Expected, Partners Cannot Change, Sexual Perfectionism, and The Sexes are Different.

It can be completed in about 10 minutes.

To determine internal consistency, test authors performed Cronbach alpha

coefficients on the eight items comprising each subscale. The alpha values for

Disagreement is Destructive, Mindreading is Expected, Partners Cannot Change, Sexual

Perfectionism, and The Sexes are Different were .81, .75, .76, .72, and .72, respectively.

Results from this sample indicate a degree of internal consistency but not as high as the

normative sample. The overall coefficient alpha was .85 and alpha levels for individual

subscales as listed above were .82, .74, .63, .74, and .67. Note that Partners Cannot

Change, and The Sexes are Different fall below the usual standard of .7 for determining

internal consistency.

Convergent validity was determined by comparing the RBI results with the

Irrational Beliefs Test (Jones, 1968), an instrument based upon Albert Ellis’ eleven

irrational thoughts (1994). All subscales, except The Sexes are Different were

significantly positively correlated with the Irrational Beliefs Test. Based upon the order

of subscales above, the corresponding r were .31, .21, .14, .28 and .11 (n = 200; p< .05

for the first four scales). The test authors note that convergent validity was challenging

because there were no other instruments that specifically dealt with irrational attitudes

regarding intimate relationships (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982).

To determine construct validity, sample results of the Relationship Belief

Inventory were compared with two other measures. The first was The Therapy Goals and

Expectations Questionnaire, developed by the same authors (Epstein & Eidelson, 1981).

It is a measure o f relationship stress and anticipated success in therapy. The second

51

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
instalment was the Locke Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale (MAS; Locke & Wallace,

1959) which assesses marital adjustment. For both the clinical sample and the non

clinical sample, significant scores on the Relationship Belief Inventory were significantly

negatively correlated with the Marital Adjustment Scale; meaning that people who

endorsed irrational relationship beliefs tended to have lower levels o f marital adjustment.

The Pearson r correlations between these two were -.57, -.24, -.38, -.18, -.25 for

Disagreement is Destructive, M ind Reading is Expected, Partners cannot Change, Sexual

Perfectionism, and The Sexes are Different, respectively (p .05).

The Pearson r correlation coefficients of the RBI and the Therapy Goals and

Expectations questionnaire were Disagreement is Destructive (-.45), Mindreading is

Expected (-38), Partners Cannot Change (-.60) and Sexual Perfectionism (-.27). All five

scales were found to be significantly negatively correlated with likelihood of treatment

success. These first three subscales were negatively correlated with desire to maintain

the relationship, and interest in conjoint therapy with the spouse. The subscale The Sexes

are Different was unrelated to any o f the measures of the Therapy Goals and

Expectations Questionnaire. Based upon these significant negative correlations the

authors determined that irrational beliefs appeared to be part of a maladaptive mind set

regarding relationships.

Despite the confidence of test authors, more recent research has suggested some

weaknesses in the RBI (Bradbury & Fincham, 1993; Emmelkamp et al., 1987; James et

al., 2002; Jones & Stanton, 1988; Sullivan, 1997;). These weaknesses were discussed in

Chapter Two.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The Relationship Belief Scale (BBS)

The Relationship Belief Scale was developed by Sullivan (1997) as a dissertation.

It was created as an alternative instrument to the Relationship Belief Inventory. This 42

item test measures the strength of attitude on five factors; Vigilance, Blame and

Punishment, All or Nothing Thinking, Perfectionistic Expectations, and Demandfor

Attention and produces subscale scores on these factors plus an overall score. It can be

completed in about 10 minutes.

The RBS was normed on 856 single college students in romantic relationships and

an additional 50 married couples recruited from the community. Unlike the RBI, the

RBS was created by performing exploratory factor analysis upon the original set of items

and then describing the emerging factors. Confirmatory factor analysis was also

performed in a follow up study which supported the five factor structure.

Test-retest reliability at four weeks was .85. Cronbach’s coefficient alpha for the

overall score for the college sample was .91 while the community sample was .85.

Results achieved in this study were an overall alpha of .92 and subscales as follows;

Vigilance .77, Blame and Punishment .82, A ll or Nothing Thinking .64, Perfectionistic

Expectations .77, and Demandfor Attention .77. The RBS has not been used in any other

published study to date.

Statistical Procedures

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question One

There will be no significant relationships comparing the RBI and RBS. To

determine the relationships between these two instruments, Pearson product moment

bivariate correlations were produced. This statistic is employed when both variables

53

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
under consideration are expressed in continuous scores (Gall, Borg, & Gall, 1996). For

this research question a correlation matrix was generated that computed Pearson product

moment coefficients for a total of twelve variables (the five subscales from the RBI, the

five subscales from the RBS and the global scores from both), and compares them with

one another. The alpha level of .01 was used in this study to designate significance of the

correlation coefficients.

Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Two

There will not be a five factor structure that emerges from the factor analysis

performed on the Relationship Belief Inventory. A principle component analysis (PC A)

was computed on the 40 items that comprise the Relationship Belief Inventory. Principle

component analysis is a statistical procedure that is applied to a set o f variables to

determine which variables in the set, form subsets that are relatively independent of one

another. “The goal of PC A is to extract maximum variance from a data set with a few

orthogonal components (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996).” In order to perform PCA, sample

size should be sufficiently large enough so that correlations can be reliably estimated.

Comrey and Lee (1992) suggest 300 cases or more are necessary and this study intended

to comply. However, missing data necessitated deleting ten cases. So the number of

respondents went from 307 to 297. This is practically close to the required 300. The data

also generally met the assumptions of normality, and linearity (Tabachnick & Fidell,

1996) although a discussion in Chapter 5 looks at these assumptions. Results of the PCA

were compared to the test authors’ (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) findings, to determine if

the five factors emerged as postulated with this sample.

Testing the Null Hypothesisfo r Research Question Three

54

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
There will not be a five factor structure that emerges from the factor analysis

performed on the Relationship Belief Scale. Another principle component analysis

(PCA) was computed on the 42 items that comprise the Relationship Belief Scale. It was

compared to the five factors identified by Sullivan (1997). It was also decided to try and

replicate the study performed by Sullivan so an exploratory factor analysis was also

completed on the items comprising the RBS.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question Four

Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the RBI, will not predict marital

satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Bivariate correlation

coefficients were generated among the scale scores of the Relationship Belief Inventory

and the global score of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. A stepwise multiple regression

analysis was conducted using the scale scores as predictor variables and the global score

of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale, as the criterion measure.

In stepwise regression the “linear equation starts out empty and IV’s are added

one at a time if they meet statistical criteria, but they may also be deleted at any step

where they no longer contribute significantly to regression (Tabachnick & Fidel, 2001).”

It is considered the best way to determine a prediction equation. It is used to determine

which subset o f IV’s makes the best prediction to a criterion variable. In this study, the

five subscales were added or deleted from the equation to determine the best set of

variables that predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the global score on the Dyadic

Adjustment Scale.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question Five

Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the RBS will not predict marital

55

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Bivariate correlation

coefficients were generated among the scale scores of the Relationship Belief Scale and

the global score of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Another stepwise multiple regression

analysis was conducted using the scale scores of the RBS as predictor variables and the

global score of the Dyadic Adjustment Scale, as the criterion measure.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question Six

The demographic variables o f gender, age, education level, years married, times

married, household income, position in fam ily life cycle, and parent's divorce, will not

predict endorsement o f irrational beliefs, as measured by the total scores o f a) the

Relationship Belief Inventory and b) the Relationship Belief Scale. A stepwise multiple

regression analysis was performed using the predictor variables of the demographic

factors listed above, and irrational relationship beliefs as the criterion variables Two

separate analysis’ were conducted; one using the composite score of the RBI as the

criterion variable and another using the composite score of the RBS.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question Seven

There are no combinations o f demographic factors and irrational beliefs, as

measured by a) the Relationship Belief Inventory and b) the Relationship Belief Scale,

that predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. A

stepwise multiple regression analysis was performed using both the subscales of the RBI

and the demographic variables as predictor variables and marital satisfaction, as

measured by the global score on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale, as the criterion variable.

Another stepwise multiple regression analysis was performed using the subscales of the

RBS and the demographic variables as the predictor variables.

56

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Summary o f Chapter Three

In chapter three, correlational research design was described. Its strengths and

weaknesses were explained. The target population and accessible population were

discussed. The methods employed to select the sample were outlined and the

demographic characteristics o f the sample were presented. Methods of data collection

were described. Each o f the instruments was described and reliability and validity

research were offered for each. The statistics employed for testing the null hypothesis of

each research question was outlined.

57

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER IV

Results

The statistical results of the study will be presented in this chapter. The

procedures o f data screening will be described. The research problems will be restated

followed by a review o f the research questions with their respective null hypothesis.

Analysis of the statistical results will be explained. The chapter will conclude with a

summary o f the analyses.

Data Screening Procedures

Data Entry

Data from the surveys were entered into SPSS version 11.5, on a personal

computer. The principle investigator and a trained assistant entered all the data. The data

were gathered over the course of a year and entered as protocols were received. Once all

the data was entered, all protocols were systematically checked for data entry accuracy.

The primary researcher and a trained assistant worked in tandem with one person reading

the paper and pencil responses while the other visually checked the scores that had been

entered into the computer program. Thirty-four small data entry errors were noted and

corrected. Twenty-three of the errors were found in the initial 25 protocols. It is

assumed that data entry accuracy increased as experience and proficiency increased.

Missing Data

All variables were screened for missing data. It was noted that ten respondents

(3 .2%) had thoroughly completed the survey questions except they omitted the last page

o f the RBI. It is impossible to know why this occurred. The RBI and RBS are similarly

formatted. In half o f the printed surveys the RBI came first and in the other half the RBS

58

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
came first. All o f the respondents that omitted the last page of the RBI had surveys in

which the RBI was first. The RBI contains 40 items. Items 30 to 40, eleven items in all,

are found on this last page. Since the missing responses represented 27.5% of all

possible points on any one individual’s RBI responses, it was determined that these cases

be deleted. Therefore, cases 12, 19, 27, 45, 75, 85, 176, 223, 231, and 237 were deleted,

leaving 297 participants in the study.

On fifteen of the RBI protocols, it was discovered that 18 responses were missing

from 12 different questions. The original protocols were examined to check for errors.

The RBI protocol was visually scrutinized to see if there was a pattern to those omitted.

None was found. The total number of responses possible from the 297 respondents was

11,880. The 18 missing points represented <1% of the total data. Tabachnick and Fidell

(2 0 0 1 ) suggest that one way of handling missing data is to replace the missing points

with the mean of the item and this method was employed. Missing data was replaced by

the sample mean o f the respective item. A total of 18 items were replaced by the sample

mean on the RBI protocols.

Fourteen of the RBS protocols, had 14 data points missing from 14 different

items. Again, after double checking for data entry errors and scrutiny o f which items

were omitted, there did not appear to be a pattern. These 14 represented <1% of the total

responses possible with this sample so it was decided to replace the missing points with

the sample mean o f each item. A total o f 14 data points were replaced using this method.

On the demographic variables, two participants failed to indicate income. For two

cases, 21 and 238 the missing data was left as is and these cases were left out of portions

of the study that used demographic information as variables.

59

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Univariate Underlying Assumptions

Responses to the RBI and RBS, the global score of the DAS and the demographic

data were examined for outliers, normality, and homogeneity of variance in order to

determine if these assumptions had been met.

Univariate outliers were identified by obtaining z scores for each case, item by

item. Scores that exceeded z = +-3 .29 were considered outliers (Tabachnick & Fidell,

2001). For the RBI, 9 items contained 31 cases that were identified as outliers. The

original protocols were searched for data entry accuracy, and to identify response

patterns. Cases that contained response outliers fell into two distinct patterns.

The first pattern of outliers was people who indicated that they were unhappy in

their marriages and also endorsed high levels o f irrational beliefs. This study sample

represented a non-clinical population. For the most part, most marriages were not in

distress and most people reported relatively high levels of marital satisfaction. People

who indicated that they had attended counseling in the last 12 months, for marital

distress, were excluded from the study. People who reported unhappiness in their

marriages were few. So, some of the outliers were created by extreme endorsement of

irrational beliefs. RBI responses fall on a six point scale, 0, 1,2, 3, 4, and 5. The extreme

responses are Strongly Disagree or Strongly Agree. Participants who reported unhappy

marriages and also endorsed extreme levels of irrational beliefs were considered to be

part of the population that this study was drawn from so the cases that fell into this

pattern were left in the study.

The second obvious response pattern, were people who, although they indicated

they were happily married, had strongly endorsed an item that measured irrational belief.

60

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Careful study was made o f how participants responded to other similar items. For

example, if a participant indicated they were happily married but also responded

“Strongly Agree” to an item contributing to the Sexual Perfectionism subscale, other

items that also contribute to that subscale were inspected. If the responses were

consistent with one another or within one response score it was considered a valid

opinion expressed by a participant that represented the target population of the study.

Cases with outliers that followed these two conditions were left in the study as is.

Three cases containing outliers were identified as problematic. Case 83 appeared

to be a strong response on a question that was worded with a double negative. It was

assumed that the respondent got confused since the response did not match their

responses on similar items. Two other cases, 17 and 123 had responses that were

inconsistent with their other responses and no other explanation could be made for the

extreme score. Tabachnick and Fidell (2001) recommend a conservative way to

eliminate outliers is to substitute the offending score with the item mean. All three cases

were retained by using this method.

The same procedure for identifying outliers was done for the RBS. Eighty-six

outliers spread over 23 different items were identified. Again, the original responses

were looked at to determine why they were outliers. The same two patterns were

obvious. Participants were either unhappy in their marriages and also endorsing extreme

levels o f irrational belief on the RBS or second, happily married people who had an

extreme endorsement o f an irrational belief and consistently indicated so. All 86 outliers

were deemed representative of the target population so all were retained.

To insure normality of the sample, graphs, skewness values, kurtosis values, the

61

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Shapiro Wilks test of significance, and Normal QQ plots were examined. On the RBS 23

test items had responses in which the skewness and kurtosis z-scores exceeded +- 3.29.

On the RBI, 10 test items contained responses that fell outside of the z +- 3.29. All were

created from the outliers that had previously been examined. All cases were deemed part

o f the population intended to study so all cases were left in the study. Since the RBI, the

RBS, and the DAS are standardized, published tests, deleting test items was not an

option. Chapter five contains a discussion regarding the assumptions of normality of the

sample and the implications in this study.

Multivariate Outliers

Mahalnobis Distance with p< .001 was computed on all items of the RBI, and

RBS, demographic variables, and the DAS total score. No multivariate outliers were

detected on the RBI or RBS but several were found in the demographic variables. Upon

further investigation, it was discovered that the variables Number of Times Married, and

Religion contained multivariate outliers. People who endorsed that they had been

married three or more times were outliers. People who endorsed that they belonged to

No Religion or Other Religion were outliers. Tabachnick and Fidell (2001) suggest one

way to deal with multivariate outliers is to collapse category cells. It was decided that the

variable Number o f Times married would collapse from 3 to 2 categories - Once, and

Two or more. This created a dichotomous variable and also eliminated the multivariate

outliers. Since the Religion scale was not a continuous scale and would not be suitable as

a variable for some o f the statistics which were planned for the study, the Religion

variable would collapse from 6 categories to 3 - LDS, Catholic, and Other. After these

categories were collapsed there were no multivariate outliers.

62

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Summary o f Data Screening

All original paper and pencil protocols were double checked against the data

entered to assure accuracy. On the RBI ten cases with eleven missing responses (110

responses total) were deleted from the study. Other points of missing data were

conservatively replaced with the sample item mean. Univariate outliers were identified

and examined. Three cases were deemed problematic on three respective items and the

offending responses were substituted by the item mean. All cases were scrutinized for

skewness, kurtosis, and homogeneity o f variance at the univariate and multivariate levels.

Mahalnobis distances were computed to identify multivariate outliers. The demographic

categories of religion and number of times married contained multivariate outliers so

these variable categories were collapsed which solved the problem.

Restatement o f the Problem

The research problem in this study is three-fold. First, two tests, designed to

measure irrational beliefs in romantic relationships, have been developed—the

Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI, Epstein & Eidelson, 1982) and the Relationship

Belief Scale (RBS, Sullivan, 1997). While the RBI has been used in many studies since

1982, the RBS author Sullivan claims that the new test has stronger psychometric

properties and is a better predictor o f marital adjustment (Sullivan, 1997). This study

seeks to investigate evidence of validity and reliability for these two tests.

Second, the relationship between irrational relationship beliefs as measured by the

RBI and RBS and marital satisfaction will be explored. Third, this study seeks to know if

the demographic factors of age, gender, household income, education, religion, number

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
of years married, number of times married, position in the family life cycle or parental

divorce, predict irrational beliefs.

Analysis o f the Statistical Results

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question One

There will be no significant relationships comparing the RBI and RBS.

Descriptive statistics for these two tests are outlined in Tables 2 and 3. The correlation of

each item to the overall total score is also indicated. Table 4 compares the means and

standard deviations achieved on the RBI, RBS, and DAS from this sample, to those from

other samples.

In order to determine relationships between these two measures, bivariate

correlations were created between the subscale scores, and total scores of both the RBI

and RBS (See Table 5). Significance was determined at thep < .01 level, two-tailed.

The RBI total score and the RBS total score (r = .6 6 ) were significantly correlated.

The subscales o f each test were scrutinized for the strength of their correlation to

the opposing test. All five subscales of the RBI were found to be significantly correlated

with the RBS total score: Disagreement is Destructive (r = .6 8 ), Mindreading is

Expected (r = .44), Partners Cannot Change (r = .41), Sexual Perfectionism (r = .31) and

The Sexes are Different (r = .30).

All the subscales o f the RBS were significantly correlated with the RBI Total

Score: Vigilance (r = .46), Blame and Punishment (r = .51), All or Nothing Thinking (r =

.52), Perfectionistic Expectations (r = .62) and Demandfor Attention (r = .53). In fact,

RBS scale Perfectionistic Expectations was more significantly correlated with the RBI

total score than two of the RBI sub scales, The Sexes are Different (r = .61) and Partners

64

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Cannot Change (r = .59). The stronger correlations were found in the RBS sub scales

relationship to the RBI total score than the other way around. This suggests that the RBI

is measuring constructs not measured by the RBS. For instance RBI subscales Sexual

Perfectionism and The Sexes are Different have lower levels of correlation with the RBS

total score and lower levels o f correlation with almost every other subscale including

ones also from the RBI. The RBS does not assess beliefs about gender or sexuality.

With only one exception, each RBI subscale significantly correlated with each

RBS subscale. Only the RBI subscale The Sexes are Different did not significantly

correlate with the RBS subscale Demandfor Attention at the .01 level, two-tailed. The

most highly correlated subscales between the two measures were Disagreement is

Destructive (RBI) With Perfectionistic Expectations (RBS) (r = .6 8 ). The RBI and RBS

are significantly related to one another and appear to be measuring similar constructs.

Testing the Nidi Hypothesis fo r Research Question Two

There will not be a five factor structure that emerges from the factor analysis

performed on the Relationship Belief Inventory. Cronbach coefficient alphas were

produced for the RBI subscales and results were Disagreement is Destructive(.Sl),

Minding Reading is Expected (.74), Partners Cannot Change (.63), Sexual Perfectionism

(.74), and The Sexes are Different (.67).

A principle component analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation was computed on

the 40 items that comprise the RBI. Varimax was chosen in order to maximize the

variance, thereby increasing the high loadings and decreasing the low loadings

(Tabachnick and Fidell, 2001). The analysis was forced to 5 factors. Prior to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 2

RBI Mean, Standard Deviation and Pearson r with RBI Total (n = 297)

Item M SD r / RBI Total


RBI 1 .69 .89 .54

RBI 2 1.29 1.09 .29

RBI 3 1 .1 2 1.03 .45

RBI 4 1.82 1.38 .41

RBI 5 3.46 1.56 .15

RBI 6 1 .2 1 1.03 .45

RBI 7 1.18 1 .2 0 .33

RBI 8 1.65 1.32 .35

RBI 9 1.48 1.23 .38

RBI 10 2.06 1.35 .42

RBI 11 1.57 1.16 .52

RBI 12 1.78 1 .2 0 .54

RBI 13 .65 .69 .27

RBI 14 2.08 1.29 .1 2

RBI 15 1.91 1.27 .41

RBI 16 2.03 1 .1 2 .35

RBI 17 2.80 1.17 .2 0

RBI 18 1.61 1 .1 0 .34

RBI 19 1.62 1.31 .43

RBI 20 2.59 1.49 .19

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 2 continued

RBI 21 1.79 1.15 .54

RBI 22 1.94 1.16 .41

RBI 23 2.09 1.19 .36

RBI 24 2.05 1.29 .33

RBI 25 2.41 1.27 .25

RBI 26 1.87 1.19 .48

RBI 27 1.27 1.02 .45

RBI 28 .91 .90 .24

RBI 29 1.36 1.06 .44

RBI 30 2.92 1.32 .36

RBI 31 1.15 1.06 .51

RBI 32 1.29 1.15 .36

RBI 33 .65 .74 .13

RBI 34 1.24 1.08 .51

RBI 35 1.33 1.15 .45

RBI 36 1.37 1.29 .53

RBI 37 1.11 .96 .57

RBI 38 1.47 1.15 .29

RBI 39 1.72 1.32 .46

RBI 40 1.75 1.38 .49

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 3

RBS Mean, Standard Deviation and Pearson r with RBS Total (n = 297)

Item M SD r / RBS Total


RBS1 1.95 1.35 .42

RBS2 1.31 1.34 .45

RBS3 2.26 1.34 .46

RBS4 .79 1.13 .45

RBS5 .45 .82 .33

RBS6 1 .8 8 1 .2 2 .37

RBS7 1.23 1.35 .64

RBS 8 .78 1.04 .45

RBS9 .14 .6 8 .17

RBS10 1.14 1 .1 2 .56

RBS11 1.91 1.36 .49

RBS12 1 .6 8 1.40 .60

RBS13 .71 .99 .6 6

RBS14 .6 8 .8 6 .48

RBS15 1.23 1.15 .65

RBS16 1.76 1.42 .42

RBS17 1.59 1.36 .55

RBS18 .26 .74 .31

RBS19 .89 1.14 .26

RBS20 1.51 1.23 .24

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 3 continued
RBS21 1.14 1.16 .54

RBS22 1.32 1.56 .28

RBS23 .2 2 .55 .30

RBS24 1.56 1.42 .60

RBS25 .65 .79 .38

RBS26 .38 .75 .42

RBS27 .97 1.09 .56

RBS28 .41 .6 8 .36

RBS29 .79 1 .0 0 .6 6

RBS30 1.54 1.28 .54

RBS31 1 .1 2 1.19 .67

RBS32 1.44 1.46 .52

RBS33 .19 .63 .24

RBS34 1 .8 8 1.57 .56

RBS35 .93 1.05 .58

RBS36 .91 1.13 .64

RBS37 .95 1 .1 1 .36

RBS38 1.05 1 .1 2 .52

RBS39 1.04 1 .0 2 .52

RBS40 .80 1 .0 1 .64

RBS41 .99 1 .0 0 .46

RBS42 .73 .93 .65

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 4

Comparison o f M a n d SD o f RBI, RBS, and DAS Scores with Previous Samples

Epstein & Eidelson


1982

Scale M SD M SD
RBI
Disagreement Is Destructive 11.7 6 .0 10.7 4.7

Mindreading Is Expected 1 2 .6 5.3 14.4 4.8

Partners Cannot Change 1 0 .1 4.4 1 0 .1 4.6

Sexual Perfectionism 13.4 5.9 16.6 5.5

The Sexes are Different 18.4 5.9 12.7 6 .0 0

RBI Total Score 66.5 18.0 64.6 -

Sullivan’s 1997
RBS
Satisfied Married
Vigilance 1 0 .2 6.7 1 1 .0 7.2

Blame & Punishment 9.4 6.5 1 2 .2 5.9

All or Nothing Thinking 5.4 4.1 5.5 4.2

Perfectionistic Expectations 9.5 5.9 13.5 7.8

Demand for Attention 10.7 5.6 13.2 5.1

RBS Total Score 45.3 22.9 55.5 2 1 .2

Spanier Study
1976

Dyadic Adjustment Scale 115.4 13.6 114.8 17.8

Total Score

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 5

Correlation Matrix o f the Subscale and Total Scores o f the RBI and RBS

RBI RBI RBI RBI RBI RBI


D M C S MF Total
4 4 ** 28** 3 4 ** sK*
RBI D 1 .26**

RBI M 1
38** 2 i** 16** .66**

RBI C 1
1 y** 2 3 ** 5 9 **

RBI S 1
2 5 ** 64**

RBI MF 1 61**

RBI Total 1

RBS V

RBS B

RBS A

RBS P

RBS D

RBS Total

Note. RBI D = Disagreement is Destructive, RBI M = Mindreading is Expected, RBI C = Partners Cannot

Change, RBI S = Sexual Perfectionism, RBI MF = The Sexes are Different, RBS V = Vigilance, RBS B =

Blame and Punishment, RBS A = All or Nothing Thinking, RBS P = Perfectionistic Expectations, RBS D =

Demand for Attention.

* p , .05. ** p , .01

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 5 continued

RBS RBS RBS RBS RBS RBI


V B A P D Total
RBI D 4 7 ** .46** .6 8 ** .53** .6 8 **
.54**

RBI M .31** .2 2 ** ^^ ^ % 38** .54** 4 4 **

RBI C o i ^% .36** 42** .28** 2 2 ** 41**

RBI S . 1 2 ** .2 2 ** .26** .37** .29** .31**

RBI MF 2 3 ** .31** ^j ^^ .28** .1 2 * 30**

RBI Total 4 5 ** .51** .52** .62** .53** .6 6 **

RBS V 1
71** .55** .54** .35** .82**

RBS B 1 .52** .6 6 ** 3 7 ** .85**

i{s
RBS A 1 .50** .43**

RBS P 1 .59** g4**

RBS D 1 .6 8 **

RBS Total 1

Note. RBI D = Disagreement is Destructive, RBI M = Mindreading is Expected, RBI C = Partners Cannot

Change, RBI S = Sexual Perfectionism, RBI MF = The Sexes are Different, RBS V = Vigilance, RBS B =

Blame and Punishment, RBS A = All or Nothing Thinking, RBS P = Perfectionistic Expectations, RBS D =

Demand for Attention.

* p, .05. ** p , .01

interpreting the analysis Tabachnick and Fidell (2001) recommend 4 tests to determine if

there is merit in analyzing the factor analysis. They are; significant bivariate correlations,

Bartlett’s Test o f Sphericity, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling and

inspection o f the communalities.

72

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The scree test plot was also examined. The scree test is a graph produced by

placing the factors along one axis and the eigenvalues, or measure of variance, placed

along the other (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). Each factor is represented along with its

eigenvalue in descending order. This results in a visual representation of the significant

contribution that each factor makes to the variance. This graph was visually scrutinized

for support of the factor structure.

A correlation matrix was produced and examined to determine if there was a

pattern of bivariate correlations. In this case, several paired items with an r over .30 were

identified. The Bartlett’s Test of Sphericitiy is a sensitive test of the hypothesis that

correlations in a matrix are zero. In this study, the approximate chi-square = 3683.74,

d f = 780, sig = .000. This is considered high and therefore the null hypothesis is

rejected, indicating that the principle component analysis can be interpreted. One caution

with the Bartlett’s test is that with a high n the test tends to show significance even if

correlations are low (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2001). Therefore, another test of factorability

was desired.

The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling (KMO) is a test used to determine

the viability of the factor analysis. In this case the KMO was .793. According to Kaiser

(1974) this is considered good and falls at a high level in the “Middling” category and

provides further encouragement to interpret the PCA.

The communalities produced were inspected to see how well individual items

relate to the total correlation. The communalities were inspected for results that exceed

.20. In this case 39 o f the 40 extraction communalities exceed the cutoff and provide

further confidence in interpreting the PCA.

73

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The resulting initial eigenvalues were inspected to determine variance. The first

five factors have an initial eigenvalue of 6.7, 2.8, 2.5, 2.2, and 1.8 respectively. These

five factors account for 40.80% of the total variance indicating that the results are not

orthogonal. The scree plot in Figure 1 does not provide strong support for the five factor

structure.

Table 6 displays the results of the PCA performed on responses to the RBI. This

table shows the five components and the item loadings. Each item was viewed to

determine which component it loaded most significantly towards. Then the items were

ranked in order o f the strength of the loading. In this instance, the components that were

extracted do not correspond directly to the target RBI subscales.

Component one most closely corresponds to Disagreement is Destructive and

accounts for 16.95% of the variance in the responses. This is by far the most significant

component. On the RBI, items 1,6, 11, 16, 21, 26, 31, and 36 comprise the subscale

Disagreement is Destructive. In Table 6 , under component one, all of these intended

items loaded significantly toward the target component. However, items 2, 7, 12, 22, 27,

and 37 also loaded toward component one. On the RBI protocol, these items are

designed to assess the subscale Mindreading is Expected. In this PCA, component one

does not differentiate between these two constructs. Instead these items tend to load

towards a new component. Upon inspection of all these items from the two subscales, the

common theme appears to be related to that of inadequate communication.

Component 2 contributed 7.06% to the variance of the scores. It most closely

aligns with RBI subscale Sexual Perfectionism. Six of the eight intended items loaded

most significantly to this target component. Item 9, “If I’m not in the mood for sex when

74

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
my partner is, I don’t get upset about it.” and item 14, “A good sexual partner can get

himself/herself aroused for sex whenever necessary” did not load most significantly

toward the intended component.

Figure 1

Scree Plotfo r Principal Component Analysis o f RBI

Scree Plot
8

CD 2

CO
>
c=
CD
CD
i II 0
4 ----------- 1 -------------■------------■----------- ■ ------------1 ------------1 ------------ ■----------- ■----------- ■ ------------ ■ -----------■ ----------- 1 --------'" i 1" ’ ■ 11 1 " ■ —■ ■— ■

1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 39

Component Number

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 6

RBI Principle Component Analysis - Pattern Matrix (n = 297)

Compl Comp 2 Comp3 Comp 4 Comp 5 Com m unalities

RBI 21(D) .697 .555

RBI 12 (M) .692 .503

RBI 6 (D) .644 .432

RBI 37 (M) .608 .509

RBI 31(D) .604 .459

RBI 26(D ) .595 .416

RBI 11(D) .576 .413

RBI 22 (M) .565 .452

RBI 1(D) .561 .392

RBI 16(D) .542 .345

RBI 27 (M) .491 .457

RBI 36(D) .486 .421

RBI 7 (M) .417 .508

RBI 2 (M) .415 .246

RBI 29 (S) .700 .567

RBI 39 (S) .6 8 6 .526

RBI 34 (S) .680 .533

RBI 24 (S) .665 .459

RBI 4 (S) .664 .464

RBI 19 (S) .581 .394

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 6 continued
-> Com m unalities
1 2 4 5
RBI 40 (MF) .636 .525

RBI 30 (MF) .601 .449

RBI 35 (MF) .559 .470

RBI 15 (MF) .556 .413

RBI 5 (MF) .501 .412

RBI 25 (MF) .476 .273

RBI 20 (MF) .458 .480

RBI 10 (MF) .445 .308

RBI 13 (P) .664 .455

RBI 28 (P) .662 .479

RBI 33 (P) .622 .415

RBI 18 (P) .429 .327

RBI 38 (P) .376 .240

RBI 3 (P) .375 .291

RBI 8 (P) .301 .327

RBI 9 (S) .289 .394

RBI 17 (M) .698 .508

RBI 32 (M) .562 .444

RBI 14 (S) .421 .240

RBI 23 (P) .307 .196

Note: (D) = Disagreement is Destructive, (M) = Mindreading is Expected, (S) = Sexual

Perfectionism, (MF) = The Sexes are Different, (P) = Partners Cannot Change.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Component 3 contributed 6.47% of the variance and matches exactly with RBI

subscale The Sexes are Different. All 8 of the original items loaded most significantly on

this component.

Component 4 accounts for 5.69% of the variance and aligns with RBI subscale

Partners Cannot Change. Seven o f the eight items proposed to be in this component

loaded most significantly toward it. Item 23 “A partner who hurts you badly once

probably will hurt you again” did not load most significantly to the intended component.

Component 5 contributes 4.61% of the total variance and is comprised of the leftover

items from the subscale Mindreadmg is Expected and item 14 and 23.

Table 7 displays the RBI subscales along with the items that comprise that

subscale from the actual test. The column “PC A Loading” reflects the rotated factor

loadings. To determine significant loadings Tabachnick and Fidell (2001) suggest that a

cut off o f .45 is “modest”. Results for this sample show that only 26 of the 40 items

(65%) loaded significantly toward the factor postulated by the RBI authors, Eidelson &

Epstein (1982). When the cut off was lowered to .30 to be consistent with other analysis

in this study, seven additional items, making 33 total (83%) loaded significantly toward

the factors proposed by the original test. Seven items indicated by an asterisk did not

load significantly toward the target subscale at the .30 level or greater and 3 items loaded

significantly on more than one component.

In summary, a five factor structure was produced for the RBI from this sample,

however, the overall explained variance of the five factors was only 40.80% and seven items

did not load on expected factors even at the lenient cutoff of .30. All eight items from the

subscalq Disagreement is Destructive plus six from the subscale Mindreading is

78

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 7

Principle Component Analysis, Loadings o f RBI Subscale Items

RBI Subscale RBI PCA


Item Loading
Disagreement is 1 .561

Destructive 6 .644

11 .576

16 .542

21 .697

26 .595

31 .604

Mind Reading is 2
*

Expected 7 *

12
*

17 .698

22 .347

27 .425

32 .562

37 *

Partners Cannot 3 .375

Change 8 .301

13 .664

18 .429

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 7 continued

23 *

28 .662

33 .622

38 .376

Sexual Perfectionism 4 .664

9 *

14 *

19 .581

24 .665

29 .700

34 .680

39 .6 8 6

The Sexes are Different 5 .501

10 .445

15 .556

20 .458

25 .476

30 .601

35 .559

40 .636

* This item did not significantly load on this factor.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Expected formed a new component that contributed most to the variance. Items from

these two subscales reflect an attitude that mind reading is preferred because actual

communication runs the risk o f disagreement, which can be destructive. Two subscales

Sexual Perfectionism and Partners Cannot Change remained predominantly intact. The

subscale The Sexes are Different was completely intact with all eight items hitting the

target component. These results represent weak support for the five factor structure

proposed by test authors Epstein and Eidelson (1982) and suggest that there is

considerable variability in the subscales Disagreement is Destructive and Mindreading is

Expected.

Testing the Nidi Hypothesis fo r Research Question Three

There will not be a five factor structure that emerges from the factor analysis

performed on the Relationship Belief Scale. Cronbach coefficients were produced on the

subscales o f the RBS and the resulting alphas were Vigilance (.77), Blame and

Punishment (.82), A ll or Nothing Thinking (.64), Perfectionistic Expectations (.77) and

Demandfor Attention (.77). Alpha levels for all subscales except All or Nothing

Thinking indicate good internal consistency of the items contributing to each subscale.

The original proposal for this study, called for a Principal Component Analysis to be

performed in order to confirm Sullivan’s postulate that the RBS had a five factor

structure. After attaining the results of the PCA, it was decided to also replicate the

original exploratory factor analysis performed by Sullivan in 1997 in order to see if a

better factor model could be made. The results of both the PCA and the exploratory

factor analysis are presented.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The principle component analysis of the RBS.

A principle component analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation was computed on

the 42 items that comprise the Relationship Belief Scale. It was forced to 5 factors.

Once again the four methods to determine factorability o f R were assessed. The

correlation matrix produced contained some sizeable correlations. Several items with an

r over .30 were noted.

The Bartlett’s Test of Sphericitiy was evaluated. The approximate chi-square =

4631.50, d f = 861, sig = .000 is considered high and therefore the null hypothesis was

rejected, encouraging the interpretation of the analysis. In this study the Kaiser-Meyer-

Olkin Measure o f Sampling (KMO) was .894. According to Kaiser (1974) this is

considered high and falls in the “Meritorious” category and further supports

interpretation o f the PCA.

The communalities were inspected for results that exceed .20. All but one of the

42 items exceeded this cutoff providing further confidence in interpreting the PCA. The

scree plot was visually inspected (See figure 2) but support was weak for 5 factors.

The resulting initial eigenvalues were inspected to determine variance. The first

five factors have an initial eigenvalue of 10.5, 2.6, 2.3, 1.6, and 1.5. These five factors

account for 44.72% of the total variance. Results of the principle component analysis can

be seen in Table 8 . To create this table each of the item loadings was inspected to

determine which o f the components it loaded most significantly toward. These were then

rank ordered from largest to smallest.

In order to determine significant loadings a cut-off of >.30 was chosen.

Tabachnik and Fidell (2001) consider .32 to be the minimum cutoff loading for

82

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
determining significance. However, the .30 was chosen in order to provide continuity

across the three studies that used factor analysis or principle component analysis.

Sullivan (1997) used .30 in his study and so that is the cutoff selected. Twenty-seven of

the 42 items (approximately 64%) of the RBS loaded significantly toward the original

factors when the .30 was used.

Observe that fifteen items loaded most significantly to component one. This

component accounted for 25.05 % of the variance and is the strongest component.

However, it is a mix o f items from four of the subscales. Five of the items 7, 12, 21, 31,

36 were from subscale Blame and Punishment. Four items 13, 27, 32, and 42, were from

subscale Vigilance. Another 4 items, 10, 24, 29, and 34 were from subscale

Perfectionistic Expectations. Two items from All or Nothing Thinking were also part of

this component. A common theme for all of these could not be identified when all o f the

items were inspected individually.

Component 2 most closely resembles Demandfor Attention and accounted for

6.27% o f the variance. All seven of the intended items loaded most significantly to this

component. Items 15 and 35 from the Perfectionistic Expectations subscale, combined

with all seven Demandfor Attention items to create this component.

Component 3 closely matches the subscale Vigilance since five of the original

items hit the target component. It accounts for 5.581% of the variance. But even though

five of the original Vigilance items are there, another five items are missing. The RBS

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Figure 2

Scree Plot o f Principal Component Analysis o f RBS

S cree Plot
12

10

0) EL
3
(0 2
>
c
<D
05
Lj j 0

1 4 7 10 13 16 19 22 25 28 31 34 37 40

C om ponent Number

subscale Vigilance is comprised of ten items. Items 9, 19, and 41 from the A ll or Nothing

subscale combined to form this component.

Component 4 contained six items and accounted for 4.00% of the variance. It was

comprised of 4 items from All or Nothing Thinking, one item from Blame and

Punishment and another item from Vigilance.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Component 5 contained four items from the subscale All or Nothing Thinking plus

an additional two from Blame and Punishment and another two from Perfectionistic

Expectations. It represented 3.80% of the variance.

Magnitude o f variance explained by the factors was also different from

Sullivan’s results. In his study Factor One, Vigilance was the one that explained the most

variance. In this study, the factor resembling Vigilance was the 3rd strongest factor, while

the new component created from 15 items from 4 different subscales emerged to be the

strongest component or one which explained the most variance.

For this sample, only 27 of the 42 original RBS items loaded toward the factors

postulated by Sullivan (1997). Also, 16 items loaded significantly towards two or more

components. The strongest component, representing 25.25% of the variance was

comprised o f a mix o f 15 items from four subscales. After inspecting the 15 items a

common theme could not be derived. These results do not support the five factor

structure as proposed by Sullivan but rather suggest considerable variability in the factor

structure.

85

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8

Results o f the RBS Principle Component Analysis - Pattern Matrix (n = 297)

Compl Comp2 Comp3 Comp4 Contp 5 Com m unalities

RBS 31(B) .804 .682

RBS 12(B) .672 .537

RBS 21(B) .635 .439

RBS 32 (V) .631 .466

RBS 38(A) .628 .428

RBS 29 (P) .616 .639

RBS 13 (V) .601 .664

RBS 7(B) .582 .6 8 8

RBS 36(B) .548 .522

RBS 42 (V) .513 .544

RBS 27 (V) .482 .425

RBS 24 (P) .476 .476

RBS 10 (P) .462 .448

RBS 34 (P) .453 .467

RBS 4(A ) .315 .232

RBS 16(D) .721 .534

RBS 1(D) .703 .511

RBS 6 (D) .617 .484

RBS 11(D) .601 .440

RBS 15 (P) .547 .614

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 8 continued

RBS 39(D) .547 .614

RBS 30(D) .529 .413

RBS 35 (P) .450 .425

RBS 25 (D) .361 .374

RBS 37 (V) .534 .318

RBS 8 (V) .531 .438

RBS 40 (V) .523 .529

RBS 18 (V) .401 .387

RBS 3 (V) .392 .279

RBS 9(A) .353 .145

RBS 19(A) .351 .260

RBS 41(A) .348 .310

RBS 23(A) .750 .582

RBS 33 (A) .6 6 6 .487

RBS 28 (A) .606 .437

RBS 26(B) .515 .430

RBS 14(A) .413 .340

RBS 22 (V) .319 .277

RBS 17(B) .544 .546

RBS 5 (P) .498 .379

RBS 2(B ) .492 .394

RBS 20 (P) .444 .332

87

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The exploratory factor analysis o f the RBS.

Sullivan (1997) performed a series of exploratory factor analysis in the course of

designing and refining the RBS. The principle axis promax solution fit Sullivan’s data

best (Sullivan, 1997) so a principle axis promax (k=3) with oblique rotation was used to

create the factor analysis in order to attempt a close replication of Sullivan’s study.

The four tests o f factorability were performed in order to determine the

factorability o f the analysis. The correlation matrix produced was visually examined to

identify some sizeable correlations.

The Bartlett’s Test o f Sphericity produced an approximate chi-square = 4631.50,

d f = 861, sig = .000. This is considered high, therefore the null hypothesis was rejected

and the factor analysis could be interpreted.

The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling (KMO) was .89, in the

“Meritorious” category (Kaiser, 1974) and provided evidence of the factorability of the R

matrix. The communalities were inspected for results that exceed .20. In this case all but

5 of the 42 extraction communalities exceed the cutoff and provide further rationale for

interpreting the results.

The scree test provided weak support for the 5 factor structure (see Figure 3). It

appeared that 3 factors were strong and an argument could be made for 6 factors.

The resulting initial eigenvalues were inspected to determine variance. The first

five factors have an initial eigenvalue of 10.5, 2.6, 2.3, 1.6, and 1.5 respectively. These

five factors account for only 37.62% of the total variance. This is not high and suggests

that the first 5 factors do not explain the majority of the variance in the responses. The

cutoff point for item loadings in the Sullivan study were items greater than .30 for the

88

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Figure 3

Scree Plot fo r Exploratory Factor Analysis o f RBS

S cree Plot
12

10

<D
_g>
(0 2
>
c
<D
05
lu o

7 10 13 16 19 22 25 28 31 34 37 40

Factor Number

principle factor and items greater than .20 for secondary factors. Tabachnik and Fidell

(2001) identify this as poor but the goal was to come as close to the Sullivan study as

possible so the more lenient criteria was preserved.

Table 9 shows the results of the Factor Analysis. Each of the RBS items is shown

with the factor that it loaded most significantly toward, even if that loading was not

significant. In order to produce this table, each item was studied to determine the factor

that it loaded most significantly towards. The items were rank ordered from largest to

89

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9

Results o f the RBS Factor Analysis -- Pattern Matrix ( n =- 297)


Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 Communalities
RBS 31 (B) .874 .675

RBS 29 (P) .631 .608

RBS 21 (B) .626 .376

RBS 32 (V) .612 .390

RBS 12 (B) .607 .485

RBS 38 (A) .602 .353

RBS 15 (P) .496 .578

RBS 24 (P) .421 .432

RBS 10 (P) .413 .393

RBS 36 (B) .403 .483

RBS 27 (V) .385 .376

RBS 4(A ) .248 .194

RBS 16 (D) .705 .460

RBS 1(D) .655 .424

RBS 6 (D) .628 .376

RBS 11 (D) .526 .372

RBS 39 (D) .511 .404

RBS 30 (D) .465 .359

RBS 35 (P) .378 .386

RBS 25 (D) .299 .268

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 9 continued

RBS 13 (V) .541 .659

RBS 40 (V) .489 .500

RBS 8 (V) .451 .354

RBS 42 (V) .440 .513

RBS 37 (V) .414 .2 1 2

RBS 3 (V) .314 .226

RBS 41 (A) .261 .252

RBS 19 (A) .233 .117

RBS 9(A ) .207 .058

RBS 23 (A) .675 .481

RBS 33 (A) .589 .368

RBS 28 (A) .475 .311

RBS 26 (B) .417 .330

RBS 18 (V) .322 .280

RBS 14 (A) .321 .293

RBS 22 (V) .241 .179

RBS 17(B) .499 .280

RBS 7(B) .463 .689

RBS 2(B) .396 .295

RBS 5 (P) .395 .239

RBS 20 (P) .329 .160

RBS 34 (P) .324 .410

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
smallest within each factor. The results of the loadings were not orthogonal and eight

items loaded significantly on more than one factor. But, for the purposes of this study the

item was identified with the factor to which it had the greatest loading.

Twelve items combined to form factor one. Items 12, 21, 31, and 36 were from

Blame and Punishment, items 10, 15, 24, and 29 from Perfectionistic Expectations, two

items, 27 and 32 from Vigilance, and another two, 38 and 4 from All or Nothing

Thinking. By looking at the individual items a theme or category is not readily apparent.

However, this is the strongest factor, explaining 23.72% of the variance.

For factor two, Demandfor Attention, six of the seven original items loaded most

significantly on the target factor, plus item 35 from Perfectionistic Expectations. But,

item 25, “After my partner doesn’t call me once when promised, I just know we are going

to have communication problems” did not significantly load on this factor. The

eigenvalue of .299 falls below the lenient cutoff of .30 but this was the factor to which

item 25 had the largest loading. Despite the fact that all Demandfor Attention items hit

the target factor, this factor only explained 4.88% of total variance.

Factor three most closely resembles the subscale of Vigilance and accounted for

4.12% o f the variance. Six o f the original ten items from the Vigilance scale loaded most

significantly to this factor. An additional three items, 9, 19, and 41 from the subscale All

or Nothing Thinking combined to form this factor.

Factor four was comprised o f 4 more items from All or Nothing Thinking, 2 items

from Vigilance, and 1 item from Blame and Punishment. It contributed 2.60% to the

variance. This factor cannot be called All or Nothing Thinking since 5 of the 9 factors

92

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
that make up that subscale loaded most significantly to other factors. The fifth factor was

equally split with 3 items from Blame and Punishment and another 3 from Perfectionistic

Expectations. It accounted for only 2.29% of the variance.

The results of the factors identified in the factor analysis account for only 37.62%

o f variance in the sample. While the five factors identified have some loose similarities

to the factors postulated by Sullivan (1997) a five factor structure did not emerge.

In order to see the results of the RBS factor analysis and PCA and to compare

them with Sullivan’s results refer to Table 10. On this table, Sullivan’s original factors

are shown along with the items that comprise that scale on the actual test. The loading

achieved in Sullivan’s study is shown next to the loadings derived in this study on both

the factor analysis and the PCA. Items that did not significantly load at the .30 level are

indicated by an asterisk. There were 16 items, of 42 that did not significantly load toward

the factors that Sullivan found. Another 7 items loaded significantly toward two or more

factors. It is important to note on Table 10 that with only a few exceptions, Sullivan

achieved higher loadings than either of the two analyses performed in this study. In his

exploratory factor analysis, Sullivan’s loadings are larger for 34 items. Compared to the

PCA, his loadings are more significant for 32 of the items. The factor analysis done to

replicate Sullivan’s was the weakest of the matches. Both the factor analysis and the

PCA failed to support the five factor model postulated by Sullivan. Possible reasons for

these differences will be explained in Chapter Five.

Testing the Null Hypothesisfo r Research Question Four

Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the Relationship Belief Inventory

(RBI), will not predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale.

93

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 10

RBS Loadings from Sullivan's Study, Factor Analysis and Principle Component Analysis
RBS Factor RBS Item Sullivan’s FA PCA
->
Vigilance .406 .314 .392

8 .6 6 6 .451 .531

13 .617 .541 .525

18 .531 .301 .401

22 .483 * *

27 .430 * *

32 .382 * *

37 .494 .414 .534

40 .467 .489 .523

42 .503 .440 .482

Blame & Punishment 2 .527 .396 .492

7 .667 .463 .474

12 .538 * *

17 .641 .499 .544

21 .559 * *

26 .559 * .322

31 .453 * *

36 .278 * *

All or Nothing Thinking 4 .338 * *

9 .560 * *

14 .578 .321 .413

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 10 continued

19 .496 * *

23 .648 .675 .750

28 .601 .475 .606

33 .528 .589 .6 6 6

38 .356 * *

41 .465 * *

Perfectionistic Expectations 5 .530 * *

10 .546 .413 .462

15 .542 .496 .518

20 .360 * *

24 .574 .421 .476

29 .542 .631 .616

34 .542 .315 .453

35 .395 * *

Demand for Attention 1 .328 .655 .703

6 .591 .628 .617

11 .395 .526 .601

16 .464 .705 .721

25 .397 * .361

30 .544 .465 .529

39 .343 .511 .544

* This item did not significantly load on this factor at the .30 level

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Bivariate correlation coefficients were generated among the scale scores of the RBI and

the global score o f the DAS. A stepwise multiple regression analysis was conducted

using the scale scores as predictor variables and the global score of the DAS, as the

criterion measure. An alpha level o f .05 was used to determine significance.

Inspections o f the bivariate correlations (See Table 11) reveal that 4 of the 5 subscales

significantly negatively correlate with the DAS total score at the .05 level.

Disagreement is Destructive (r = -.368), Partners Cannot Change (r = -.338),

Mindreading is Expected (r = -. 139), and Sexes are Different (r -. 136) were all

significant predictors of the DAS total score. The RBI subscale score of Sexual

Perfectionism did not significantly predict the DAS total score. This indicates that people,

who believe disagreement is destructive, believe their partners cannot change for the

better, or expect their partner to read their mind without communicating, tend to have less

happy marriages. Expecting perfect sex was not a predictor of marital adjustment.

Overall, the RBI Total Score (r = -.302) significantly correlated with the DAS at

the .01 level indicating that the RBI is significantly negatively correlated with marital

satisfaction. This means that higher levels of irrational beliefs, as measured by the RBI,

predicted poor marital adjustment.

A best fitting model was selected from the stepwise multiple regression analysis,

in which R = .458, F (3,293) = 25.943, p = .000 (See Table 12). Significant RBI scales

that predicted the DAS total were Disagreement is Destructive, I - -5.918, p = .000,

Partners Cannot Change, t =-4.82, p = .0 0 0 , and Sexual Perfectionism, t = 2.374,/? =

.018.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 11

Correlation Matrix fo r RBI Subscales and DAS Total Score

DAS Total RBI D RBI M RBI C RBI S RBI MF RBI Total

_ 3 0 2 **

O
CO
DAS Total 1 -.368** -.139** -.338** -.136*

r
RBI D 1 .441** .288** .344** .262** .735**

RBIM 1 .385** .218** .166** .664**

RBI C 1 .176** .237** .591**

RBI S 1 .258** .640**

RBI MF 1 .611**

RBI Total 1

Note. RBI D = Disagreement is Destructive, RBI M = Mindreading is Expected,


RBI C = Partners Cannot Change, RBI S = Sexual Perfectionism, and RBI MF = The
Sexes are Different.
* p , .05. **p, .01.

Disagreement is Destructive, (R2 = 1 3 6 ) accounted for approximately 13% of the total

variance in the DAS score with Partners Cannot Change (R2 = .059) and Sexual

Perfectionism (R2 = .015) adding approximately 6 % and 2% respectively, of the total

variance. This suggests that respondents, who are afraid of disagreement, believe their

partners cannot change for the better, and have high expectations regarding sexual

performance tend to report less marital satisfaction than other respondents. These results

should be considered exploratory since approximately 21% of the variance in the DAS

was explained by this model and these three predictors.

97

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 12

Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis fo r RBI subscales and Dyadic


Adjustment

Model 3

Variable B SEB P R2 t sig


Change

W
uo
Disagreement -.768 .130 .136 -5.918

00
1
.0 0 0

is
Destructive

Partners
Cannot -.822 .169 -.265 .059 -4.862 .0 0 0

Change

Sexual
Perfectionism .301 0.127 .132 .015 2.374 .018

Testing the Null Hypothesisfo r Research Question Five

Irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by the Relationship Belief Scale

(RBS) will not predict marital satisfaction, as measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale

(DAS). Bivariate correlation coefficients were generated between the scale scores of the

RBS and the global score o f the DAS (See Table 13). An alpha level o f .05 determined

significance.

At the bivariate level, the RBS Total Score (r = -.573) significantly correlated

with DAS total scores. In fact, RBS subscales, Vigilance (r = -.645), Blame and

Punishment (r = -.650), All or Nothing Thinking (r = -.332) and Perfectionistic

98

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 13

Pearson r Correlation Matrix for RBS Subscales and DAS Total Score

DAS RBS (V) RBS (B) RBS (A) RBS (P) RBS (D) RBS Total

DAS 1 -.645** -.650** _ 3 3 2 ** -.406** -.140* -.573**

RBS (V) 1 .710** .554** .549** .357** .823**

RBS (B) 1 .520** .669** .372** .850**

RBS (A) 1 .508** .438** .728**

RBS (P) 1 .596** .846**

RBS (D) 1 .687**

RBS Total 1

Note. RBS (V) = Vigilance, RBS (B) = Blame and Punishment, RBS (A) = All or Nothing Thinking, RBS

(P) = Perfectionistic Expectations, RBS (D) = Demand for Attention.

* p , .05. * * p . .01

99

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Expectations (r = -.406) were all significantly negatively correlated with the DAS Total

Score at the .01 level. Demandfor Attention (r = -.140) was significantly correlated

with the DAS total score at the .05 level.

Results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis produced a model, in which

R = .715, F (3,293) = 102.441,/? = .000 (See Table 14). Three subscales, Blame and

Punishment, t = - 7.220, p = .000, Vigilance, t = -6.839, p = .000, and Demand fo r

Attention, t = -3.629 , p - .000, combined to be significantly predict the DAS Total

Score. Scores on the subscale Blame and Punishment (R2 = .422) accounted for 42% of

the variance in the DAS score. Vigilance (R2 = .068) added 7% of the variance. Demand

fo r Attention (R2= .022) accounted for another 2%. Fifty-one per cent of the variance in

the DAS total score could be accounted for by sample responses on these three subscales.

This indicates that people who blame and punish their partners, and are vigilant in

tracking their partner’s actions, and also have a high demand for attention, tend to report

lower levels of marital satisfaction.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question 6(a)

The demographic variables o f gender, age, education level, years married, times

married, household income, parental divorce and position in fam ily life cycle, will not

predict endorsement o f irrational beliefs, as measured by the total scores o f the

Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI). Bivariate correlations were produced between the

demographic factors and the total score of the RBI. The .05 level was used to determine

significance. There were no demographic factors that significantly correlated with the

RBI total score (See Table 15). A stepwise multiple regression analysis was performed

using the predictor variables o f gender, age, education level, years married, times

100

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 14

Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis fo r RBS subscales and Dyadic


Adjustment

Model 3

Variable B SEB P R2 t sig


Change

Blame & -.887 .123 -.425 .422 -7.220 .0 0 0

Punishment

Vigilance
-.817 .1 2 0 -.400 .068 -6.839 .0 0 0

Demand for
Attention .393 .108 .161 .0 2 2 3.629 .0 0 0

married, household income, parental divorce and position in family life cycle and the RBI

total score as the criterion variable. The stepwise multiple regression analysis failed to

provide a significant model. This indicates that with this sample, no demographic factors

or combination o f factors predicted endorsement of irrational beliefs about relationships

as measured by the RBI.

The demographic factor of religion was omitted from this test because the 3

categories o f LDS, Catholic, and Other did not comprise a continuous scale. In order to

test if religion was a factor the sample means were compared for religion from the three

religious categories, LDS, Catholic, and Other. There were no significant differences in

the mean scores.

101

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 15

Demographic Factors and Pearson r Correlations fo r RBI and RBS Total Score

RBI Total RBS Total

Age -.003 -.076

Gender -.019 -.020

Education -.087 .013

Household Income -.114 -.038

Times Married .025 .014

Years Married .013 -.079

Family Life Stage .025 -.021

Parental Divorce -.090 -.067

Note: No significance.
*p < .05. **p < .01.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question 6(b)

The demographic variables o f gender, age, education level, years married,

household income, parental divorce and position in fam ily life cycle, will not predict

endorsement o f irrational beliefs, as measured by the total scores o f the Relationship

Belief Scale (RBS). There were no significant correlations at the bivariate level (See

Table 15).

102

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table 16

Summary>o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis

RBI Subscales, Demographic Factors and Dyadic Adjustment

Model 4

Variable B SEB P R2 t sig


Change

Disagreement -.752 .129 -.333 .141 -5.845 .000


Is Destructive

Partners -.843 .168 -.273 .059 -5.022 .000


Cannot Change

Sexual .275 .125 .122 .014 2.201 .029


Perfectionism

Education -1.673 .840 -.104 .011 -1.992 .047

A stepwise multiple regression analysis was performed using the predictor

variables o f gender, age, education level, household income, years married, times

married, parental divorce and position in family life cycle and the RBS total score as the

criterion variable. The stepwise multiple regression analysis did not reveal a significant

model. Similar to question 6a, no demographic factors or combinations o f factors were

found to predict irrational beliefs as measured by the RBS. The .05 level, two-tailed was

used to determine significance.

103

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Testing the Null Hypothesis for Research Question Seven (a)

There are no combinations o f demographic factors and irrational beliefs, as

measured by the Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI) that predict marital satisfaction, as

measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). A stepwise multiple regression

analysis was performed using the subscales of the RBI and the demographic variables as

predictor variables and marital satisfaction, as measured by the global score on the DAS,

as the criterion variable. Significance was determined using the .05 level, two-tailed.

The stepwise multiple regression analysis produced model 4 where R = .475, F

(4,290) = 21.078,/? = .000 (See Table 16). The factors which combined were

Disagreement is Destructive, t = -5.845, p = .000, Partners Cannot Change, 1 = -5.022, p

= .000, Sexual Perfectionism, t = 2.201, p = .029, and Education, t = -1.992,p = .047.

Since the intent of this research question was to discover if demographic factors combine

with the irrational beliefs to significantly predict marital satisfaction, Model 4 was chosen

because it was the only combination that revealed a demographic factor having any

significant predicting quality. When the RBI subscale scores Disagreement is

Destructive (R2 = .141), Partners Cannot Change (R2 = .059), and Sexual Perfectionism

(R2 = .014) were combined with Education level (R2 =.011), these factors accounted for

22% of the total variance in the DAS score.

All 4 o f the significant subscales, Disagreement is Destructive, Partners Cannot

change and Sexual Perfectionism were negatively correlated meaning that endorsing

these subscales tends to predict lower levels of marital satisfaction.

It is interesting to note the demographic factor Education is negatively correlated

with Dyadic Adjustment. Meaning, higher levels of education actually predicted lower

104

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
levels of reported marital satisfaction. Individuals, who believe disagreement is

destructive, and that their partners cannot change for the better, have high expectations

for sexual performance, and are more educated, tend to be less happily adjusted in their

marriages. Given the overall R2 these results are minimal, and one should be cautious in

making interpretations.

Testing the Null Hypothesis fo r Research Question Seven (b)

There are no combinations o f demographic factors and irrational beliefs, as

measured by the Relationship Belief Scale (RBS) that predict marital satisfaction, as

measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). A stepwise multiple regression

analysis was performed using both the subscales of the RBS and the demographic

variables as predictor variables and marital satisfaction, as measured by the global score

on the DAS, as the criterion variable.

The stepwise multiple regression analysis produced a significant model in which

R = .731, F (5,289) = 66.484, p = .000 (See Table 17). In this model Vigilance,

t = -6.810, p = .000, Blame and Punishment, t - -7.224. p = .000, Demandfor Attention,

t —3.827, p = .000, Education, t = -2.710,p = .007 and Gender t = -2.043,p = .042 all

contributed to predicting the DAS total score.

The five factors, Vigilance (R2 = .425), Blame and Punishment (R2 = .072),

Demand fo r Attention (R2 = .021), Education (R2= .010), and Gender (R2 = .007), account

for approximately 52% of the total variance on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale. Vigilance,

Blame and Punishment and Demandfor Attention were all negatively correlated

indicating that high levels o f endorsement of these beliefs predict lower levels of marital

satisfaction. Education is negatively correlated indicating that those people with higher

105

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
levels o f education tended to report lower levels of marital satisfaction. Gender is also

negatively correlated. Since women were coded as “ 1” and men as “2”, this indicates

that men reported less marital satisfaction than women. Again, the factor of gender

makes only modest contributions to the variance in this model. These results should be

considered exploratory.

Table 17

Summary o f Stepwise Multiple Regression Analysis RBS Subscales, Demographic

Factors and Dyadic Adjustment

Model 5

Variable B SEB P R2 t sig


Change

Vigilance -.812 .119 -.401 .425 -6.810 .000

Blame &
Punishment -.892 .124 -.432 .072 -7.224 .000

Demand for
Attention .407 .106 .168 .021 3.827 .000

Education -1.775 .655 -.110 .010 -2.710 .007

Gender -2.374 1.162 -.085 .007 -2.043 .042

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Summary of Results

Results o f Research Question One

These two instruments, the RBI and RBS are significantly correlated. The total

scores of each were significantly correlated with each other. All the subscales of the RBI

were significantly correlated with the RBS total score. All the subscales of the RBS were

significantly correlated with the RBI total score. Furthermore, all of the subscales of the

RBS significantly correlated with all of the RBI subscales, with only one exception. The

RBI sub scale The Sexes are Different was not significantly correlated with the RBS

subscale Demand fo r Attention at the .01 level but was significant at the .05 level. Two

RBI subscales, The Sexes are Different and Sexual Perfectionism had lower levels of

correlation with many o f the subscale scores on both tests.

The RBI and RBS are related to one another. The RBI and RBS are significantly

correlated and appear to be measuring similar constructs. This strengthens and validates

the construct validity of both tests.

Results o f Research Question Two

This question sought to determine if a five factor structure would emerge from the

RBI from this sample. A Principle Component Analysis (PC A) was performed. Results

from the PCA provided weak support for the five factor model proposed by Epstein and

Eidelson (1982). While thirty-three of 40 items (83%) loaded significantly on the test

factors as designed, the subscales Disagreement is Destructive and Mindreading is

Expected evidenced substantial variability.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Results o f Research Question Three

This question sought to determine if a five factor structure would emerge from the

RBS scores from this sample. A principle component analysis was performed forcing the

data into five factors. Some items were consistently loading on factors similar to the

results obtained by Sullivan but there was enough discrepancy to warrant a second

statistic, an Exploratory Factor Analysis. The intent was to replicate the final study done

by Sullivan in his exploratory study. Again, the results pointed to variability in the

subscales.

Sixteen items did not significantly load toward the factors identified by Sullivan.

Only two subscales Demandfor Attention and Vigilance were recognizable as the target

factors from the RBS. Items from the other three RBS factors were spread throughout the

remaining factors achieved by the exploratory factor analysis. The factors themselves did

not emerge in the same order as they had in the exploratory factor analysis performed by

Sullivan. Results o f the factor analysis and PCA do not support the five factor structure

of the RBS.

Results o f Research Question Four

This question sought to determine if irrational relationship beliefs, as measured by

the Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI), would predict marital satisfaction, as measured

by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). At the bivariate level four of the subscales.

Disagreement is Destructive, Partners Cannot Change, Mindreading is Expected, and

Sexes are Different were all significant predictors of scores on the DAS. People who

strongly endorsed these beliefs tended to be less happy in their marriages.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The RBI total score was also significantly correlated with the DAS, indicating

that people who consistently endorsed high levels of irrational beliefs on the RBI tended

to report less happy marriages.

Results of the multivariate regression analysis also confirmed these results. When

combined, the responses on the sub scales of Disagreement is Destructive, Partners

Cannot Change and Sexual Perfectionism modestly predicted less happy marriages.

Results o f Research Question Five

The purpose o f this question was to determine if irrational beliefs, as measured by

the Relationship Belief Scale (RBS) would correlate with marital adjustment, as

measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). At the bivariate level, 4 of the 5

subscales, Vigilance, Blame and Punishment, All or Nothing Thinking, and Perfectionistic

Expectations were all significantly negatively correlated with the results of the DAS.

This means that people who responded with high levels of agreement to these irrational

beliefs tended to report lower levels of marital happiness. The RBS total score was also

negatively correlated with the DAS score.

At the multivariate level Blame and Punishment, Vigilance, and Demand fo r

Attention accounted for a significant portion of the variance. People who endorsed high

levels o f these 3 sub scales, tended to report low levels o f marital satisfaction.

Results o f Research Question Six (a)

Question 6(a) sought to determine if any o f the demographic factors were

significant predictors o f irrational beliefs, as measured by the RBI. At the bivariate level,

there were no demographic factors that significantly correlated with the RBI Total Score.

A stepwise multiple regression analysis was attempted using all o f the demographic

109

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
factors except Religion. The regression failed to produce a significant model. This

indicates that in this study, the demographic factors of age, gender, household income,

education, years married, position in family life cycle, number of times married, and

parental divorce, did not predict a participant’s level of irrational beliefs, as measured by

the Relationship Belief Inventory (RBI)

Results o f Research Question Six (b)

This question was similar to the above except the Relationship Belief Scale (RBS)

was used to measure the level o f irrational belief. Once again, there was no significant

correlation between the demographic factors and the RBS total score. Another stepwise

multiple regression analysis was performed, however it did not produce a significant

model. For this sample, although there was variance in the levels of irrational beliefs

participants held, the demographic factors did not correlate with or predict those scores.

Results o f Research Question Seven (a)

This question sought to determine if the demographic factors would combine with

any irrational beliefs to predict marital quality. Irrational beliefs were measured by the

subscale scores on the RBI, i.e. Disagreement is Destructive, Mindreading is Expected,

Partners Cannot Change, Sexual Perfectionism, and The Sexes are Different. Marital

quality was measured by the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS). In this study, the subscale

scores on Disagreement is Destructive, Partners Cannot Change, and Sexual

Perfectionism, combined with the demographic factor of Education to significantly

predict marital satisfaction as measured by the DAS. This suggests that highly educated

people who believe that disagreement is destructive to marriage, that partners are not

capable o f change, and who expect perfection in sexual intimacy tend to be less happy in

110

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
their marriages. The significance of these results is minimal and caution should be used

before generalizing these results to another population.

Results o f Research Question Seven (b)

This question is similar to 7(a) but the irrational beliefs were measured by the

subscale scores on the RBS, i.e., Vigilance, Blame and Punishment, All or Nothing

Thinking, Perfectionistic Expectations, and Demandfor Attention. Results indicate that

the combination o f Education, Gender, Vigilance, Blame and Punishment, and Demand

fo r Attention combined to significantly predict poor marital satisfaction. This means that

highly educated males, who are vigilant in watching their partners, demand a lot of

attention, attribute blame and punishment to their spouses, tended to report lower levels

of marital satisfaction. The relationship was small and so these results should be used

and interpreted cautiously.

Ill

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER V

Discussion

Introduction

Cognitive behavioral psychology focuses on changing the faulty, erroneous or

irrational beliefs that people hold. When irrational thoughts are replaced with rational

ones the resulting behavior tends to be more health promoting (Beck & Emery, 1985;

Ellis, 1994; Meichenbaum, 1977). Researchers have drawn upon cognitive behavioral

models of psychology in an effort to improve the quality of marriage and to predict

unhappy marriages.

Numerous authors have looked at the impact of irrational beliefs on marriage

(Bradbury & Fincham, 1990; Ellis & Harper, 1961; Larson, 1992; Meichenbaum, 1977;

Schwebel & Sullivan, 1996) and note that individuals who endorse irrational beliefs

about their relationships, often experience difficulty adjusting to marriage. These

individuals also tend to report less marital satisfaction than people who endorse more

rational beliefs (Baucom et al, 1989; Schwebel & Sullivan, 1996).

Two instruments designed to assess these irrational beliefs are the Relationship

Belief Inventory (RBI, Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) and the Relationship Belief Scale

(RBS, Sullivan, 1997). The RBI has been widely used to assess irrational relationship

beliefs but the psychometric strengths and reliability of this test have been questioned

(Bradbury & Fincham, 1993; Emmelkamp et al., 1987; James et al., 2002; Sullivan

1997). As a result, Sullivan (1997) designed the RBS to serve as an alternative

instrument to the RBI. Sullivan’s initial studies provide evidence that the RBS may be

the preferred instrument for assessing irrational relationship beliefs.

112

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The purpose of this study was to help further define the relationship between

irrational relationship beliefs and marital adjustment. Furthermore, it sought to explore

the strength of the two instruments designed to measure irrational relationship beliefs.

Specifically, it focused on the robustness of the factor structure of each test with the

sample used in this study and helps determine which of these instruments is more suited

for future research. Additionally, this study assesses the relationship between irrational

relationship beliefs and the following demographic factors; age, gender, years married,

times married, household income, education, position in the family life cycle, and

parent’s divorce.

Population and Sample

There were several factors that combined to create a highly homogenous sample.

Overall, the participants could be characterized as religious, committed to marriage,

satisfied with their marriage, well educated, upper middle class, and mature. Although

subscale and overall test score means indicated no statistical differences between these

participants and other samples, the relevance and application of this study may be

limited.

Much of the research on irrational relationship beliefs has used unmarried people

in dating or co-habitating relationships (James, 2002; Larson, 1992; Sullivan 1997). The

target population for this study was non-clinical, married people. Participants in this

sample indicated that they had not sought marriage therapy in the last twelve months and

most of the participants reported high levels of marital satisfaction as measured by the

Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS).

For reasons o f convenience and interest, the sample was drawn from the greater

113

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Salt Lake City, Utah area and represents the population of the state. Utah was first

populated by Mormon pioneers in 1847. The name “Mormon” is a nickname for

members of The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints, respectfully referred to as

LDS. Currently, the LDS population represents 75% of the total Utah population (The

Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints, 2005). Since a resounding number of people

in Utah are LDS it was expected that LDS people would be a large part of the sample.

Despite the LDS conservative emphasis on stable marriages and families (The Church of

Jesus Christ o f Latter Day Saints, 2005), the LDS respondent’s scores on the RBI, RBS

and the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS, the measure of marital adjustment) were similar

to non LDS participants in this sample. LDS people in this sample did not endorse

significantly more or less irrational beliefs nor did they report being more or less happily

married than non-LDS participants. Despite the lack of difference in mean scores on the

measures used in this study, it is possible that the homogenous sample may have

impacted the results of the study.

The sample was drawn from two general sources. The first source was made up

of 225 (76%) individuals attending psycho educational lectures, who were presented with

this research study and who indicated a willingness to participate in the study. These

people were given a questionnaire packet, which they completed at home, and mailed

back to the researcher anonymously, within 2 weeks. The second source was a

convenient sample o f 72 individuals known to the researcher. These individuals were

also given a questionnaire packet to complete at home and mail back to the researcher

anonymously.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Research Question One

The RBI and RBS purport to measure the construct of irrational relationship

beliefs. Question one sought to determine if the RBI and RBS measure similar constructs

and the extent of the correlation that exists between the subscales of each test and the

total scores on the RBI and RBS.

Results o f the Pearson r indicate a significant degree of correlation between the

RBI and RBS total scores. With only one exception, every subscale of the RBI was

significantly correlated with every subscale on the RBS ip <01). The exception was the

RBI subscale The Sexes are Different which correlated significantly with the RBS

subscale Demandfor Attention but at a more lenient level of significance ip < .05). These

results indicate that these two tests are related and appear to be measuring a similar

construct of irrational or unrealistic relationship beliefs.

While almost all the subscales were related to one another, the RBS subscales

had a stronger correlation to the RBI total score than the other way around. This suggests

that the RBI may be measuring constructs not measured by the RBS. The RBS does not

assess gender attitudes or sexual expectations represented by the scales The Sexes are

Different and Sexual Perfectionism (Sullivan, 1997).

One RBI scale that appears to have a weaker relationship as compared to the other

subscales was The Sexes are Different. This subscale was originally thought to measure

the degree to which someone believed that men and women have different ways of

interacting and different needs within intimate relationships (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982).

According to the RBI authors, endorsing this irrational belief keeps couples polarized

over their differences, and distracts them from making changes that could increase their

115

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
adjustment. The sample mean achieved in this study was significantly higher (1 standard

deviation) on RBI scale The Sexes are Different than the original normative sample (See

Table 4). Participants in this study endorsed statements such as “Men and women

probably will never understand the opposite sex very well” and “Men and women will

always be mysteries to each other”. One possible explanation for this finding could be

that the majority o f participants were LDS, and more likely to hold traditional attitudes

regarding the roles of men and women (The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints,

1995). According to test designers (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) this scale, The Sexes are

Different, is anticipated to be negatively correlated with marital adjustment, meaning the

more you endorse the belief that men and women are different the less happily married

you should be. Question 4 provides further discussion related to this scale’s ability to

predict marital satisfaction.

The weakness found in this scale was consistent with the Emmelkamp study

(1987) where high endorsement of the scale The Sexes are Different actually predicted

the random sample o f non-clinical participants over the clinical one. This finding is in

direct contrast to what test authors Eidelson and Epstein (1982) found in their study. This

phenomenon might be better explained by the attitudes reflected in the popular self-help

Mars vs. Venus literature (Cowlishaw, 2001; Grey, 1992). It is possible that endorsing

the belief that The Sexes are Different helps to lower expectations people have of one

another. If differences are attributed to gender it may prevent blaming and possibly

normalize conflict thereby increasing positive adjustment.

Research Question Two

The RBI was created in 1982 by Eidelson and Epstein and has been used in

116

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
numerous studies to assess irrational beliefs in romantic relationships. However, the RBI

was created using a rational approach without the benefit of factor analysis (Eidelson &

Epstein, 1982). Although the RBI has been shown to be related to marital satisfaction, the

actual validity o f the scales and items contributing to them are in question. Are the total

scale items and subscales valid and reliable? This is one of the most significant questions

proposed by this study. Only one published study has examined the reliability o f the RBI

using factor analysis (James et al, 2002). Results of the James study indicate serious

weakness in two of the subscales, The Sexes are Different and Partners Cannot Change.

This study sought to determine the reliability of the RBI subscales by using

principle component analysis (PCA) to determine if the items would emerge in five

distinct components as proposed by test authors and account for a significant portion of

the variance in scores.

There were two methods used to examine the results from the PCA. The first

method determined if each test item loaded significantly on its target subscale. Thirty -

three of 40 items (83%) loaded significantly toward the factor proposed by the RBI

authors. The seven items that failed to load were 2, 7, 12, and 37 from subscale

Mindreading is Expected, item 23 from Partners Cannot Change and items 9 and 14

from the Sexual Perfectionism subscale.

In this study the cutoff point used for significant item loadings for the PCA and

factor analysis was >.30. Tabachnik and Fidell (2001) suggest that .32 be used as the

minimum cut off for factor loadings. This lower .30 was chosen because Sullivan (1997)

had used this cutoff in designing the RBS and it made comparisons easier and consistent

across the board.

117

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
A second method used to examine results of the PCA was to determine if items

loaded most significantly on its target subscale. On the RBI, three items 3, 22, and 27

loaded significantly on 2 or more subscales. Item 9 did not load on any scale. When the

items were grouped with the factors to which they had the highest loading (see Table 6),

five factors emerged, but they were not the same five factors that made up the RBI.

All 8 items from scale Disagreement is Destructive and six items from

Mindreading is Expected formed a new component in the PCA. This new component

consisted o f 14 items, and was the largest contributing component for the RBI and

explained the most variance (16.95%). The theme of these items was related to poor

communication skills. For instance, RBI item 21 from Disagreement is Destructive states

“I get very upset when my partner and I cannot see things the same way” and RBI item

12 from Mindreading is Expected says “I get very upset if my partner does not recognize

how I am feeling and I have to tell him/her”.

The strength of the sub scale Disagreement is Destructive found in this study is

similar to the results of the exploratory factor analysis performed by the James (2002)

study, in that it emerged as the strongest factor accounting for the most variance.

Although it must be noted that in the James study, the items from the Mindreading is

Expected subscale did not combine with the Disagreement is Destructive subscale.

The subscale The Sexes are Different was the only subscale to remain intact with

all items loading significantly, and primarily towards the target subscale. This finding

contrasts with the results from the James (2002) study in which The Sexes are Different

split into two different factors; one that was described as Sexes are Different in Needs and

another labeled Difficulty in Understanding the Opposite Sex. As discussed earlier

118

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
participants in this sample scored significantly higher on The Sexes are Different than the

normative sample. This is possibly due to sampling reasons.

RBI subscales Sexual Perfectionism, and Partners Cannot Change had almost all

their items load most significantly on the target component. However, the contribution to

the variance was minimal (7.06% and 5.69%, respectively) for these two scales. This

finding differs from the James study results in which several items from scale Partners

Cannot Change failed to load significantly on the target subscale.

To summarize, in this study, the items from two subscales Disagreement is

Destructive and Mindreading is Expected combined to form a new factor. The items that

loaded on the remaining three scales were similar to the factors reported by Eidelson and

Epstein (1982). However, the total variance explained by these loadings on the three

factors was modest and results of the PCA provide weak support for the five factor

structure proposed by Eidelson and Epstein (1982) for the RBI.

Research Question Three

For his dissertation, Sullivan (1997) created the Relationship Belief Scale using

exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis. His study results indicated

that the RBS was a superior instrument to the RBI. Question three was designed to

determine if a similar five factor structure would emerge for the RBS in a different

sample group.

Initially, a Principle Component Analysis (PCA) was performed on the RBS and

results indicated poor support for the five factors found by Sullivan (1997). Specifically,

15 of the 42 items (36%) failed to load significantly towards the target subscale. The

only subscale to remain relatively intact was Demandfor Attention. The seven items that

119

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
comprise this subscale loaded most significantly to form the second strongest component.

This is in contrast to Sullivan’s study in which Demandfor Attention was found to be the

weakest o f his five factors. The strongest component to emerge and the one that

accounted for the most variance (25.05%) was one comprised of fifteen various items

from four of the subscales. There was no common theme that described these items.

Since the results for the PCA were inconclusive, a decision was made to use

exploratory factor analysis to further investigate the factor structure for the RBS. The

factor analysis produced results similar to those achieved by the PCA. Seventeen of the

42 items (40%) did not load significantly towards the expected subscale. Once again, the

subscale Demand for Attention was the most consistent with the original subscale. Items

from subscales Vigilance, All or Nothing Thinking, Blame and Punishment and

Perfectionistic Expectations showed substantial variability and failed to load consistently

with the original factors. A new factor comprised of twelve items from four of the

various RBS subscales emerged as the strongest factor. Upon scrutiny o f these items, a

common theme could not be determined.

Neither the factor analysis nor the principle component analysis supported a five

factor structure as proposed by Sullivan. Table 10 compares the loadings derived from

the Sullivan study to the loadings achieved in this study. The loadings in the Sullivan

study are all greater except for the subscale Demandfor Attention in which this sample

achieved higher loadings. The most obvious explanation for the differences in these

results can be found in the demographic make up of the samples. Sullivan’s sample

consisted of 856 single dating people drawn from a university population plus 100

married people from the community, in contrast to this study which was made up of 297

120

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
married people, primarily from an LDS background. One possible explanation for this

finding could be assumed from Schwebel’s (1992) work which postulates that unmarried

individuals often have unrealistic and high expectations of marriage. Additionally,

Sullivan (1997) found that the mean scores on the RBS subscales were greater for his

unmarried participants than for the married individuals, indicating that unmarried people

endorsed more irrational beliefs.

Research Question Four

Question four aimed at assessing if irrational relationship beliefs, measured by the

RBI would predict marital satisfaction for this sample. Previous research has indicated

that people who endorse irrational beliefs tend to report less marital satisfaction (Baucom

et al., 1989; Eidelson & Epstein, 1982). Scores on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale Total

(DAS) were used to assess a participant’s level o f marital adjustment. Results from this

sample, using the Pearson r indicate that the RBI total score was significantly negatively

related to marital satisfaction. As such, individuals who were more likely to endorse a

greater number o f irrational beliefs were also more likely to report lower levels of marital

satisfaction. The significant negative correlation between irrational relationship beliefs

and marital adjustment found in this study is consistent with results from other studies

(Baucom et al., 1989; Bradbury & Fincham 1990; Eidelson & Epstein, 1982; Sullivan

1997).

Individual subscale scores were also examined to determine their relationship to

marital adjustment. Disagreement is Destructive, Partners Cannot Change, Mindreading

is Expected, and The Sexes are Different were all significantly negatively correlated with

marital adjustment. This finding suggests that people who anticipate disagreement to be

121

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
harmful, believe that partners never change, expect their partners to read their minds, or

believe that men and women have different needs in relationships tend to also report

lower levels of marital satisfaction. The subscale Sexual Perfectionism did not

significantly predict marital quality. This weakness found in the Sexual Perfectionism

scale is similar to the findings in the Bradbury and Fincham (1993) study but dissimilar

to the Moller and Van Zyl (1991) study done in Russia.

Results of the multiple regression analysis found that the only significant

combination o f RBI subscales in predicting marital adjustment was Disagreement is

Destructive, Partners Cannot Change, and Sexual Perfectionism. This indicates that

people, who believe that disagreement is harmful, think their partners can’t change, and

expect perfect sexual performance tend to report less happy marriages. The shared

variance of these three variables is only 21%, so caution should be used in the application

of these findings to other populations.

Also the Pearson r co-efficients for the RBI predicting the DAS were significant,

but not high. The range o f co-efficients/or Disagreement is Destructive, Mindreading is

Expected, Partners Cannot Change, Sexual Perfectionism, and The Sexes are Different

for predicting marital adjustment as measured by the DAS are -.368, -.139, -.338, -.031,

-. 136, respectively. The RBI Total score Pearson r was -.302 (see Table 11). Similar

results for the RBS are identified in the next section.

Research Question Five

Question five sought further evidence to support the relationship between

irrational relationship beliefs as measured by the RBS and marital satisfaction. The

resulting Pearson r indicated that the RBS total score significantly predicts marital

122

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
quality, meaning that people who endorsed the irrational beliefs on the RBS tended to

report lower levels o f marital adjustment. All five of the RBS subscales, were

significantly negatively correlated with the DAS total score. This finding suggests that

individuals who are vigilantly watching their partners, blaming and punishing their

partners, look at problems in their relationship in “all or nothing” terms, expect perfection

from their spouse and demand attention from them, tend to report less happy marriages.

At the multivariate level of analysis, the stepwise multiple regression analysis

revealed one combination o f subscales that appeared to predict unhappy marriages.

Blame and Punishment, Vigilance, and Demand fo r Attention combined to significantly

predict poor marital adjustment. Results of the multivariate predictions indicate that

these three variables account for 51% of the variance in the DAS scores. But, 42% of

this comes from subscale Blame and Punishment while the other two variables make

minimal contributions. Caution should be used in generalizing these findings outside of

married, religious populations.

It appears that the RBI and RBS were shown to be significantly related to marital

satisfaction. However, the correlations were larger for the RBS than for the RBI. The

Pearson r coefficients for Vigilance, Blame & Punishment, All or Nothing Thinking,

Perfectionistic Expectations and Demandfor Attention were -.645, -.650, -.332, -.406, -

. 140, respectively. The RBS Total score Pearson r coefficient was -.573 (see Table 11).

These findings suggest that the RBS has a stronger relationship to marital satisfaction

than the RBI.

Research Question Six

Considering the impact that irrational beliefs have on marital satisfaction it is

123

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
important to identify characteristics of people who endorse these beliefs. To date,

previous research has been inconclusive regarding demographic features and their

relationship to endorsing irrational relationship beliefs. In this study the demographic

factors of gender, age, education level, years married, times married, household income,

parent’s divorce and position in family life cycle, were used along with the scores on the

RBI and RBS to determine which demographics correlate with endorsing irrational

beliefs. The Pearson r and the stepwise regression analysis performed on this sample

indicate that none of these demographic factors are significant predictors of irrational

relationship beliefs as measured by either the RBI or RBS.

This finding is consistent with the research to date. In retrospect, this sample

could have included a non married contingent to determine if the demographic factor of

being unmarried is a better predictor of irrational relationship beliefs.

The demographic factor of religion was not included in the multivariate

regression analysis because religion was not expressed as a continuous variable.

Research Question Seven (a)

In question seven (a) the demographic factors of gender, age, education,

household income, years married, times married, position in family life cycle and

parent’s divorce were combined with the scale scores of the RBI to determine if

combinations o f factors and irrational beliefs predict marital satisfaction. For instance,

would recently married people, who endorse beliefs represented by the scale

Mindreading is Expected report less marital satisfaction? The stepwise multiple

regression analysis produced one significant model. The significant factors were

Disagreement is Destructive, Partners Cannot Change, Sexual Perfectionism, and the

124

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
demographic factor of Education which were all negatively correlated with marital

satisfaction. This finding suggests that well educated individuals who believe

disagreement is destructive, that their partners can’t change, and have high expectations

for sexual performance report being less happy in their marriages. However, the shared

variance of these four variables is only 22%. Therefore, caution should be used before

generalizing to other populations because the strength of the relationship was fairly small.

Research Question Seven (b)

This question combined the subscale scores of the RBS with all the demographic

factors to see if there was a combination that predicted poor marital adjustment. From the

stepwise multiple regression analysis, one significant model emerged containing five

factors: Vigilance, Blame and Punishment, Demandfor Attention, and demographic

factors o f Education, and Gender. All o f these factors were negatively correlated with

marital satisfaction. These results suggest that highly educated men, who are vigilantly

watching their partner, ascribing blame and punishment, and have a high demand for

attention are inclined to report less happy marriages. The variables in this model account

for 53% of the shared variance. However, scale Vigilance contributes 42% of this alone.

The contribution of the other three variables is relatively small. Although these results

show statistical significance, it is small, and caution should be used when generalizing

these results to other populations.

Strengths

Demographic Factors

This study is the first to examine specifically the demographic features of

religion, household income, years married, times married, position in the family life

125

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
cycle, and parent’s divorce with irrational relationship beliefs. Some studies have

included gender (Bradbury & Fincham, 1993; Bushman, 1999; DeBord & Romans, 1996;

Shapiro & Kroeger, 1991; Sullivan & Schwebel, 1996) but previous results have been

inconclusive.

Large sample size

The 297 participants represent the largest number of married individuals to take

both the RBI and RBS, in a published study to date. Sullivan’s study (1997) used a large

sample of unmarried college students plus a community sample of 100 married people.

The James study (2002) had 205 participants who were married or cohabitating, but these

individuals only completed the RBI.

The sample size was sufficiently large enough to perform the exploratory factor

analysis and principle component analysis as recommended by Comrey and Lee (1992)

who suggests the minimum number of cases for this type of analysis should be 300. This

study started with 307 cases but due to missing information, 10 cases were deleted from

the study. This brought the total number to 297, just 3 short of the recommendation by

Comrey and Lee. Bryant (Grimm & Yarnold, 2000) states that factor analysis could be

performed with as few as 100 observations and still be valid.

Limitations

Assumptions o f Normality

The assumption o f univariate, bivariate and multivariate normal distribution of the

sample is in question. On the RBI, 9 items contained 31 cases in which outliers were

identified. Ten items had skewness and kurtosis z scores that exceeded the customary

+- 3.29. After scrutiny of the original paper and pencil protocols only 3 of these were

126

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
altered with the sample mean of that item. The rest were all deemed representative of the

population intended by the study.

For the RBS, 86 response outliers were identified from 23 different items and

these all had skewness or kurtosis z scores that exceeded the standard. Again, all 86 were

viewed as part o f the population of the study and left as is. These outliers were created

by a response pattern intrinsic to the likert scale. For instance, one o f the RBS items

states T m so considerate to my partner, that even if I have a one-night stand it is okay.”

This item had a strong polarized response pattern. O f all participants, 273 marked

“strongly disagree”, 19 marked “disagree” and 5 people indicated “strongly agree”.

These responses are not normally distributed but dichotomous. All of the items

containing outliers were created by response patterns similar to this.

The data could not be manipulated and items could not be dropped from the

instruments because the RBI and RBS are standardized, published tests. Also results

were to be compared to other studies in which the RBI and RBS had been used in

entirety. Nevertheless, the fact that response pattern to many items did not meet the

assumptions necessary for the statistics employed puts the results in question.

The Unique and Highly Homogenous Population

Overall, the participants in this study could be described as religious, committed

to marriage, well educated, upper middle class, and mature. Although subscale and

overall test score means indicated no statistical differences between these participants and

other samples, the unique qualities o f this sample most likely are reflected in the results

of the study and therefore limit generalizability to other populations.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The LDS Population

The majority o f participants (74%) were LDS which is representative of the State

of Utah (The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints, 2005). However it does not

represent the overall population of the United States. LDS people are reported to hold

distinct cultural attitudes regarding marriage and the importance of the family (The

Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints, 1995). Therefore, caution must be used

when considering the results from this study as it may not generalize to the population at

large.

Non-Random Sample

This was not a random sample. All of the participants selected themselves for

participation. Taking a packet, filling it out in privacy, and mailing it back, with no

recompense, represents a high degree of self-motivation. Motivated responders may have

opinions that are different than the general population.

Possible Influence o f Treatment

Most of the respondents in this study received a psycho educational lecture

sometime prior to responding to the measures. These lectures focused on issues of

parenting skills, communication in the family, time management, and methods to manage

depression. It is possible that information in the lectures attuned the participant to a

response bias and represents a treatment that was not addressed or equally applied across

the board.

Like all participants, the ones from the convenient sample completed their

measures in privacy and returned them anonymously. But, they were personally known

to the researcher and may have answered in a manner to please or withhold opinions to

128

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
protect their privacy. These participants may have felt an obligation to the researcher and

this too may have colored their responses.

Implicationsfo r Practice

Clinical Use o f the RBI and RBS

This study represents a critical addition to the study o f irrational relationship

beliefs among individuals who report being in a marital relationship. It provides

additional evidence regarding the validity of the RBI and RBS instruments. Clinicians

who frequently use these instruments to assess irrational beliefs should use caution until

the reliability o f both can be further established.

The Link between Irrational Relationship Beliefs and Marital Satisfaction

Results of this study indicate that endorsing irrational relationship beliefs is

predictive of unhappy marriages. These results are consistent with previous studies.

Clients who endorse irrational relationship beliefs should be challenged and educated in

order to decrease their high and unrealistic expectations.

Demographic Factors

The results of this study suggest that the demographic factors of age, gender,

years married, times married, religion, household income, education, position in the

family life cycle and parent’s divorce, do not predict irrational relationship beliefs. These

results are consistent with other studies performed previously. Clinicians are therefore

cautioned to assess all clients for irrational or unrealistic relationship beliefs.

Specific Value fo r Therapists serving LDS Couples

The LDS participants did not score significantly higher on the RBI, RBS, or DAS

129

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
than the rest of the participants of this study. Therapists working with LDS couples can

assess and intervene regarding irrational relationship beliefs as they would with other

populations.

Implications fo r Further Research

1. Both the RBI and RBS demonstrated serious problems with internal consistency and

factor variability. More studies employing exploratory factor analysis and

confirmatory factor analysis should be performed with different samples to further

delineate the reliability of each.

2. When results o f this study are compared to unmarried samples, it appears that marital

status may be a demographic feature that predicts endorsing irrational relationship

beliefs. A comparison study that focuses primarily on irrational relationship beliefs

of married and unmarried participants would help clarify this relationship.

3. Portions of this study could be replicated with non-religious populations Although

significant differences were not found between the LDS participants in the sample

and other non-LDS participants, most sample respondents identified themselves with

a religion. It is possible that results from this study generalize to religious

populations and not necessarily LDS ones.

4. Portions of this study could be replicated with clinical participants. Most of the

respondents in this sample reported being relatively happily married. It is possible

that a clinical population would provide more variance in response and a normally

distributed sample.

5. Future research could examine the effect of psycho educational intervention on

changing irrational beliefs. A psycho educational treatment could be applied to

130

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
participants who highly endorse irrational relationship beliefs to determine if the

beliefs are enduring.

Summary

In order to help couples increase their marital satisfaction, researchers and

practitioners have drawn upon cognitive behavioral models of psychology (Baucom, et

al., 1989; Doherty, 1997; Ellis, 1961; Epstein & Eidelson, 1981; Larson, 1992; Schwebel

& Fine, 1994). Specifically, they have investigated the link between endorsing irrational

relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction. Previous studies have found significant

negative correlation between marital satisfaction and endorsing irrational relationship

beliefs (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982; Debord & Romans, 1996; Moller & Van Zyl; 1991;

Sullivan 1997).

The purpose of this study was threefold. 1) Two instruments have been designed

to assess the level o f endorsement of some of these beliefs; The Relationship Belief

Inventory (Eidelson & Epstein, 1982) and the Relationship Belief Scale (Sullivan, 1997).

This study sought to explore the validity and reliability o f these two instruments. 2) The

relationship between irrational relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction was to be

determined for this sample. 3) The demographic features of age, gender, years married,

times married, household income, education, religion, position in the family life cycle

and parent’s divorce were considered to determine their relationship with irrational

relationship beliefs.

In summation, results from this sample indicate serious weaknesses in the factor

structure of both the RBI and RBS. These results suggest that the reliability of both

instruments is in question until further studies can establish their reliability. The RBI and

131

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
RBS appear to be related measures and are most likely measuring similar constructs.

Irrational relationship beliefs as measured by the RBI and RBS are significantly

negatively correlated to marital adjustment.

Regarding combinations of irrational beliefs that predict marital adjustment,

results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis produced some significant models

where irrational beliefs measured by the RBS and RBI combined to be significant

predictors of marital adjustment. However, results of the combinations were minimal and

caution should guide the application to other populations. There was no relationship

found between the demographic factors in this study and irrational relationship beliefs for

this sample.

The relationship between irrational relationship beliefs and marital satisfaction

has been shown in this study and repeatedly in other research. This study illustrates the

need for valid and reliable instruments to measure endorsement of irrational relationship

beliefs.

132

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
References

Addis, J. (2002). Marital Adjustment and irrational beliefs. Journal o f Rational-Emotive


& Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 20, 1, 3-13.

American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, &


National Council on Measurement in Education. (1999). Standards fo r
educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC: American Educational
Research Association.

Antill, J. K. & Cotton, S. (1982). Spanier’s Dyadic Adjustment Scale: Some


confirmatory analyses. Australian Psychologist, 17, 181-189.

Arrindell, W. A., Emmelkamp, P.M.G. & Bast, S. (1983). The Maudsley Marital
Questionnaire (M.M.Q.): A further step toward is validation. Personal and
Individual Differences, 4, 457-464.

Aub, N. & Linden, W. (1991). Marital disturbance, quality of communication and


socioeconomic status. Canadian Journal o f Behavioural Science, 23 (2), 125-
132.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bahr, S. J., & Galligan, R. J. (1984). Teenage marriage and marital stability. Youth and
Society, 15, 387-400.

Baucom, D. H., (1986). Attributions in distressed relations: How can we explain them?
In S. Duck and D. Perlman (Eds.?, Heterosexual relations, marriage, and divorce.
London: Sage Publications.

Baucom, D. H. & Epstein, N. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral marital therapy.


Philadelphia, PA. Brunner/Mazel.

Baucom, D. H., Epstein, N., Sayers, S., & Goldman-Sher, T. (1989). The role of
cognitions in marital relationships: Definitional, methodological, and conceptual
issues. Journal o f Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 57, 31-38.

Beach, S. R. & O’Leary, K. D. (1993). Marital discord and dysphoria: For whom does
the marital relationship predict depressive symptomatology? Journal o f Social
and Personal Relationships, 10, 3, 405-420.

Beck, A. T., & Emery, G. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive
perspective. New York: Basic.

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B.F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy o f
depression. New York: Guilford.

133

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Booth, A., & Edwards, J. (1985). Age at marriage and marital instability. Journal o f
Marriage and the Family, 47, 67-75.

Booth, A., Johnson D. & Edwards J. N., (1983). Measuring marital instability. Journal
o f Marriage and the Family, 44, 387-393.

Bradbury, T. N. & Fincham F. D. (1992). Attributions and Behavior in Marital


Interaction. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 613-628.

Bradbury, T. N. & Fincham F.D. (1993). Assessing dysfunctional cognition in marriage:


A reconsideration of the relationship belief inventory. Psychological Assessment,
5, 92-101.

Bryant, F. B. (2000). Assessing the Validity in Measurement, In Grim, L.G. & Yarnold,
P R. (Eds.) Reading and Understanding Multivariate Statistics. Washington, DC.
American Psychological Association.

Bunk, B. & Nijskens, J. (1980). Communication and satisfaction in intimate


relationships. Gedrag, 4, 240-460.

Burr, W. R. (1973). Theory construction and the sociology o f the family. New York:
Wiley.

Bushman, W. J. (1999). The relationship between conflict, love, and satisfaction and
relationship beliefs, problem-solving techniques and negotiating strategies in
romantic relationships. Dissertation Abstracts International'. Section B: The
Sciences & Engineering 59 (8-B) Feb 1999, 4534.

Carter, E. & McGoldrick, M. (Eds.). (1989). The family life cycle: A framework fo r
fam ily therapy. (2nd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Cate, R. M., & Lloyd S. A. (1992) Courtship, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

The Church o f Jesus Christ o f Latter Day Saints (1995). The Family: A proclamation to
the world. Salt Lake City, UT. The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter Day Saints

The Church o f Jesus Christ o f Latter Day Saints (2005). Church practices influence Utah
health and demographics statistics. Retrieved October 10, 2005 from
http:www.lds.org/newsroom/show package/0,15367,3899-1 .html

The Church o f Jesus Christ o f Latter Day Saints (2005). Why church families stay
together. Retrieved October 10, 2005 from
http://www.lds.Org/newsroom/showpackage/0,15367,3899-1 --44-2-166,00. html

Coie, J., Watt, N., Hawkins, S., Ramey, S., Markman, H. J., Long, B., & West S. (1991,

134

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
June). Prevention Research: Conceptual model o f strategies and procedures.
Paper presented at the National Prevention Conference, Washington, D.C.

Coie, J., Watt, N., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., Asarnow, J. R. Markman, H. J., Ramey,
S. L., Shure, M. B., & Long, B. (1993). The science of prevention: A conceptual
framework and some directions for a national research program. American
Psychologist, 48, 1013-1022.

Comrey, A. L. & Lee, H. B. (1992). A first course in factor analysis. (2nd Ed.) Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.

Cowlishaw, B.R. (2001). Subjects are from Mars, objects are from Venus: construction
of the self in self-help. Journal o f Popular Culture, 35, 16, p i 69 (16)

Crohan, S. E. (1992). Marital happiness and spousal consensus on beliefs about marital
conflict: A longitudinal investigation. Journal o f Social and Personal
Relationships, 9, 89-102.

DeBord, J. & Romans, J. S. C. (1996). Predicting dyadic adjustment from general and
relationship specific beliefs. Journal o f Psychology Interdisciplinary & Applied,
130, (3) 263.

Department of Health and Human Services, (2005) National Centerfo r Health Statistics;
Retrived November 10, 2005 from http:www.cdc.gov/nchs/data

Doherty, P. (1997). Assessment of irrational marital beliefs and conflict resolution styles
among premarital couples. Dissertation Absfracts International. 58-03B.

Doherty, W. J. (1983). Impact of divorce on locus of control orientation in adult women:


A longitudinal study. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 834-
840.

Dryden, W. & DiGiuseppe, R. (1990). A primer on rational-emotive therapy.


Champaign, IL: Research Press.

Eidelson, R. J. & Epstein, N. (1982) Cognition and relationship maladjustment:


Development o f a measure of dysfunctional relationship beliefs. Journal o f
Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 50, 715-720.

Ellis, A. (1994) Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. New York, New York: Carol
Publishing

Ellis, A. & Harper, R.A. (1961). A guide to a successful marriage. North Hollywood,
CA; Wilhire Books.

Emmelkamp, P. M. G., Krol, B., & Sanderman, R. (1987). The assessment of

135

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
relationship beliefs in a marital context. Personality & Individual Differences,
8, 775-780.

Epstein, N., & Eidelson, R. J. (1981). Unrealistic beliefs of clinical couples: Their
relationship to expectations, goals and satisfaction. American Journal o f Family
Therapy., 9 (4), 13-22.

Filsinger, E. E., & Wilson, M. R. (1983). Social anxiety and marital adjustment. Family
Relations Journal o f Applied Family and Child Studies, 32, 513-519.

Fincham, F., Grych. J., & Osborne, L. (1993, March). Inteiparental conflict and child
adjustment: A longitudinal analysis. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of
the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.

Fincham, F.D. (1985). Attributions in close relationships. In J. H. Elarvey and G.Weary


(Eds.) Contemporary attribution theory and research. New York: Academic
Press.

Fitzpatrick, J. & Sollie, D. L. (1999). Unrealistic gendered and relationship-specific


beliefs: Contributions to investments and commitment in dating relationships.
Journal o f Social & Personal Relationships, 16, 6, 852.

Forthofer, M. S., Markman, H. J., Cox, M. Stanley, S., & Kessler, R. C. (1996)
Associations between marital distress and work loss in a national sample. Journal
o f Marriage and Family, 58, 597-605.

Fowers B. J. & Olsen D. H. (1986). Predicting marital success with PREPARE: A


predictive validity study. Journal o f Marital and Family Therapy, 12, 403-413.

Frank, J. D. & Frank J. B. (1990). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study o f


psychotherapy (3rd ed.) Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University.

Gall, M. D., Borg, W. R. & Gall, J. P. (1996). Educational research: an introduction.


White Plains, N Y : Longman Publishers.

Gottman, J. (1994). Why marriages succeed or fail. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Grey, J. (1992) Men are from Mars, Women are from Venus. New York: Harper Collins.

Hamamci, Z. (2005). Dysfunctional relationship beliefs in marital satisfaction and


adjustment. Social Behavior and Personality, 33,4, 313-328.

Harvey, J. H. & Fine, M. A. (2004). Children o f Divorce: Stories o f loss and growth.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Holman, T.B., Larson, J. H. & Harmer, S. L. (1994). The development and predictive

136

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
validity of a new premarital assessment instrument. The preparation for marriage
questionnaire. Family Relations, 43, 46-52.

Holmes T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment scale. Journal o f
Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218.

James, S., Hunsley, J., & Hemsworth, D. (2002). Factor Structure of the Relationship
Belief Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 6, 729-744.

Janus S. S. & Janus, C. C. (1993). The Janus report on human sexuality. New York:
Wiley.

Johnson, S. M. & Greenberg, L. S. (1985). Differential effects of experiential and


problem-solving intervention in resolving marital conflict. Journal o f Consulting
and Clinical Psychology. 53 (2), 175-184.

Jones, M.E. & Stanton, A.L. (1988). Dysfunctional beliefs, belief similarity, and marital
distress: A comparison of models. Journal o f Social and Clinical Psychology, 7
(1), 1-14.

Jones, R. G. (1968). A factored measure o f E llis’irrational belief system with personality


and maladjustment correlates. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas
Technological College. Microform iv. 921.

Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrica, 39, 31-36.

Kelly, E. L. & Conley, J. J. (1987). Personality and compatibility: A prospective


analysis o f marital stability and marital satisfaction. Journal o f Personality and
Social Psychology, 52, 1, 27-40.

Kordacova, J. & Kondas, O. (1998). Irrationality in the youth— structural differences in


irrational beliefs in relation to age. Studia Psychologica, 40, 282-286.

Kurdek, L. A. (1991a). Correlates of relationship satisfaction in cohabitating gay and


lesbian couples: A prospective longitudinal test of the contextual, investment, and
problem-solving models. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 910-
922.

Kurdek, L. A. (1991b) Marital stability and changes in marital quality in newlywed


couples: A test of the contextual model. Journal o f Social and Personal
Relationships, 8, 27-18.

Kurdek, L. A. (1993). Predicting Marital dissolution: A 5-Year prospective longitudinal


study of newlywed couples. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology, 64,
221-242.

137

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Kurdek, L. A. (1999). The nature and predictors of the trajectory of change in marital
quality for husbands and wives over the first 10 years of marriage.
Developmental Psychology, 35, 5, 1283-1296.

Kurdek, L. A & Schmitt, J.P. (1986). Early development of relationship quality in


heterosexual married, heterosexual cohabiting, gay, and lesbian couples.
Developmental Psychology, 22, 305-309.

Larson, J. H. (1992). “You’re my one and only": Premarital counseling for unrealistic
beliefs about mate selection. The American Journal o f Family Therapy, 20, 242-
253.

Larson, J. H. & Holman, T. B. (1994), Premarital predictors of marital quality and


stability. Family Relations, 43, 228-237.

Lewis. R. A., & Spanier, E. B. (1979). Therorizing about the quality and stability of
marriage. In W. R. Burr. R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I.L. Reiss (Eds). Contemporary
theories about the family. (Vol 1), 268-294. New York: Free Press.

Lichtenberg, James W. (1992) Physical illness and subscription to Ellis’s Irrational


Beliefs. Journal o f Counseling and Development, 71, 2, 157-163.

Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959) Short marital adjustment and prediction tests;
Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251-255.

Manning, W. D. (1993). Marriage and cohabitation following premarital conception.


Journal o f Marriage and the Family, 55, 4, 839-850.

Markman, H. J. & Hahlweg, K. (1993). The prediction and prevention of marital


distress: An international perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 1,29-
43.

Martin, T. C., & Bumpass, L. (1989). Recent trends in marital disruption. Demography,
26, 37-51.

Marquardt, E. (2005). Between two worlds. New York, NY. Crown Publishing Group.

McDonald, C.E. (1995). Parenting irrational beliefs and marital adjustment as correlates
o f parenting stress in young families. (Doctoral Dissertation: University of
Alberta, Canada, (1995). UMI 55 (7-A) 1879

McLanahan, S., & Bumpass, L. (1988) Intergenerational consequences of family


disruption. American Journal o f Sociology, 94, 130-152.

Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modification: An integrative approach.


New York: Plenum.

138

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Moller, A. & Van Zyl, P.D. (1991) Relationship beliefs, interpersonal perception, and
marital adjustment. Journal o f Clinical Psychology, 47, 1, 28-33.

Rollins, B.C., & Feldman, H. (1970). Marital satisfaction over the family life cycle.
Journal o f Marriage and the Family, 32, 20-28.

Romans, J. S. C. & DeBord, J. (1995). Development of the Relationship Beliefs


Questionnaire. Psychological Reports, 76 (3) p. 1248.

Rusbult, C. E. (1987). Responses to dissatisfaction in close relationships: The exit-voice-


loyalty-neglect model. In D. Perelman & S. Duck (Eds ), Intimate relationship:
Development, dynamics and deterioration (p 209-237) Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Schwebel, A. I. & Fine, M. A. (1992). Cognitive-behavioral family therapy. Journal o f


Family Psychotherapy, 3, 1, 73-91.

Schwebel, A. I. & Fine, M. A. (1994). Understanding and’Helpmg the family: A


cognitive-behavioral model. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schwebel, A. I. & Sullivan, B. F. (1996). Coping with the loss of the marriage dream: A
cognitive-behavioral perspective. Journal o f Personal and Interpersonal Loss, 1,
57-69.

Sharp, E. A & Ganong, L. H. (2000). Raising awareness about marital expectations: Are
unrealistic beliefs changed by integrative teaching? Family Relations, 49, 1, 71-
76

Shapiro, J. & Kroeger, L. (1991). Is life just a romantic novel? The relationship between
attitudes about intimate relationships and the popular media. The American
Journal o f Family Therapy, 19 (3), 226-236.

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the
quality o f marriage and similar dyads. Journal o f Marriage and the Family, 38,
13-28.

Spanier, G. B., Lewis, R.A., & Cole, C.L. (1975). Marital adjustment over the family life
cycle: The issue of curvilinearity. Journal o f Marriage and the Family, 37, 263-
275.

Stahman, R. F., & Hiebert, W. J. (1987). Premarital Counseling: The professional’s


handbook (2nd ed). Lexington MA: Lexington Books.

Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J. (1997). Marriage in the 90's: A Nationwide Random
Phone Survey. Denver, CO: PREP, Inc.

139

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Stuart, R. B., (1995) In The Buros Institute of Mental Measurements, The I I th Mental
Measurement Yearbook, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NB. 297

Sullivan, B. F., (1994). Irrational, maladaptive, and illogical relationship beliefs: A scale
development study. Unpublished master’s thesis. The Ohio State University,
Columbus, OH.

Sullivan, B. F., (1995). Relationship beliefs and expectations of satisfaction in marital


relationships: Implication for family practitioners. Family Journal: Counseling
and Therapy fo r Couples and Families, 3 (4), 298-305.

Sullivan, B. F., (1997). The development, validation, and psychometric properties of the
relationship belief scale. (Doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, 1997).
Dissertation Abstracts International, 58-05B, 2755.

Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd Eds). New
York, NY: Harper Collins

Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th Eds.)
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon

Vivian D. & O’Leary, K. D. (1990). Physical aggression in marriage. In P.D. Fincham &
T.N. Bradbury (Eds.) The psychology o f marriage: Basic issues and applications.
(p 323-348). New York: Guilford Press.

Wallerstein, J. S., Lewis, J. M. & Blakeslee, S. (2000). The unexpected legacy o f


divorce. New York, NY: Hyperion.

Wambolt, F. S., & Reis, D. (1989). Defining a family heritage and a new relationship:
Two central themes in the making of a marriage. Family Process, 28, 317-335.

Weiss, R. L., & Heyman, R.E. (1990). Observation of marital interaction. In


F.D.Fincham & T. N. Bradbury (Eds), The psychology o f marriage: Basic
issues and applications. 87-117. New York:Guilford Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix A

Northern Arizona University

Letter from Institutional Review Board

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
N o r t h e r n A r iz o n a U n iv e r s it y
INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD FOR THE
PROTECTION OF HUMAN SUBJECTS IN RESEARCH

TO: Karen L. Nickl

FROM: Melanie Birck, IRB Administrator

DATE: April 28, 2003

SUBJECT: Expedited Review of the Use of Humans in Research

Your research project, Case Number 03.0384 entitled, “The relationship between irrational
beliefs and marital satisfaction” has been approved through an expedited review procedure
conducted by the Human Subjects Committee. An expedited procedure is used when the study
appears to use research procedures, which are of minimal risk to the human subjects involved.

Research designs which assure protection from psychological, sociological, and physical damage
(and meet other civil rights conditions), are normally approved through the expedited review
procedure. Some important factors which assure protection of human subjects in research are:

1. Subjects are volunteers (or are a part of an officially approved or ongoing educational
program: and they may withdraw from the research at any time.)

2. Subjects are informed of the research through a verbal or oral explanation or


clarification and sign a consent form (for minors, a parent or guardian signs the consent
form and in medically related cases, a physician must sign for consent).

3. There are assurances that subjects cannot be identified (directly or indirectly) through
responses, and in presentation of data, which may provide a link placing them in a
position of risk of criminal or civil liability; or provide a link exposing sensitive aspects of
the subject's behavior, e.g., use of drugs, alcohol, sexual behavior or illegal conduct.

4. The research study follows procedures of confidentiality and anonymity.

5. Research involving minimal stress, non-invasive techniques, moderate exercise of


healthy volunteers and routine collection of samples of blood, saliva and similar
specimens may be considered of minimal risk.

6. Other relative modifications or comments:

To assure anonymity and research protection, consent forms and research data must be
appropriately filed and protected by the researcher and the department.

If there are any irregularities resulting from the research program, please report them to the
Institutional Review Board.

cc: Ramona Mellott, Faculty Sponsor


Department Chair
File

PO Box 4130, Flagstaff, AZ 86011-4130 (928) 523-4880 fax (928) 523-1075

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix B

Letter of Informed Consent

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
N o r t h e r n A r iz o n a U n iv e r s it y
COLLEGE OF EDUCATION
EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
Dear Participant,

You are being asked to participate in a study sponsored by Northern Arizona University. Read these
instructions and discuss questions you have with the investigator. Please keep this form for future
reference.

1. Purpose of the Study: The purpose of this study is to look at attitudes people have about marriage and
to examine the merits o f tests that measure attitudes about marriage.

2. Procedure: After agreeing to participate, you will be given an envelope containing the test materials.
The first is a sheet on which you will respond to questions about yourself and family. Next, you will
complete the Dyadic Adjustment Scale which measures adjustment in your marriage. Next, are two
questionnaires, The Relationship Belief Index and The Relationship Belief Scale, both o f which measure
your attitudes regarding marriage. Filling out these forms takes between 20 and 30 minutes.

3. Confidentiality: Do NOT put your name on the materials or envelope. Your answers will never be
associated with you or your name. Your responses will be kept in the strictest confidentiality in
accordance with professional ethics. At the end o f two years, all research materials will be destroyed.

4. Benefits: The outcome of this study will help researchers, therapists and educators to design and
implement marriage therapy and education. The benefits to you might be the good feelings you get when
you further a good cause. The materials are intriguing and may provide some entertainment.

5. Risks: Participation in this study poses no physical harm to you. However, you may experience some
fluctuation in mood as you think about your marriage and marriage in general. If you do experience
distress please alert the investigator who will help you or refer you to services. Please be advised that it is
not possible to identify all potential risks in a scientific study like this.

6. Refusal/Withdrawal: You are under NO obligation to participate. You may withdraw at anytime.

There is a dated approval stamp on the bottom o f this form The stamp indicates that this project has been reviewed and
approved by the Northern Arizona University Institutional Review Board (IRB) for the Protection of Human Participants.
Contact the Human Research Protections Administrator at 928-523-4889 or 928-523-4340 if you have questions about: 1) the
conduct o f the project, 2) your rights as a research participant, or 3) a research-related injury. Any other questions about this
study should be addressed to the primary investigator, Karen Nickl, 801-296-6606 or Dr. Ramona Mellott, 928-523-6534.

WI N Sml l i U? ; M0 : ,Z!. 0RF'/jFiW


N A U N IVBOARD
E R S IT Y ’

C O N S E N T J T : ■;
VALID U r : T I L

. .p r e p a r in g e d u c a tio n p r o fe s s io n a ls to c r e a te th e sc h o o ls o f t o m o r r o w
PO Box 5774, Flagstaff, A Z 8601 1-5774 (928) 523-7103

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix C

Letter of Support and Permission

The Institute o f Marriage and Family Counseling

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
The Institute of Marriage and Family Counseling
3670 Quincy Avenue, Suite 104
Ogden, UT 84403
(801) 621-5666

Leon H. Blake, Ph.D.

April 24, 2003

Ramona N. Mellott Ph.D.


Chair & Associate Professor
Educational Psychology
Northern Arizona University
Box 5774,
Flagstaff, AZ 86011

Dear Dr. Mellott,

It is my pleasure to provide this letter o f permission for Karen Nickl. Karen is a


Professional Counseling Intern with us. She has explained to me the nature o f her
research for her dissertation study. She intends to poll audience members who are
attending workshops, seminars, and lectures that we occasionally provide to the public.
Specifically, Karen will have access to any group that she is addressing but may also
survey groups that myself or others address, with the speaker’s agreement.

I hope this provides you with what you require. Please contact me if I can be of
further assistance.

Sincerely,

Leon H. Blake, Ph.D.


Director

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix D

Email Letter from Norman Epstein, Ph.D.

Granting permission to use the Relationship Belief Inventory

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
From: ne4 <Norman_B_EPSTEIN@umail.umd.edu>
To: Nickl <woodennickl@safeaccess.com>
Date: Saturday, April 15, 2000 8:44 AM
Subject: Re: RBI - use in Dissertation

Karen,

I'm sorry for the delay in responding to you. I have been very busy. I'll
send you a copy of the RBI early this conning week.

Norman Epstein

>This is a multi-part m essage in MIME format.


>
>-----=_NextPart_000_0004_01 BFA079.184AB760
>Content-Type: text/plain;
> charset="iso-8859-1"
>Content-Transfer-Encoding: quoted-printable
>
>Dr. Epstein
>
>l am a doctoral student at Northern Arizona University pursuing a degree =
>in Counseling Psychology. I am interested in using the Relationship =
>Belief Inventory in a study for my dissertation. Please advise me on =
>how to obtain this instrument, and cost. I would especially appreciate =
>a sample protocol to share with my committee.
>
>l thank you for your attention to this request.
>
>Karen L. Nickl
>P.O. Box 50531
>Parks, AZ 86018
>
>520-635-4334
>
>woodennickl@safeaccess.corn
>
>------=_NextPart_000_0004_01BFA079.184AB760
>Content-Type: text/html;
> charset="iso-8859-1"
>Content-Transfer-Encoding: quoted-printable
>
x iD G C T Y P E HTML PUBLIC "-//W3C//DTD W3 HTML//EN">
><HTML>
4/16/00

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix E

Email Letter from Bryce F. Sullivan Ph.D.

Granting permission to use the Relationship Belief Scale

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
From: Bryce F. Sullivan <bsulliv@siue.edu>
To: Nickl <woodennickl@safeaccess.com>
Date: Friday, February 11, 2000 10:57 AM
Subject: Re: Relationship Belief Research Interest

Karen,

You are free to use it in your research. I don't have a test publisher
handling it, so you can format it with a six-point scale as I have on my
web site. I haven't published an article on the RBS, but I have a great
deal of data supporting it's validity and reliability. Much of the
research is included in my dissertation.

I can send you scoring directions if you'd like. I'd like to hear how
things turn out if you choose to use it in your dissertation.

> Nickl wrote:


>
> Dear Dr. Sullivan
>
> I am a doctoral student at Northern Arizona University. I am completing my Ed.D in
Counseling Psychology. I have a strong research interest in cognitive behavioral
interventions in marital counseling. While searching the literature, I came across your
dissertation abstract and was excited to se e a new instrument to measure dysfunctional
relationship beliefs. I was even more surprised to find it on your web site!
>
> I am interested in using your instrument in my dissertation. Please provide me with
information on how to obtain it for this purpose and the cost involved. I would also like to
know of any other recent, large sample studies that have correlated the RBS to marital
distress/dissatisfaction.
>
> I thank you for your time and attention to my request.
>
> Sincerely,
>
> Karen L. Nickl
> PO Box 50531
> Parks, AZ 86018
>
> 520-635-4334

Bryce F. Sullivan, Ph.D.


Department of Psychology
Southern Illinois University at Edwardsville
Edwardsville, IL 62026-1121
2/ 11/00

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Appendix F

Demographic Information Questionnaire

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Demographic Questionnaire

Do NOT p u t your n a m e on this form. P le a se an sw e r carefully a n d truthfully. Your an sw e rs


will b e h e ld in a confidential m a n n e r a n d u s e d only for scientific purposes.

1. W hat is your a g e ? y e a rs old

2. W hat is your g en d e r? (circle one) M ale F em ale

3. W hat level of form al ed u c atio n h a v e you attain ed ? (Circle one)

S om e H igh S chool O r GED H igh School G ra d S o m e C ollege C o lleg e D e g ree

Post G ra d u a te D e g re e

4. Indicate the ra n g e of your a n n u a l h o u sehold incom e. (Circle one)

$8,000 - $24,000 $25,000 - $49,000 $50,000 - $74,000 $75,000 o r m ore

5. A re you currently m arried ? (circle one) Y es No

6. How m an y tim es h av e you b e e n m arried ? O n c e Twice T hree o r m ore

7. How m an y y e a rs h av e you b e e n m a rrie d to your sp o u se? ________ y ea rs


(Round u p to n e a re st year)

8. Do you or your sp o u se h av e children? (Circle one) Y es No

9. W hat is th e a g e of your o r your sp o u se s oldest living child? y e a rs old

10. H ave you sought p rofessional co u n selin g services for


m arital d istre ss in th e last 12 m onths? Y es No
(Do not include p rem arital counseling.)

11. To w hich of th e following religions a r e you m ost significantly affiliated? (Circle one)

LDS C atholic P ro testan t Jewish No Religion


O ther_________________

12. Did your p a re n ts divorce? (Circle one) Y es No

13. If so, how old w e re you w hen their separation/divorce o c c u rre d ? y e a rs old

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

También podría gustarte