Está en la página 1de 13

The Rebirth of Children's Learning

Robert S. Siegler

Child Development, Vol. 71, No. 1. (Jan. - Feb., 2000), pp. 26-35.

Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28200001%2F02%2971%3A1%3C26%3ATROCL%3E2.0.CO%3B2-5

Child Development is currently published by Society for Research in Child Development.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained
prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in
the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/journals/srcd.html.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is an independent not-for-profit organization dedicated to and preserving a digital archive of scholarly journals. For
more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

http://www.jstor.org
Mon Apr 23 11:26:00 2007
Child Development, January /February 2000, Volume 71, Number 1, Pages 26-35

The Rebirth of Children's Learning

Robert S. Siegler

Learning is a central part of children's lives, but the study of learning is a rather peripheral part of the field of
cognitive development. Fortunately, this situation is starting to change; recent theoretical and methodological
advances have sparked renewed interest in children's learning. This renewed interest has already yielded a set
of consistent and interesting findings regarding how children learn, as well as intriguing proposals regarding
the mechanisms that underlie the learning. Increasing our focus on children's learning promises to yield prac-
tical benefits as well as a more exciting field of cognitive development.

INTRODUCTION Prominent successors to Piaget's


- theory,
- in particu-
-
lar neonativist and theory-theory approaches, also
At one time, children's learning was the central topic
have focused on children's thinking, largely to the ex-
in developmental psychology. This has not been the
clusion of their learning. The research that they have
case for many years, however. With the cognitive rev-
inspired has expanded our understanding of devel-
olution in adult experimental psychology and the rise
opment by revealing substantial, domain-specific, cog-
of Piaget's theory within developmental psychology,
nitive capabilities that children possess from early in
the emphasis shifted from learning to thinking.
life and by demonstrating the key roles of causal con-
This shift laid the foundation for a rich and vibrant
nections, often mediated by unobservable constructs,
field of cognitive development. The gains, however,
in these early understandings. Like Piaget's theory,
came at a cost. We now know quite a bit about chil-
however, they have told us little about how children
dren's thinking at different ages, but we know little
come to have these understandings.
about how they get from here to there. In a sense, we
It is no accident that recent theories have focused
threw out the baby of learning with the bath water of
more on the ways children typically think at particu-
associationism.
lar ages than on the processes by which they learn to
The movement away from studying children's
think in more advanced ways. Intellectually, it makes
learning reflected more than a shift in interest; it also
sense to map out landmarks within the developmen-
reflected an assumption that development and learn-
tal progression before trying to specify the mecha-
ing are fundamentally different processes. Piaget
nisms by which children move from here to there. Lo-
frequently distinguished between development, by
gistic factors militate in the same direction; simply
which he meant the active construction of knowl-
put, it is easier to determine what children know at
edge, and learning, by which he meant the passive
different ages than to determine how they acquire the
formation of associations. Active developmental pro-
knowledge. Research approaches also create their own
cesses were of interest; passive learning processes
momentum; the great recent progress in understand-
were not. This distinction was valuable in focusing at-
ing certain topics that have been in vogue, such as un-
tention on children's efforts to make sense of the
derstanding of other people's minds and of living and
world and in exposing hidden assumptions that had
nonliving things, has raised numerous interesting
shaped previous research on children's learning. How-
questions regarding alternative interpretations and
ever, Piaget's stance had the unfortunate side effect of
potential extensions of previous findings.
producing skepticism about the importance of any
Arrayed against these varied factors that exert pres-
kind of learning for development. This led to a drastic
sure toward continuing to focus on how children think,
decline in studies of children's learning. As Stevenson
rather than on how they learn, is one central fact:
(1983) commented:
learning is a central part of children's lives. Learning
By the mid-1970s, articles on children's learning is even more central in the lives of children
dwindled to a fraction of the number that had been than in the lives of adults. Adults frequently have
published in the previous decade, and by 1980, it considerable expertise with the tasks they undertake.
was necessary to search with diligence to uncover As they gain experience, they continue to learn, but
any articles at all. . . . The discussion of children's
learning had been displaced by a newfound inter- o 2000 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
est in cognitive development (p. 213). All rights reserved. 0009-3920/ 2000/ 7101-0004
Robert S. Siegler 27

much of the learning involves relatively small refine- The empirical studies that have revealed the com-
ments of existing competencies rather than acquisition monalities in children's learning rest on a foundation
of new capabilities. In addition, how well they per- of recent theoretical and methodological advances. The
form in their jobs and in their other everyday tasks next two sections of this article examine these theoreti-
such as driving is important to their own and other cal and methodological advances; the following sec-
people's well-being. Thus, performance is very im- tion highlights four findings that have emerged con-
portant in adults' lives, relative to learning. sistently from the empirical research; and the final
In contrast, childhood is a period of life in which section focuses on two key issues for future consider-
learning plays a particularly large role relative to per- ation: the relation between learning and development
formance. Infants, toddlers, and preschoolers fre- and educational implications of current research on
quently need to acquire new capabilities; their ability children's learning.
to learn is very important. On the other hand, how well
they perform any given task at a given time ordinarily
EMERGING THEORIES OF
is unimportant. The quality of the pictures they draw,
CHILDREN'S LEARNING
their skill at playing games, and their knowledge of
songs and television characters matters little. What is In the past few years, theoretical proposals regarding
important is that they, and school-age children, learn children's learning have increased considerably in
to act in ways that will allow them to perform effectively both number and precision. A wide range of theoret-
in future settings. Five hundred years ago, 14-year-olds ical approaches have contributed to this trend, as is
were adults, because they had learned enough that apparent in the chapters of the most recent Handbook of
they could function as adults; today, they are children, CIzild Psychology (Damon, 1998). In a chapter focused
because they need to learn so much more before they on information processing approaches, Klahr and
can function in adult capacities. Thus, although learn- MacWhinney (1998) noted that beneath the superficial
ing is important for adults, it plays an even larger role differences between connectionist and symbolic infor-
in the lives of children. Any theory of development mation processing models of children's learning, there
that has little to say about how children learn is a se- are important similarities. Both types of models indi-
riously limited theory of development. cate that learning involves considerable parallel pro-
The importance of children's learning for a coherent cessing; that it influences the activations of many local
understanding of development has led a small but in- units rather than being limited to high-level changes
creasing number of investigators to take on the chal- such as those envisioned in stage approaches; and that
lenges associated with studying it directly. These in- it produces qualitative as well as quantitative change.
vestigators come to the task from a variety of theoretical Rogoff (1998) emphasized that within sociocul-
backgrounds: neo-Piagetian (e.g.,Fischer & Bidell, 1998; tural approaches to development, learning involves
Karmiloff-Smith, 1992), cultural contextualist (Ellis & not just increasing knowledge of content but also in-
Gauvain, 1992; Granott, 1993), dynamic systems corporation of values and cultural assumptions that
(Thelen & Ulrich, 1991; van Geert, 1998),and informa- underlie views about how material should be
tion processing (Johnson & Morton, 1991; Munakata, taught and how the task of learning should be ap-
1998; Siegler, 1996). They do not study learning of proached. Case's (1998) examination of neo-Piagetian
paired associates or nonsense syllables. Instead, they theories provides a third example. He proposed that
investigate how children learn meaningful concepts children learn new information by organizing it to fit
and skills such as object permanence, reaching, face central conceptual structures for thinking about num-
recognition, scientific and mathematical problem ber, space, and other domains. To cite a fourth exam-
solving, arithmetic, and so on. Thus, the new field of ple, Gelman and Williams (1998) examined constraints
children's learning, unlike the old one, emphasizes on learning. They argued that all theories of cognitive
acquisition of concepts and skills that are impor- development posit that learning is constrained, that
tant in children's lives. almost all of the theories recognize that many of the
Despite differences in the investigators' theoretical posited constraints help rather than hinder learning,
orientations, modem investigations of children's learn- and that the theories vary more in their views regard-
ing have yielded highly similar results. Such common- ing the specificity and origins of the constraints than
alities are especially encouraging, because they sug- in whether learning is constrained. Finally, Spelke and
gest that the regularities in children's learning are Newport (1998)proposed that innate knowledge pro-
sufficiently strong that they shine through differences vides the building blocks from which more refined and
in investigators' preconceptions and specifics of tasks, culturally contingent learning is later created. Thus, al-
content domains, and populations. though neither neo-Piagetian nor information pro-
28 Child Development

cessing nor sociocultural nor neonativist theories (Siegler & McGilly, 1989;Siegler & Shrager, 1984).The
have focused primarily on how children learn, all are variability is even present within a single trial. Chil-
devoting increasing attention to it. dren sometimes express one strategy in speech and a
One theory that does focus primarily on children's different one in gesture on the same trial (Goldin-
learning is the overlapping waves theory (Siegler, Meadow, Alibali, & Church, 1993).Other times, verbal-
1996). This theory is based on three assumptions: izations alone reveal multiple strategies; for example,
(1) children typically use a variety of strategies and children frequently use category naming and rehearsal
ways of thinking, rather than just a single one, to solve on a single free recall trial (Coyle & Bjorklund, 1997).
a given problem; (2) the diverse strategies and ways This last phenomenon-cognitive variability within
of thinking coexist over prolonged periods of time, a single trial-raises interesting challenges for formal
not just during brief transition periods; (3) experience models of strategy choice. To date, such models have
brings changes in relative reliance on existing strate- focused on situations in which a single strategy is
gies and ways of thinking, as well as introduction of chosen on a given trial (e.g., Siegler & Shipley, 1995).
more advanced approaches. However, the models could be extended in any of three
A schematic illustration of these assumptions is pro- ways to choosing among multiple strategies on a sin-
vided in Figure 1. Examination of any vertical slice of gle trial. One possibility is that two or more strategies
the figure indicates that multiple approaches are used could be organized into a linked unit, and the linked
at one time. Comparing several vertical slices indicates unit could be chosen; thus, children might on one trial
that relative frequencies of strategies shift continuously choose "name a category and rehearse its members,"
over time, with new strategies sometimes being added though on another trial they might choose one of the
and older strategies sometimes ceasing to be used. component strategies (e.g., "name the category") alone.
Following the curve for a given strategy indicates that A second possibility is that separate strategy choices
a single strategy will often be used for a protracted could be made at different times within a trial; children
period, even after later-developing, more advanced might first choose category naming as a strategy and
approaches are also known. later in the trial choose rehearsing the members of the
The cognitive diversity postulated by overlapping category. A third possibility, especially applicable to
waves theory appears to be present at every level of cases where different strategies are expressed in differ-
analysis. It is present within individuals as well as ent modalities, is that strategy choices could be made
across them; in studies of arithmetic, serial recall, spell- in parallel in the two modalities; thus, children might
ing, time telling, and other tasks, most children used choose one strategy in gesture and simultaneously
at least three strategies (Siegler, 1996).The variability choose a different one in speech. These possibilities are
also is evident within an individual solving the same not exclusive; all are plausible, and all may be used.
problem on two occasions close in time; presented the Overlapping waves theory also specifies four di-
same simple addition problem or the same time on an mensions along which learning occurs: acquisition of
analog clock, one third of children used different strat- novel ways of thinking, more frequent use of the more
egies on two presentations within a one-week period effective ways of thinking from among the existing
possibilities, increasingly adaptive choices among
alternative ways of thinking, and increasingly effi-
cient execution of the alternative approaches.
The most obvious dimension of learning is acquisi-
tion of new, more advanced ways of thinking. Such
acquisition can occur through drawing analogies to
better understood problems, through direct verbal
instruction, through forming mental models of the
situation and reasoning about them, or through ob-
servations during the course of problem solving
(Anderson, 1991; Sternberg, 1985).Acquisition of new
strategies involves a mix of associative and metacog-
nitive processes, and also a mix of conscious and un-
conscious processes. In at least some cases, new strat-
egies are constructed on an unconscious level before
Age people are aware of doing anything different than
they had done previously; behavioral indices show
Figure 1 Schematic depiction of the overlapping waves model. that new approaches are being used, although verbal
Robert S. Siegler 29

reports of use of the new strategy lag slightly behind with variability existing within individual children as
(Siegler & Stern, 1998). Thus, discovery is not exclu- well as between children. In each area, children also
sively a metacognitive process, nor is it exclusively an have been found to rely increasingly on the relatively
associative process. Both types of processes are crucial. advanced approaches as they learn more about the
Although important, acquiring new ways of think- domain. These same features are characteristic of
ing is not the only way learning occurs. A second di- adults' thinking and learning, as has been demon-
mension of learning involves increasing reliance on strated in such domains as multidigit mental arith-
the more advanced alternatives within the set of ap- metic, sentence-picture verification, and spatial rea-
proaches that children already know. This is a more soning (LeFevre, Sadesky, & Bisanz 1996; Marquer &
common vehicle of cognitive growth than is com- Pereira, 1990; Newton & Roberts, in press).
monly recognized. For example, Lemaire and Siegler These findings regarding children's and adults'
(1995) found that from the end of the first week of in- learning have formed the basis for computer simula-
struction to the end of the year, French children who tion models that use a common set of principles to ac-
were learning single-digit multiplication used the count for cognitive growth from early childhood
same set of strategies. The frequency of the most ad- through adulthood (Shrager & Siegler, 1998; Siegler &
vanced strategy (retrieval of the answer from mem- Shipley, 1995).These simulation models suggest that
ory) increased considerably during this period, and the overlapping waves pattern arises through the
the frequency of the least advanced strategy (adding workings of several learning processes. Problem-
one of the multiplicands the number of times indicated solving experience leads to an increasingly extensive
by the other) decreased considerably, but most children database becoming associated with both strategies
used the same set of strategies throughout the period. and problems. This database includes information on
Learning also can occur through increasingly pre- the speed and accuracy of each strategy on problems in
cise fitting of strategy choices to the demands of prob- general, problems with particular features, and specific
lems and situations. Even if both the set of strategies problems. Experience using each strategy also leads
and the frequency of use of each strategy remain the to its execution becoming increasingly automatized.
same, each strategy can be chosen increasingly often The increasingly extensive database on characteristics
in those cases in which it is the best available alterna- of strategies and problems makes possible increasingly
tive. Such changes also were present in the Lemaire refined choices among strategies and increasing reli-
and Siegler (1995) study. Over the course of the year, ance on the most advanced strategies. The increasing
children fit their strategy choices increasingly precisely automatization of the strategies leads to increasingly
to the demands of problems and to the limits of their fast, accurate, and effortless execution of strategies.
own knowledge. In particular, they used retrieval in- An interesting aspect of the most recent of these
creasingly consistently on the easiest problems, prob- computer simulations, SCADS (Strategy Choice and
lems on which they usually could retrieve the correct Discovery Simulation; Shrager & Siegler, 1998) is that
-
answer, and they increasingly limited use of repeated within it, discovery of new strategies arises through
addition to the most difficult problems, problems on the interplay of associative and metacognitive learn-
which retrieval was less accurate. ing processes. Automatization of execution of strate-
Another dimension along which learning occurs is gies leads to the freeing of cognitive resources that
improved execution of existing approaches. Even with- previously had been needed to monitor execution of
out changes along the other three dimensions, chil- the strategies. Some of these freed cognitive resources
dren's performance can improve greatly as they become are used to search for redundant processing within
increasingly skillful in executing each approach. In the existing strategies. If such redundancies are found,
Lemaire and Siegler (1995)study, for example, on those strategy discovery heuristics are used to generate po-
problems on which children retrieved answers to a tential strategies from the components of previous
given multiplication problem at all three times of mea- ones. These potential strategies are then evaluated
surement, percentage of errors decreased from 23%)to against conceptual constraints on legitimate strate-
2%, and mean solution time decreased from 4 s to 2 s. gies in the domain. If the potential strategy is consis-
Data consistent with the overlapping waves model tent with the conceptual constraints, it is tried. With
have been obtained across such varied tasks as tod- each use of the new strategy, speed and accuracy
dlers' locomotor activity, preschoolers' arithmetic, and characteristics become associated with it. This emerg-
elementary and high school children's scientific ex- ing database and the databases of the prior strategies
perimentation (see Siegler, 1996, for a recent review of together determine when the new approach is used.
these studies). In all of these areas, children have been Thus, newly discovered strategies that are more effec-
found to use multiple strategies at any given age, tive than known alternatives are used increasingly,
30 Child Development

and new strategies that are inferior to known alterna- methods. Standard cross-sectional and longitudinal
tives come to be used less or not at all. methods, which sample the thinking of children at dif-
The fact that learning within the Shrager and Sieg- ferent ages, fit well with theories that emphasize such
ler (1998) model involves an interaction between questions as "When do children understand " and
metacognitive and associative processes is unlikely to "What is the developmental sequence of knowledge
be unique to the specific model or content domain. states by which children come to understand . "
Rather, children's learning in most domains seems In contrast, if a central theoretical question is
likely to reflect the interaction of associative and higher "Through what processes do children learn , "stan-
level processes. The reason why developmental psy- dard cross-sectional and longitudinal methods are less
chologists could at one time focus on associative pro- useful. The problem is that within these methods, ob-
cesses and at a later time on higher level processes is servations of emerging competence are spaced too far
that both are important parts of cognition and learn- apart in time to yield detailed information about the
ing. Focusing on one to the exclusion of the other learning process. They lack the temporal resolution
yields a one-sided picture of cognitive growth. needed to indicate how change occurs.
Other models of children's learning, such as those This is where microgenetic methods are particu-
constructed within dynamic systems theories (Smith, larly useful-for answering questions about learning
Thelen, Titzer, & McLin, 1999;van Geert, 1998),differ in processes. As noted by Siegler and Crowley (1991),
their particulars but they share with overlapping waves such methods have three main properties:
theory a number of assumptions about how learning
1. Observations span the period of rapidly chang-
occurs. Within both approaches, children learn by
ing competence.
doing; learning occurs through performance. Another
2. Within this period, the density of observations
shared assumption is that variability is a central
is high relative to the rate of change.
characteristic of the cognitive system, rather than
3. Observations are analyzed intensively, with the
reflecting measurement error. A third shared as-
goal of inferring the representations and pro-
sumption is that learning comes about through
cesses that gave rise to them.
many simultaneously changing aspects of the sys-
tem rather than through any one central change that The second property is especially important. Densely
moves the system as a whole from one state to an- sampling changing competence during the period of
other. A fourth shared assumption is that a wide vari- rapid change provides the level of temporal resolution
ety of constraints-anatomical, physiological, envi- needed to understand the learning process. If chil-
ronmental, and cognitive-guide the form of learning. dren's learning usually proceeded in the most straight-
Together, these theories converge on a new agenda forward way possible, such dense sampling of ongoing
for studying children's learning. Rather than trying to changes would be unnecessary. We could examine
identify tile age at which children develop a given ca- their thinking before and after changes occurred, iden-
pability, we would trace over time the set of approaches tify the shortest path between the two states, and infer
that they use. In other words, we would examine that children moved in a beeline from the less ad-
changing distributions of existing approaches as well vanced one to the more advanced one. Such beelines
as emergence of new ones. Another priority would be are the exception rather than the rule, however. Cog-
to examine changes that occur with age and experience nitive changes involve regressions as well as progres-
in children's choices among alternative approaches, sions, odd transitional states that are present only
that is, in ability to flexibly adjust what they do to the briefly but that are crucial for the change to occur,
demands of the problem and situation. A third prior- generalization along some dimensions from the be-
ity would be to examine the circumstances surround- ginning of learning but lack of generalization along
ing new forms of behavior-what leads up to discov- other dimensions for years thereafter, and many other
eries and how they are generalized once they emerge. surprising features. Simply put, the only way to find
Fortunately, all of these issues can be addressed through out how children learn is to follow them closely while
use of a particular method for studying children's they are learning.
learning, the microgenetic approach.
FOUR LESSONS FROM RECENT STUDIES
MICROGENETIC METHODS FOR STUDYING OF CHILDREN'S LEARNING
CHILDREN'S LEARNING
In the past decade, microgenetic methods have been
The central questions within prevailing theories in- used to study an increasing range of populations and
fluence, and are influenced by, prevailing research content domains: infants' learning of reaching and
Robert S. Siegler 31

locomotor skills (Adolph, 1997;Thelen & Ulrich, 1991), minimal external demands: taking a shower, driving
preschoolers' learning of attentional strategies and to work, taking a walk, and so on.
number conservation (Miller & Aloise-Young, 1996; Early variability is related to later learning. A third
Siegler, 1995), elementary schoolers' learning of mem- common finding from microgenetic studies is that
ory strategies, mathematical principles, analogical rea- the initial variability of thinking is positively related
soning, and pictorial representation (Alibali, 1999; to the subsequent rate of learning. In many studies,
Bjorklund, Coyle, & Gaultney, 1992; Chen & Klahr, the greater the initial variability, the more likely that
1999; Goldin-Meadow, Alibali, & Church, 1993), and children will generate useful problem solving strate-
adolescents' and adults' learning of scientific experi- gies and abandon ineffective older ones (Alibali &
mentation skills (Kuhn, Garcia-Mila, Zohar, & Ander- Goldin-Meadow, 1993; Graham & Perry, 1993; Perry
sen, 1995;Schauble, 1996).Despite varying theoretical & Lewis, 1999; Siegler, 1995). Several specific forms
predispositions of the investigators, diverse content of initial cognitive variability have been found to be
domains of the tasks, and widely separated ages of positively related to subsequent learning: number of
the children studied, the descriptions of learning that strategies used over a set of problems, frequency of
have emerged from microgenetic studies are strikingly shifting from one strategy to another within a single
similar. Consider four of the most consistent findings. trial, frequency of self-corrections and deletions in
Change is gradual. In the large majority of studies of verbal descriptions of strategies, and frequency of
children's learning, researchers have found change to expressing one strategy in speech and another in ges-
be gradual. Older, less powerful ways of thinking ture on a single trial.
about a task often continue to be employed for a long Not all types of variability are positively related
time after newer, more advanced ways of thinking to learning. For example, Coyle and Bjorklund
about it are also available (Kuhn, 1995; Schauble, (1997) found that changes in strategy use from one
1990, 1996; Siegler, 1994). Change is especially likely trial to the next were negatively related to percent
to be gradual in cases where the new approach is not correct recall. This may have reflected children
hugely advantageous relative to existing approaches. adopting a win-stay-lose-shift approach, in which
This is often the case, because early approaches tend they tended to shift strategies from one trial to the
to be reasonably effective. Counting from one gener- next when recall on the earlier trial was incorrect but
ally yields correct solutions to arithmetic problems tended to maintain the same strategy if it yielded
(Siegler & Jenkins, 1989), unsystematic scientific ex- correct recall. Consistent with this possibility, McGilly
perimentation strategies often allow identification of and Siegler (1989) found that strategy shifts on a
causal relations (Schauble, 1996), inefficient map serial recall task occurred more often following in-
drawing strategies usually get the ambulance to the correct recall than following correct recall. More
hospital (Karmiloff-Smith, 1979), and so on. Even generally, because cognition is variable in so many
when a new approach eventually offers large advan- different ways, it will be important to examine the
tages, it may not do so at first, because children can- relation between initial variability and subsequent
not execute it effectively (Bjorklund, Miller, Coyle, learning using a variety of measures of both vari-
& Slawinski, 1997; Miller & Seier, 1994).When a new ability and learning.
way of thinking is much more effective than any pre- Discoveries are constrained by conceptual understand-
vious approach, it sometimes becomes dominant ing. A fourth consistent finding is that discovery of
quite quickly (Alibali, 1999; van Geert, 1998), but new strategies is guided by conceptual understanding
more often change is gradual. of the domain (Coyle & Bjorklund, 1997; Gelman &
Discoz~eriesfollozcsuccess as zclell asfailure. A second Gallistel, 1978; Granott, 1993; Schauble, 1990, 1996).
consistent characteristic of children's learning is that The novel strategies that children attempt generally
children discover new strategies when they have been make sense; they are not generated via blind trial and
succeeding on a task as well as when they have been error. Newly generated strategies do not always yield
failing (Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Miller & Aloise- correct solutions to the problems that elicited them,
Young, 1996; Siegler & Jenkins, 1989). Necessity but they usually are reasonable efforts in that direc-
sometimes is the mother of invention, but at other tion. It is important to note that there certainly are
times, invention occurs without external pressure. times when children generate conceptually flawed
Children frequently generate novel strategies after strategies. These can arise either through children
having solved several problems correctly and on having an incomplete understanding of the goals
problems that they have previously solved correctly. that legitimate strategies in the domain must meet or
This finding is in accord with the everyday observa- through the situation requiring children to generate
tion that many discoveries arise in situations with an answer even though they do not know any plau-
32 Child Development

sible strategy for doing so. Despite these exceptions, approach. As in the less advanced sum strategy, chil-
it is striking how often newly discovered strategies dren count from l ; as in the more advanced min strat-
conform to the principles underlying legitimate egy, children count each number only once, rather
strategies in the domain. than twice as in the sum strategy. However, it was not
The consistent phenomena that have arisen from until the microgenetic study of addition that the
microgenetic studies have given rise to a set of in- shortcut sum strategy was identified as a transitional
triguing proposals regarding the processes that pro- approach. The example illustrates how microgenetic
duced the changes. To account for the persistent use data constrain ideas about transition mechanisms,
of nonoptimal strategies despite more effective strat- both in the negative sense of ruling out otherwise
egies being known, the construct of utilization defi- plausible accounts and in the positive sense of doc-
ciency has been proposed (Bjorklund, Miller, Coyle, & umenting the path of change.
Slawinski, 1997; Miller & Seier, 1994).To account for
discoveries being made in the absence of external
LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT
pressure, the SCADS computer simulation progres-
sively frees attentional resources as it gains experi- As noted earlier, the movement away from studying
ence executing existing strategies, thus activating children's learning was fueled in large part by the
strategy discovery heuristics (Shrager & Siegler, view that learning and development were fundamen-
1998).To account for positive relations between initial tally different processes. Recent studies of children's
variability and subsequent learning, investigators learning provide reason to rethink this conclusion. As
have focused on the ways variable behavior reveals Kuhn (1995) commented:
the possibilities inherent in the task environment
In the 1960s and 1970s, development was con-
(Neuringer, 1993; Stokes, 1995). To account for how
trasted to a simplistic, nonrepresentational concep-
children discover legitimate addition strategies with-
tion of learning that has little relevance today. Mod-
out ever trying illegal ones, the idea of goal sketches
em research has made it clear that learning processes
has been proposed (Siegler &Jenkins,1989),tested and
share all of the complexity, organization, structure,
supported through empirical experiments (Siegler &
and internal dynamics once attributed exclusively to
Crowley, 1994),and formally specified as a part of the
development. If the distinction has become blurred,
SCADS model (Shrager & Siegler, 1998).
it is not because development has been reduced to
The connection between the microgenetic studies
"nothing but" learning, but rather because we now
and the detailed ideas about mechanisms is no coinci- recognize learning to be more like development in
dence. Microgenetic studies yield sufficiently detailed
many fundamental respects (p. 138).
information both to suggest ideas about how the data
were generated and to rule out many otherwise plau- Findings from cognitive developmental neuro-
sible alternative accounts. To cite one such case, prior science provide additional support for the view that
to the first microgenetic study of single-digit addi- learning and development are both similar and insep-
tion, the prevailing model (Groen & Resnick, 1977) arable. Regardless of whether the change is species-
was that children first solved such problems by typical, such as development of stereopsis in re-
counting from one (the sum strategy), then by count- sponse to binocular exposure to patterned light, or
ing from the first addend, and then by counting from idiosyncratic, as when a rat learns to turn left in a
the larger addend (the rnin strategy).That is, children maze to obtain food, synaptic changes involve a cycle
were hypothesized first to solve problems such as 3 + of proliferation and pruning (Greenough, Black, &
5bycounting"1,2,3-1,2,3,4,5-1,2,3,4,5,6,7, Wallace, 1987). First, there is a burst of formation of
8"), then by counting "4,5,6,7,8," and then by count- new synaptic connections; then, experience prunes
ing "6,7,8." Although this model seemed plausible, a away those synapses not involved in subsequent pro-
microgenetic study of development of single-digit cessing. The terms "learning" and "development" are
addition disconfirmed it; children discovered the min used differently, with development referring to changes
strategy without ever having counted from the first ad- that are more universal within the species, that occur
dend. Conversely, the microgenetic study indicated over longer time periods, and that occur in response to
that shortly before children discovered the min strat- a broader variety of experiences. At the level of pro-
egy, they began to use a different transitional ap- cess, however, the two have a great deal in common.
proach that had not been hypothesized. This was the Increasing our focus on children's learning will
shortcut sum strategy; children using it would solve yield a more comprehensive understanding of devel-
3 + 5 by counting "1,2,3 - 4,5,6,7,8." In retrospect, opment; it also may yield valuable educational appli-
the shortcut sum strategy made sense as a transitional cations. It is no secret that many children do not learn
Robert S. Siegler 33

well in school. Rigorous developmental analyses of REFERENCES


how children learn-and fail to learn-reading, writ-
Adolph, K. E. (1997).Learning in the development of infant
ing, and mathematics may produce better under- locomotion. Monograpizs oftile Societyfou Research iit Child
standing of the learning difficulties and may contrib- Developmeizt, 62(3, Serial No. 251).
ute to better programs for remedying them. This is Alibali, M. W. (1999). How children change their minds:
already starting to happen. Geary (1994) identified a Strategy change can be gradual or abrupt. Developmental
number of contributors to mathematics disability: Psycilology, 35, 127-145.
limited early exposure to numbers, poor working Alibali, M. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993).Gesture-speech
memory capacity for numerical information, and lim- mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands
ited conceptual understanding of arithmetic opera- reveal about a child's state of mind. Cognitive Psychology,
tions and counting. Similarly, Griffin (in press) formu- 25,468-523.
lated a neo-Piagetian analysis of how children learn Anderson, J. R. (1991).Is human cognition adaptive? Tile Be-
hazlioual aitd Buaiit Sciences, 14, 471-484.
basic numerical concepts and applied it to the task of
Bjorklund, D. F., Coyle, T. R., & Gaultney, J. F. (1992).Devel-
helping low-income first graders master these basic
opmental differences in the acquisition and maintenance
concepts, with impressive results. of an organizational strategy: Evidence for the utiliza-
Microgenetic analyses of learning may prove es- tion deficiency hypothesis. ]ouunal of Expeuimeittal Child
pecially helpful for indicating how instructional Psyc!zology, 54, 434-438.
procedures exercise their effects, and thus for de- Bjorklund, D. F., Miller, P. H., Coyle, T. R., & Slawinski, J. L.
signing more effective future instructional proce- (1997). Instructing children to use memory strategies:
dures. For example, recent studies indicate that ask- Evidence of utilization deficiencies in memory training
ing children to explain both why correct answers are studies. Developmeit tal Revien: 17, 411-441.
correct and why incorrect answers are incorrect pro- Case, R. (1998). The development of conceptual structures.
duces greater learning than only asking them to ex- In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), W. Damon (Series
plain why correct answers are correct (Siegler, in Ed.), Handbook ofchild psycizology: Vol. 2. Cogizitioit, per-
ception, aitd language (5th ed., pp. 745-800). New York:
press). Microgenetic analyses of the learning process
Wiley.
indicated that part of the reason for this advantage Chen, Z., & Klahr, D. (1999). All other things being equal:
was that asking children to explain both correct and Children's acquisition of the Control of Variables Strat-
incorrect answers led to their adopting new strate- egy. Cizild Dezlelopment, 70, 1098-1120.
gies that were more widely applicable, ones that Coyle, T. R., & Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). Age differences in,
would generate correct answers not only on the orig- and consequences of, multiple- and variable-strategy
inal problem set but also on types of problems that use on a multitrial sort-recall task. Developmeittal Psycizol-
were not initially presented. The practical implica- O ~ Y33,372-380.
,
tion is that when students have a tendency to re- Damon, W. (Series Ed.). (1998). Handbook of child psycizology
spond to instruction by adopting overly narrow (5th ed., Vols. 1-4). New York: Wiley.
strategies, it may be particularly useful to ask them Ellis, S., & Gauvain, M. (1992). Social and cultural influ-
ences on children's collaborative interactions. In L. T.
to explain both why correct answers are correct and
Winegar & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Childreit's developmeizt
why incorrect answers are incorrect. More generally,
zoithin social context (Vol. 2, pp. 155-180). Hillsdale, NJ:
as these examples illustrate, the rebirth of children's Erlbaum.
learning promises not only to create a more exciting Fischer, K. W., & Biddel, T. R. (1998).Dynamic development
field of cognitive development but also to help chil- of psychological structures in action and thought. In
dren learn. R. M. Lerner (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Haizdbook of
child psyc!zology: Vol. 1. Tizeouetical models ofhumaiz devel-
opment (5th ed., pp. 467-562). New York: Wiley.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Geary, D. C. (1994). Cizildren's mathematical dezlelopment: Re-
search and practical implications. Washington, DC: Ameri-
Much of the research reported in this article was made can Psychological Association.
possible by funding from the Spencer Foundation Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). Tile clzild's utzderstand-
and National Institutes of Health Grant 19011. iizg of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Gelman, R., & Williams, E. (1998). Enabling constraints for
ADDRESS AND AFFILIATION cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity
and epigenesis. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), W.
Corresponding author: Robert S. Siegler, Psychology Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psydzology: Vol. 2.
Department, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed., pp. 575-630).
PA 15213; e-mail: rs7k@andrew.cmu.edu. New York: Wiley.
34 Child Development

Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M. W., & Church, R. B. (1993). Munakata, Y. (1998). Infant perseveration and implications
Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to for object permanence theories: A PDP model of the AB
read the mind. Psychological Rezlieu~,100, 279-297. task. Dez~elopmetztalScience, 1, 161-184.
Graham, T., & Perry, M. (1993). Indexing transitional Neuringer, A. (1993). Reinforced variation and selection.
knowledge. Dez~elopmet~tal Psychology, 29, 779-788. Animal Learning and Behazlior, 21, 83-91.
Granott, N. (1993). Patterns of interaction in the co- Newton, E., & Roberts, M. (in press). An experimental
construction of knowledge: Separate minds, joint ef- study of strategy selection and strategy development.
fort, and weird creatures. In R. Wozniak & K. W. Fi- Memory nnd Cognition.
scher (Eds.), Dezlelopment in context: Acting and thinking Perry, M., & Lewis, J. L. (1999).Verbal imprecision as an index
i n specific enzlironments (pp. 183-207). Hillsdale, NJ: of knowledge in transition. Dezlelopmei~fnlPsychology, 25,
Erlbaum. 749-759.
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., &Wallace, C. S. (1987). Expe- Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In
rience and brain development. Child Dez~elopmetif,58, D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), W. Damon (Series Ed.),
539-559. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception,
Griffin, S. A. (in press). Evaluation of a program designed to and lnnguage (5th ed., pp. 679-744). New York: Wiley.
teach number sense to children at risk for school failure. Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of
Joilrnal of Research in Mnflien~aficsEducntioti. prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence.
Groen, G. J., & Resnick, L. B. (1977). Can preschool children Journal ofExperinlenfn1 Child Psychology, 49, 31-57.
invent addition algorithms? Journnl of Educational Psy- Schauble, L. (1996).The development of scientific reasoning
chology, 69, 645-652. in knowledge-rich contexts. Dezlelopmental Psychology,
Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology nnd cognitizle de- 32,102-119.
velopment: The case offace recognition. Oxford: Black~vell. Shrager, J., & Siegler, R. S. (1998). SCADS: A model of chil-
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). Micro- and macro-developmen- dren's strategy choices and strategy discoveries. Psycho-
tal changes in language acquisition and other represen- logicnl Science, 9, 405-410.
tational systems. Cognitive Science, 3, 91-118. Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to under-
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A dezlelop- standing cognitive development. Current Directions in
mental perspectizle oil cogilifizle science. Cambridge, MA: Psychological Science, 3, 1-5.
MIT. Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A micro-
Klahr, D., & MacWhinney, B. (1998).Information processing. genetic study of number conservation. Cognitive Psychol-
In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), ogy, 28,225-273.
Handbook of child psychology: 1701. 2. Cognition, perception, Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of chntige in
nizd lailg~iage(5th ed., pp. 631-678). New York: Wiley. children's thiilkitig. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kuhn, D. (1995). Microgenetic study of change: What has it Siegler, R. S. (in press). Microgenetic studies of self-explana-
told us? Psychologicnl Science, 6, 133-139. tions. In N. Granott & J. Parziale (Eds.), Microdezdop-
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., &Andersen, C. (1995). rnent: Trnilsition processes iti dezlelopmeilt and learning.
Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic
Societyfor Resenrch in Child Dez~elopment,60(4, Serial No. method: A direct means for studying cognitive develop-
245). ment. American Psychologist, 46, 606-620.
LeFevre, J. A., Sadesky, G. S., & Bisanz, J. (1996).Selection of Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1994). Constraints on learning
procedures in mental addition: Reassessing the prob- in non-privileged domains. Cognifizle Psychology, 27,
lem-size effect in adults. Journnl ofExperimenfn1 Psychol- 194-227.
ogy: Learning, Memory, nnd Cognition, 22, 216-230. Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. A. (1989). Hozil childreti discozler
Lemaire, P., & Siegler, R. S. (1995). Four aspects of strategic neul strnfegies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
change: Contributions to children's learning of multiplica- Siegler, R. S., & McGilly, K. (1989). Strategy choices in chil-
tion. Journnl ofExperimenta1 Psychology: General, 124,83-97. dren's time-telling. In l. Levin and D. Zakay (Eds.), Time
Marquer, J., & Pereira, M. (1990). Reaction time in the study and human cognition: A life span perspectizje (pp. 185-218).
of strategies in sentence-picture verification: A reconsid- The Netherlands: Elsevier Science.
eration. The Quarterly Jourtinl ofExperimenta1 Psychology, Siegler, R. S., & Shipley, C. (1995). Variation, selection, and
42A, 147-168. cognitive change. In T. Simon and G. Halford (Eds.), De-
McGilly, K., & Siegler, R. S. (1989). How children choose zdoping cognitizle competence: Neal npproaches to process
among serial recall strategies. Child Deuelopnzelzt, 60, modeling. Hillsdale, N J : Erlbaum.
172-182. Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addi-
Miller, P., & Aloise-Young, P. (1996). Preschoolers' strategic tion and subtraction: How do children know what to do?
behaviors and performance on a same-different task. In C. Sophian (Ed.), The origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229-
Journal ofExperimenta1 Child Psychology, 60, 284-303. 293). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Miller, P. H., & Seier, W. L. (1994). Strategy utilization defi- Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). A microgenetic analysis
ciencies in children: When, where, and why. In H. W. Reese of conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries.
(Ed.), Adzlances in child developn~etifand belznvior (Vol. 25, Journal of Experimeiltal Psychology: General, 127, 377-
pp. 108-156). Nelv York: Academic Press. 397.
Robert S. Siegler 35

Smith, L. B., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). theory In W. Kessen (Ed.), P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.),
Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Htstory, theory, and
the A-not-B error. Psychological Rezlieu1, 106, 235-260. methods (pp. 213-236). New York: Wiley.
Spelke, E. S., & Newport, E. L. (1998). Nativism, empiri- Stokes, P. D. (1995). Learned variability. A n m a 1 Learning G.
cism, and the development of knowledge. In R. M. Behazllor, 23, 164-176.
Lerner (Ed.), W. Damon (Series Ed.), Handbook of child Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills. Monogmphs
psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human dez~elopment of the Soctety for Research tn Chtld Development, 56(1, Serial
(5th ed., pp. 275-340). New York: Wiley. No. 223).
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond lQ: A triarchic theory ofhtlmni~ van Geert, P. (1998).A dynamic systems model of basic de-
intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. velopmental mechanisms: Piaget, Vygotsky, and be-
Stevenson, H. (1983). How children learn: The quest for a yond. Psychological Reotez~l,105, 634-677.
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 1 of 2 -

You have printed the following article:


The Rebirth of Children's Learning
Robert S. Siegler
Child Development, Vol. 71, No. 1. (Jan. - Feb., 2000), pp. 26-35.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28200001%2F02%2971%3A1%3C26%3ATROCL%3E2.0.CO%3B2-5

This article references the following linked citations. If you are trying to access articles from an
off-campus location, you may be required to first logon via your library web site to access JSTOR. Please
visit your library's website or contact a librarian to learn about options for remote access to JSTOR.

References

Learning in the Development of Infant Locomotion


Karen E. Adolph; Bennett I. Bertenthal; Steven M. Boker; Eugene C. Goldfield; Eleanor J. Gibson
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 62, No. 3, Learning in the
Development of Infant Locomotion. (1997), pp. i+iii+v+1-162.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-976X%281997%2962%3A3%3Ci%3ALITDOI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-7

All Other Things Being Equal: Acquisition and Transfer of the Control of Variables Strategy
Zhe Chen; David Klahr
Child Development, Vol. 70, No. 5. (Sep. - Oct., 1999), pp. 1098-1120.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28199909%2F10%2970%3A5%3C1098%3AAOTBEA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-5

Experience and Brain Development


William T. Greenough; James E. Black; Christopher S. Wallace
Child Development, Vol. 58, No. 3. (Jun., 1987), pp. 539-559.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28198706%2958%3A3%3C539%3AEABD%3E2.0.CO%3B2-J
http://www.jstor.org

LINKED CITATIONS
- Page 2 of 2 -

Strategies of Knowledge Acquisition


Deanna Kuhn; Merce Garcia-Mila; Anat Zohar; Christopher Andersen; Sheldon H. White; David
Klahr; Sharon M. Carver
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 60, No. 4, Strategies of
Knowledge Acquisition. (1995), pp. i+iii+v-vi+1-157.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-976X%281995%2960%3A4%3Ci%3ASOKA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-B

How Children Choose among Serial Recall Strategies


Kate McGilly; Robert S. Siegler
Child Development, Vol. 60, No. 1. (Feb., 1989), pp. 172-182.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0009-3920%28198902%2960%3A1%3C172%3AHCCASR%3E2.0.CO%3B2-W

Hidden Skills: A Dynamic Systems Analysis of Treadmill Stepping during the First Year
Esther Thelen; Beverly D. Ulrich; Peter H. Wolff
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 56, No. 1, Hidden Skills: A
Dynamic Systems Analysis of Treadmill Stepping during the First Year. (1991), pp.
i+iii+v-vi+1-103.
Stable URL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0037-976X%281991%2956%3A1%3Ci%3AHSADSA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-2

También podría gustarte