Está en la página 1de 19

Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yfrne

Review

Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social–emotional


behavior: A review of single administration studies
Peter A. Bos a,⇑, Jaak Panksepp b, Rose-Marie Bluthé c, Jack van Honk a,d
a
Department of Experimental Psychology, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS, Utrecht, The Netherlands
b
Department of VCAPP, College Veterinary Medicine, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-6520, USA
c
Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique (INRA), Unité Mixte de Recherche 1286, PsyNuGen, Universite de Bordeaux 2, F-33076 Bordeaux, France
d
Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, J-Block Groote Schuur Hospital Observatory, Cape Town, South Africa

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Steroids and peptides mediate a diverse array of animal social behaviors. Human research is restricted by
Available online 21 January 2011 technical–ethical limitations, and models of the neuroendocrine regulation of social–emotional behavior
are therefore mainly limited to non-human species, often under the assumption that human social–emo-
Keywords: tional behavior is emancipated from hormonal control. Development of acute hormone administration
Social behavior procedures in human research, together with the advent of novel non-invasive neuroimaging techniques,
Testosterone have opened up opportunities to systematically study the neuroendocrinology of human social–emo-
Estradiol
tional behavior. Here, we review all placebo-controlled single hormone administration studies addressing
Vasopressin
Oxytocin
human social–emotional behavior, involving the steroids testosterone and estradiol, and the peptides
Hormone administration oxytocin and vasopressin. These studies demonstrate substantial hormonal control over human social–
Humans emotional behavior and give insights into the underlying neural mechanisms. Finally, we propose a the-
oretical model that synthesizes detailed knowledge of the neuroendocrinology of social–emotional
behavior in animals with the recently gained data from humans described in our review.
Ó 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction [71,125], partner preference [111], maternal bonding [116], social


status [168,213], and sexual behavior [35], but these models are of-
Ever since Hippocrates introduced the theory on humors in an- ten restricted to non-human species. One reason humans are not
cient Greece, excesses and shortages of key bodily fluids were tra- included in these models is the suspicion that evolutionarily recent
ditionally claimed to be responsible for personality characteristics neo-cortical expansions in higher primates, along with cultural
and health problems. Although our current knowledge of human evolution in humans during the last several million years
biology holds few similarities to such pre-scientific concepts, were [60,122], have liberated us from such biological constraints. This
we to exchange ‘neurotransmitters’ and ‘neurohormones’ for the bias tends to reduce the use of evidence from animal models, much
term ‘bodily fluids’, there would be outward resemblances be- of it based on rodent research, to illuminate what controls behavior
tween our modern understanding of factors that control emotions in higher mammals, and most especially the human condition. An-
and temperaments, and the classical Hippocratic theories that other key reason for minimizing the importance of such biological
seem so naive to us today. For instance, it is not uncommon for tra- factors for illuminating human behavior is the paucity of relevant
ditional neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamine to be endocrinological manipulations in well-controlled human research
envisioned as key regulators of diverse socio-emotional behaviors that would permit evaluation of more general cross-species evolu-
and related pathologies (e.g. depression and schizophrenia). A role tionary principles. Of course, both technical and ethical difficulties
for peripheral hormones in human social–emotional behavior is often limit causal investigations of relevant endocrine mechanisms
however much less acknowledged. in humans, leading to excessive reliance on correlational studies.
It is generally accepted that diverse social–emotional behaviors Such impediments for the validation of cross-species general-
in mammals are under control of steroid hormones and neuropep- izations are gradually diminishing. Despite difficulties in transla-
tides. Abundant evidence exists for hormonal control of aggression tional research, the past decade has seen the emergence of
potentially powerful new research techniques such as functional
brain imaging and grand scale genetic analyses, especially when
⇑ Corresponding author. Fax: +31 (0) 302534511.
combined with direct hormone manipulations and conducted
E-mail addresses: p.a.bos@uu.nl (P.A. Bos), jpanksepp@vetmed.wsu.edu (J.
along with rigorous psychological analyses, and are leading to
Panksepp), rosemarie.bluthe31@orange.fr (R.-M. Bluthé), j.vanhonk@uu.nl (J. van
Honk). the recognition that humans are not immune to the mental influ-

0091-3022/$ - see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.01.002
18 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

ences of many ancestral hormones. These widely available meth- ago) by gene duplication gave rise to two lineages [2]. Predecessors
ods, carefully applied, are illuminating the power and fruitfulness of the OT/AVP family are present in the earliest phyla of animal
of cross-species translational research [109,135]. Already, the use evolution and have largely maintained their structure over these
of new neuroscience techniques and new ways of administering 450 million years. In cyclostomes (jawless fish), one of the most
hormones and peptides to humans has led to increased attention anciently derived vertebrate species, only the peptide arginine
and focus on the neuro-endocrine underpinnings of human vasotocin (AVT) has been found, suggesting that the two lineages
sociality. probably evolved from AVT [3]. In addition to similarity in molec-
Our aim here is to provide a selective overview of the current ular structure of the neuropeptides through the phyla, strong par-
status of research involving steroid and peptide regulation of hu- allels exist between the gene expression patterns coding for these
man social–emotional behavior. Furthermore, based on animal re- peptides, and the diverse functions they exert [59]. AVP and OT,
search, complemented by recent human studies, we will present a and their evolutionary predecessors, are involved in sexual behav-
model for the neuroendocrinology of human social–emotional ior and sex-specific vocalizations in fish, amphibians, birds, and
behavior, which describes the behavioral functions of the steroid mammals [33,35,81]. Even in invertebrate species, conopressin, a
hormones testosterone (T) and estradiol (E), as well as the neuro- predecessor linked to the OT/AVP family, is involved in penile erec-
peptides oxytocin (OT) and vasopressin (AVP). The model positions tion and ejaculation [202], which is similar to the role of OT in hu-
the steroids and neuropeptides in a broader functional framework mans [31,184].
that importantly takes into account environmental influences on Akin to AVP and OT, the steroid hormones T and E share capac-
hormonal and neuropeptide actions, as well as the role they played ities rendering them important candidates for the regulation of so-
through evolution. We hope this model can serve as a heuristic cial–emotional behavior. These steroids have a common ancestry,
framework for future translational research. But first, we will give and are derived from closely related, yet ancient steroids [30].
a brief overview of the evolutionary trajectory of steroids and pep- Through the phyla, steroid hormones are importantly involved in
tides, and provide a summary of the social–emotional behaviors reproductive and sexual behavior, that is, in fish [30], rodents
these hormones are involved in based on animal studies. [80], and humans [166,190]. Research on the evolution of E and T
demonstrates that the first steroid receptor to evolve was an estro-
gen receptor, which later gave rise to other steroid receptors [188].
2. Steroid hormones and neuropeptides: candidates for the T and E are both mainly synthesized in the mammalian ovaries and
regulation of social behavior testes, and exert developmental and activational effects on the
CNS. These neuro-developmental effects, which drive sex-differen-
In the traditional view, hormones are secreted by bodily glands, tiation are relatively well-known [10]. The activational effects of
under the control of various brain-pituitary factors, to exert their steroids on social–emotional behavior are less well understood,
often slow effects on distal locations in the body. But recently, re- but these are currently in the spotlights of attention
search on the social–sexual neuropeptides OT and AVP has broad- [12,40,80,166]. The steroid hormones act on the brain through sev-
ened this view of hormonal actions and the role hormones play in eral genomic and non-genomic pathways, potentially having broad
the central nervous system (CNS) controlling mental processes. functional consequences for diverse social–emotional processes. T,
Synthesis of OT and AVP occurs mainly in the supraoptic nuclei and its metabolite 5a-dihydrotestosterone (DHT) both bind to the
(SON) and paraventricular nuclei (PVN) of the hypothalamus. Mag- androgen receptor (AR), which leads to relative slow genetic ef-
nocellular neurons in the SON and PVN project to the posterior fects, but recent findings show that androgens can also have fast,
pituitary to release OT and AVP in the bloodstream to exert their non-genomic effects in the order of seconds to minutes [94,136].
peripheral effects. Parvocellular neuropeptide containing neurons Also E can exert neural effects varying in longevity from a few sec-
in the PVN synthesize and project to diverse target regions within onds to days [40]. E, either directly synthesized in the ovaries or
the CNS to exert synergistic behavioral and psychological effects conversed from T by aromatase, can act on the estrogen receptor
leading to the coordination of brain and body states. However, a (ER) to exert genomic effects, but E can also have very fast non-
small portion of magnocellular neurons in the SON also projects genomic effects [12]. The temporal dynamic of steroid effects on
to various regions in the CNS [165], and might provide for a mech- behavior is even more enhanced by the temporal resolution by
anism describing the synergism between central and peripheral which aromatase can convert T–E. Aromatase, which is widespread
peptide release, which is observed in certain conditions [129]. To- in the human brain [20], can change its mode of action in a time
gether, OT and AVP neurons in the SON and PVN project to diverse span of several minutes, locally increase E availability, which puts
areas of the brain including anterior: the lateral septum, nucleus forward another pathway by which T can affect behavior [13,40].
accumbens (NAcc), and amgydala. Furthermore, these OT and Finally, important mechanisms by which steroids can influence so-
AVP neurons also project to the hippocampus rostrally, and cau- cial behavior are through developmental and direct effects that T
dally to many brain regions, especially brain stem regions such and E exert on AVP and OT expression respectively. Both E and T
as the ventral tegmental area and solitary tract nuclei, down to have a strong influence on the expression on these neuropeptides
the spinal cord [165]. Although the synthesis of OT and AVP is in several limbic brain regions [91,112,113]. Moreover, plasticity
mostly confined to highly localized nuclei in the rostral hypothal- in peptide functioning induced by sex steroids is a potential mech-
amus, the distribution of these neuropeptides is not restricted to anism underlying sex differences in social behavior [38,110]. To-
projections from these areas; the peptides can communicate more gether, the manifold mechanisms discussed above give unlimited
dynamically and holistically by extracellular diffusion and through temporal and spatial flexibility, and this dynamic neural modula-
release into the cerebral spinal fluid (CSF) for a review, see: tion is defensibly crucial in the complexity of social–emotional
[129,208]. These data show that OT and AVP do not fit the classical behavior.
profile of slow and non-specific hormone actions as typically
envisioned.
Further indications for a broad-scale role of neuropeptides in 3. Steroid and neuropeptide regulation of social–emotional
human social–emotional behavior are the preservation of their behavior
molecular structures, and cross-species behavioral effects through-
out evolution. OT and AVP originate from a single ancestral peptide The hormones and neuropeptides which were the prior regula-
that, early in vertebrate evolution (estimated at 450 million years tors of sexual behavior in evolutionary distant species, have gained
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 19

a more encompassing role through mammalian evolution as the mones of estrus to rise in the female. These hormones prime the
sustained importance of parental behavior towards offspring female brain’s heightened receptiveness to OT, which is released
increased. abundantly during mating. Responsiveness of the reward-seeking
The simple sexual-regulatory actions of OT, AVP, T and E grad- circuits and the olfactory system to the peptides are the prerequi-
ually extended to more complex social behavior such as bonding sites for pair formation to occur. In males, the release of T and AVP
between mothers and infants, partner bonding, social recognition, during mating is more important for pair formation. These findings
aggression between conspecifics, and ultimately the regulation of indicate that recognition and bonding between a mother and her
social cognition in primates. The broad spectrum of social behavior infant, and between parents might be founded on the same neural
can range from violent acts of aggression against a conspecific to mechanisms [41]. While the hormonal regulation of maternal care
apparent altruistic behavior towards unknown others. At both ends is well-documented, that of paternal care is still debated
of the spectrum, steroid and neuropeptide mechanisms are in- [171,216,217]. Despite the need for more research, a recent study
volved. We hereby provide an overview on the underlying endocri- showing that T can increase paternal care by conversion to E favors
nology of bonding and care on the one hand, and competition and the hypothesis of hormonal regulation of paternal care [189].
aggression on the other. Since bonding between individuals is dependent on mutual rec-
ognition, it is not surprising that the same steroids and peptides
3.1. Bonding and care which are involved in social bonding are also implicated in social
recognition. In most mammalian species, social recognition de-
Social bonding between individuals can be defined as behavior pends on pheromones, detected by the vomeronasal organ
in which individuals search for proximity to one another, without (VNO). Knockout mice, in which the OT or AVP receptors are dis-
any direct benefit for the individual besides the rewarding proper- abled, show large disturbances in social recognition [21,130]. Injec-
ties of the behavior itself, and the increase in reproductive fitness tions of both OT and AVP or their antagonists on specific neural
from an evolutionary perspective. Mother–infant bonding is per- locations demonstrate that OT action is essential for formation of
haps the most evolutionarily ancient form of social bonding, and social memory in the medial amygdale (MeA), while the role of
is strongly regulated by the above hormones and peptides, as well AVP in social recognition is confined to the lateral septum (LS)
as pleasure-satisfaction inducing endogenous opioids [148,150, [21,42]. AVP acts in the septum by changing output patterns to
154,155]. Studies on rats, mice, voles, chicks, and sheep have the hippocampus, which is a key region for memory formation,
uncovered a mechanism by which mother–infant recognition and and this neuroendocrine mechanism potentially underlies AVPs ef-
bonding is established [41,111,130,141]. During pregnancy estro- fects on social recognition [64]. Again, the role these peptides play
gen levels rise, which prime the brain for synthesis of OT and OT in this type of learning and social recognition seems to depend on
receptors. During parturition in mammals, which is often the onset activity of sex-specific hormones [23]. Pierman et al. [158] demon-
for formation of the mother–infant bond, high levels of catechola- strated disturbed social recognition and reduction in AVP activa-
mines are released, together with OT and AVP release from the SON tion in aromatase lacking mice, which are incapable of
and PVN [118,143]. As shown by administration studies with OT or metabolizing T into E and DHT. Administration of both of these T
an OT antagonist in rats, mice, and sheep, this release is essential metabolites restores both social recognition as well as AVP expres-
for the establishment of the mother–infant bond [117,121,144]. sion in the LS [158].
Afterwards, the influence of OT on the reward-seeking circuits
(NAcc) and the olfactory processing circuits of the brain promotes 3.2. Aggression and social challenge
establishment of mutual recognition and maternal bonding [41].
Studies on offspring show that OT effects on the reward-seeking Although aggression can be non-social, as in the case of preda-
circuitry is also responsible, in part, for the mutually soothing ef- tory aggression, most forms of aggression (e.g. aggression against
fect of maternal behavior such as breastfeeding and prevention an intruder, aggression against subordinates, or aggressive compe-
of separation distress in infants [35,68,149], effects which may tition over females) are deeply social processes. While research on
synergize with opioid-mediated social-reward circuitry of the social bonding and care has mostly focused on E and OT, aggressive
brain [150]. Although the role of OT in maternal care and bonding and competitive behavior has more often been linked to T and AVP.
is undisputed, recent findings show that AVP is also important in T is causally linked to aggression in many mammalian species
maternal behavior in rats [28]. [142], while AVP and AVT are likewise capable of increasing
Hormonal involvement in social bonding in not restricted to aggression in several bird species and mammals and are mostly
mother–infant bonding; recent studies have demonstrated implicated in male–male aggression [32,81].
involvement of peptides and steroids in partner bonding and pater- Although the role of T and AVP in social aggression is undis-
nal behavior [110]. With a focus on species wherein monogamous puted, T or AVP do not increase non-social forms of aggression
bonding naturally occurs, most of the research on this topic is per- [210]. This indicates that T might not globally affect all forms of
formed on voles. Of special interest are two families of voles, prai- aggression, but more likely, the intention to behave aggressively.
rie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and montane voles (Microtus This view has gained increasing attention since the proposal of
montanus). These genetically highly-related families show, besides the ‘‘challenge-hypothesis’’ [212], which hypothesizes that T is
many similarities, important differences in partner preference and not involved in aggressive behavior per se, but more in the percep-
bonding. The monogamous prairie vole shows cohabitation, bipa- tion of emotional social challenges an animal is confronted with.
rental care of young, and distress after forced separation from Social challenge can lead to aggression, but aggressive behavior
the partner, while the montane vole does not [108,130]. The differ- might not always be adaptive in every situation. The challenge
ences in social–emotional behavior between the two families has hypothesis proposes that T affects challenge behavior adaptively,
been traced to differences in OT and V1a receptor densities as promoted by rapid changes in neural sensitivity to steroids
[110]. Compared to montane voles, prairie voles have higher OT [80]. A recent finding contributes to this proposed dynamic steroid
receptor densities in the ventral striatal areas, especially the NAcc, mechanism by demonstrating rapid fluctuations of local forebrain
which cause OT to directly affect dopamine reward-seeking cir- E and T levels dependent on exposure to a male or female conspe-
cuits. This interaction is the basis of the model for pair bonding cific [163]. Findings that the effects of steroids and peptides are
in prairie voles [111,120], comparable to the model for mother–in- strongly dependent on contextual information concur with this
fant bonding as described above. Male pheromones induce hor- hypothesis. For instance, in many mammalian and avian species,
20 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

effects of T on aggressive and competitive behavior depend and care behavior, increased aggression by OT might seem parad-
strongly on mating and breeding strategies [212], mating season oxal, but maternal aggression can be envisioned as adaptive paren-
[178], territorial behavior, prior experience in conflict [80], and so- tal caring behavior, when mothers are protecting infants against
cial status [167]. The effects of these social peptides on aggression potentially dangerous intruders. Thus, AVP and OT both affect
also depend on social status, as shown in monkeys [213]. Variation maternal care, but since AVP and OT have opposite effects on amy-
among any of these variables demand different behavioral strate- gdalar output patterns [105], and on anxiety [49], the comparable
gies, e.g.; being highly aggressive and competitive outside mating effects of these neuropeptides are probably orchestrated by target-
season, or challenges by a low ranking animal against a superior, ing different aspects of the behavioral repertoire of maternal
will most likely reduce reproductive success. The flexibility and aggression. Taken together, these findings demonstrate that
plasticity of steroid metabolism, and their shaping effects on social involvement of steroids and neuropeptides in aggression and social
peptide expressions and receptor distributions make steroid hor- challenge is evident, although the exact mechanisms remain
mones and related neuropeptides good candidates for regulation unclear.
of the contextual dependency of social competitive behavior.
OT and AVP have both been associated with aggressive interac-
tions in male rodents. OT seems to decrease aggression [52], as op- 4. Neuropeptide and steroidal regulation of human social–
posed to AVP, which increases aggression. AVP exerts its effects on emotional behavior
aggression by acting on the amygdala and lateral septum, and the
sensitivity of the amygdala to AVP appears to be strongly depen- Although knowledge on the role of steroids and peptides in so-
dent on T [125]. Aggression is also facilitated after up regulation cial–emotional behavior has arisen primarily from animal research,
of AVP receptors by T in the hypothalamus [51]. Based on these the last decade has given rise to increasing numbers of human
and other findings the general view holds that AVP increases studies. This increase comes forth out of new research techniques
aggression in rodents. However, a recent study by Veenema et al. that are currently widely available, and new methods of adminis-
shows a more intricate effect of AVP [207]. These authors showed tration of hormones and peptides in human research. The use of
that although AVP indeed increases aggression by acting on the lat- functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) has been especially
eral septum, it also decreases aggression when acting on the bed influential in promoting the study of social neuroendocrinology in
nucleus of the stria terminalis, indicating a more versatile role of humans. Also of importance has been the wide utilization and val-
AVP in terms that it can both up- and down regulate aggression. idation of the brain effects of nasal administration of various
During stressful events, such as aggressive encounters, OT and neuropeptides.
AVP are released and affect regions such as the PVN, the septum, In contrast to the steroids which can easily do so, OT and AVP
and amgydala. OT is furthermore co-released in the blood, specifi- cannot cross the blood brain barrier (BBB). This difficulty in getting
cally during social stress in the PVN [22] and the central amgydala peptides into the human brain has been a major impediment for
[61], which results in reduced anxiety and inhibition of the hypo- studying the effects of neuropeptides on behavior. Intranasal
thalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) stress response [211]. OT thus administration of peptides is a pathway which circumvents the
seems important in attenuating the stress response and reinstating BBB, probably by diffusion through the subarachnoid space above
homeostasis [144,183]. In contrast to OT, AVP apparently has anx- the olfactory epithelium. Born et al. [24] showed that administra-
iogenic effects, and is more strongly expressed in the septum and tion of nasal spray containing AVP led to an increase of AVP in
the PVN during non-social stressful conditions, which has led to CSF and in the blood 10 min after administration and continually
the hypothesis that AVP is involved in coping with stress outside increased for 80 min. It must be noted that the increase in AVP var-
the social realm [66,215]. In addition to its anxiogenic effects, ied largely between subjects, but the average increase in AVP CSF
AVP released at the level of the septum also seems to facilitate sim- levels was nonetheless multiple times the baseline level in the pla-
ple stimulus–response associations, while interfering with com- cebo condition. Whether the heightened AVP baseline in CSF and
plex stimulus processing. This property of AVP is thought to be a blood levels relate to normal physiological fluctuations remains
mechanism which promotes goal-directed behavior aimed at more unclear, but important with regard to the dosage is that Born
successfully dealing with threat [64]. Moreover, in the PVN, AVP et al. administrated a high dose of 40 international units (IU) while
release in response to emotional stress can down regulate the most OT studies apply 24 IU, which might lead to less increased
HPA response, a candidate mechanism to attenuate stress respond- CSF values. Since AVP is not expected to cross the BBB, an unex-
ing [215] (for an elaborate review on OT and AVP regulation of the pected finding in the Born et al. study was that heightened blood
HPA stress response, see: [65]). levels of AVP were observed. The authors suggest however, that
In females, there is also a role for AVP and OT in maternal there might have been active transport of AVP between the blood
aggression, initiated to protect offspring against intruders. AVP is and CSF, a mechanism which has indeed been proposed in the con-
elevated in the amgydala during maternal aggression in highly text of administration of high pharmacological dosages [129].
anxious rats, which tend to demonstrate increased maternal Problematic with respect to this explanation, is a study which com-
aggression. Blocking of AVP receptors in the amgydala reduces pared intravenous versus intranasal administration of AVP. Neural
maternal aggression in these rats [29]. In addition, in low anxious effects were exclusively found after intranasal administration, thus
rats, administration of synthetic AVP in the central nucleus of the showing a clear dissociation between both forms of administration
amgydala increases maternal aggression. These effects of AVP on [159]. Despite these issues, which remain to be addressed, the
maternal aggression are mediated by AVP action on the amgydala, study by Born et al. does show that nasal administration of AVP
but not the PVN [29]. OT, known for its role in maternal care and is a valid way for getting peptides into the brain. This validation
bonding, also facilitates maternal aggression against conspecifics, study importantly paved the way for experiments on peptide ef-
especially against other (virgin) females [34], and is more pro- fects on human behavior, and led to a sudden increase in OT-
nounced in highly anxious animals [27]. This effect of OT is regu- administration studies in humans.
lated by the PVN and amygdala, although the effect on the Research on the effects of the steroid hormone T on behavior
amgydala seems confined to highly anxious rats. E has also been was boosted by a study which investigated the effect of T on sexual
associated with maternal aggression [146], and it has been pro- arousal in women, and thereby established a valid method for a
posed that effects of E on maternal aggression are mediated by sublingual administration of T in healthy women [190]. In that
interacting with OT [38]. With respect to the role of OT in bonding study, Tuiten et al. show that blood levels of T after a single sublin-
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 21

gual administration of 0.5 mg of T in healthy young women led to a tration of OT in healthy adult subjects can bring down cortisol lev-
10-fold increase of T in the blood, reaching the maximum 15 min els, an index for reduced stress, but adults who endured parental
after intake and returning to baseline within 1.5 h [190]. However, separation early in life show less of this reduction in cortisol
significantly elevated vaginal pulse amplitude peaked 4 h after [134]. Although the literature on human mother–infant bonding
administration. Thus, the physiological effect of T administration consists mostly of correlational studies, the abundance of correla-
peaked 2.5 h after T levels in the blood returned to baseline. The tions and the thorough animal literature which it is founded upon,
neurobiological mechanisms underlying this delayed effect is not make it very unlikely that OT is not a key player in human mother–
understood, but one explanation could be that some effects of T infant bonding.
administration are only established after T is metabolized, hence Obtaining causal data on mother–infant bonding is strongly re-
slowing down the hormone’s behavioral effects. In addition, geno- stricted for ethical reasons, but fortunately ‘‘natural administra-
mic effects could also delay the effects of the T administration, as tion’’ studies do exist, though we have to assume that there is
proposed based on animal studies that have demonstrated a simi- co-release in the brain. Evidence that peripheral OT influences
lar 4-h delay [40]. On the other hand, a recently developed method brain circuitry is currently lacking, but indications for co-release
of nasal administration of T, whereby subjects are measured after during maternal behavior such as parturition and lactation have
30 min seems to yield effects similar to the effects of sublingual been reported [144]; a potential mechanism for this co-release in
administration of T after the 4-h delay [100,206]. Another issue the CNS is described above [165]; see Section 2. During normal
regarding the T administration is the 10-fold increase of T levels delivery, the mother produces high levels of OT, which facilitate
in the blood, which seems rather high in relation to a physiological both parturition as well as the onset of breastfeeding. During
baseline. It is however conjectured by van der Made et al. [195] cesarean section, this surge in OT is not apparent. Mothers who
that this increase will not produce a proportional increase in the have undergone cesarean section show less social behavior to-
free fraction of T; the amount of T reaching the brain. The admin- wards their child and report more anxiety compared to mothers
istrated T will first bind to sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG; who have undergone normal delivery [34,192]. These women also
and to albumin, to a smaller extent) before being able to produce show less neural responsiveness towards pictures of their own
an increase of the free fraction T. Although more knowledge on children in regions of the brain known to be involved in maternal
the pharmakinetics of T administration is warranted, as well as val- bonding [15,181]. This finding might be an indication that OT,
idation of this method in males, the Tuiten et al. study was an probably in concert with E, primes the mother’s brain towards
important methodological step forwards for gathering direct in- her infant facilitating the mother–infant bond, comparable to the
sight in the effects of T on human social behavior. mechanism in animals described above. From an evolutionary per-
In contrast to T, E is safely administrated by means of oral con- spective, mother–infant bonding is perhaps the oldest and most
traceptives by millions of women on a daily basis and is thus fundamental form of social bonding, which developed early in ver-
widely available. However, research investigating acute adminis- tebrate evolution [131]. It is proposed that maternal care and
tration effects on social–emotional behavior is scarce. Also, inves- bonding underlies empathy in its broadest sense as we know it to-
tigations on the acute effects of AVP are rather limited until now, day [104], and a role of OT in other forms of social bonding has
and only seven of these studies specifically focused on social therefore been suggested. The appearance of a series of OT-admin-
behavior. Therefore, the following review of human studies on istration studies in the last decade does indeed point in the direc-
neuropeptide and steroid mechanisms will mostly focus on studies tion of a causal role of OT in human social, interpersonal behavior.
using T and OT administration, in contrast to studies employing
correlational methods, which measure peptide and hormone levels
under different conditions (Table 1). Correlational studies are 4.1.1. Social perception
undeniably of great importance in showing how certain hormone Social bonding behavior can only exist when animals are capa-
levels and responses are associated with certain behaviors, but ble of social recognition (e.g.; there is no evidence for one’s own
we will focus on administration studies because these studies pup-recognition in species like laboratory rats). Therefore social
can potentially establish a causal role of hormones in human so- perception, which is essential for social recognition and memory,
cial–emotional behavior. has been investigated in relation to OT. In a study where investiga-
tors compared perception of social stimuli with non-social stimuli,
4.1. Bonding and care OT improved recognition for facial pictures compared to pictures of
houses [164]. OT also affected evaluative ratings on arousal of
Studies on the endocrinology of human bonding have mostly fo- emotional stimuli, which was again specific to the social compared
cused on OT, since the role of OT in mother–infant bonding, both in to the non-social stimuli [145]. A comparable effect was found by
animals and humans is widely studied and well established Guastella et al., who report that OT leads to better recognition of
[34,35,193]. A series of human studies have shown that OT blood faces, although specifically for happy facial expressions, and not
levels are correlated with maternal attachment, caring behavior to- for neutral and angry expressions [90].
wards a new-born infant, and with neural responses towards in- An effect of OT for specific positive social–emotional informa-
fant cues in OT-related regions (e.g. striatum, hypothalamus) tion was also found in a task in which administration of OT led
[70,180,192]. Furthermore, OT levels in the CSF are lowered in wo- to faster recognition of positive words related to sex and relation-
men with a history of childhood abuse [92]. Since CSF levels are ships, compared to words related to happiness or safety [191]. The
more likely to reflect neural OT levels compared to blood measure- same research group tested whether this effect was caused by bet-
ments, this especially is an important finding. Also children who ter detection of positive facial stimuli at an early perceptual level,
have been subjected to parental neglect early in life, show abnor- but could not find an effect of OT on the detection of happy or an-
mally low levels of OT as well as AVP compared to control children, gry facial expressions [84]. The effect of OT is thus perhaps limited
after interacting with their mother [76]. This demonstrates that OT to later stages of processing, although the finding might also have
is not only implicated in maternal bonding on the mother’s side, been due to the fact that they used facial pictograms instead of
but is also affecting the infant, apparently facilitating mother–in- photographs. In another experiment, facial pictures were used in
fant bonding in a reciprocal way. Problems in the formation of a an emotion-detection task, which again found no effect of OT on
mother–infant bond can lead not only to lower levels of OT, but recognition, indeed pointing in the direction of an effect of OT at
also reduced sensitivity to OT administration later in life. Adminis- a later stage of processing [53]. They did find that OT slowed the
22 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

Table 1
Overview of single peptide and steroid administration studies on human social behavior. (F = females, M = males, N = number of participants, b = between subject design,
w = within subject design, ASD = autism spectrum disorder, (G)SAD = (generalized) social anxiety disorder, UG = ultimatum game).

Author Year N Sex Administration Effect of administration


Estradiol
Kaya et al. 2003 19w F 300 lg intranasal Shift from sympathetic to parasympathetic activation of the central nervous system
Schleifer et al. 2002 12w F 0.2, 0.4, 0.8 mg No effects on self reporting of mood
transdermal
Oxytocin
Alvares et al. 2010 74b M/ 24 IU intranasal Increased willingness to engage in a computerized social ball game
F
Andari et al. 2010 13w M/ 24 IU intranasal Increased social interaction during a computer game in ASD patients
F
Bartz et al. 2010 27w M 24 IU intranasal Increased empathic accuracy in subjects scoring higher on an ASD questionnaire
Baumgartner et al. 2008 41b M 24 IU intranasal Sustained trust despite unfair offers of opponents; reduced amygdala and midbrain
activation
Burri et al. 2008 10w M 24 IU intranasal Enhanced perception of sexual arousal; self reported
de Dreu et al. 2010 49/67/ M 24 IU intranasal Increased in-group cooperation, but reduced out-group cooperation
75b
Declerck et al. 2010 259b M/ 24 IU intranasal Less cooperation without social interaction, more cooperation after social interaction
F
Di Simplicio et al. 2009 29b M 24 IU intranasal Slower reaction times for indentifying fearful facial expressions
Ditzen et al. 2009 94b M/ 40 IU intranasal Reductions in cortisol levels, and increased positive communication during couple
F conflict
Domes et al. 2007 30w M 24 IU intranasal Better performance on ‘the reading the eyes in the mind task’
Domes et al. 2007 13w M 24 IU intranasal Reduced amygdala responses after exposure to negative and positive emotional faces
Domes et al. 2010 16w W 24 IU intranasal Increased responsiveness of the amygdala (and of several cortical regions to emotional
faces)
Evans et al. 2010 18w M 24 IU intranasal Decreased aversion to angry faces in associative learning task
Fischer-Shofty et al. 2010 27w M 24 IU intranasal Increased recognition of facial expression of fear
Gamer et al. 2010 46b M 24 IU intranasal Differential effects on sub-regions of the amygdala depending on emotional valence
Guastella et al. 2008 52b M 24 IU intranasal Increased gazing towards the eye region of faces
Guastella et al. 2008 69b M 24 IU intranasal Better memory for previous judgments on positive emotional faces
Guastella et al. 2009 104b M 24 IU intranasal No effect on detection of angry or happy facial expressions
Guastella et al. 2009 25b M 24 IU intranasal No effect on outcome of exposure therapy in SAD patients
Guastella et al. 2009 16w M 18 IU/24 IU intranasal Improvement of performance in mind-reading from the eyes in ASD patients
Heinrichs et al. 2003 37b M 24 IU intranasal Reductions in stress when OT administration is combined with social support
Hollander et al. 2003 15w M/ 4-h intravenous Reduction of repetitive behavior in ASD patients
F
Hollander et al. 2006 15w M/ 4-h intravenous Increased retention of emotional sentences in ASD patients
F
Hurlemann et al. 2010 48b M 24 IU intranasal Increased affective empathy and increased social learning
Keri and Benedek 2009 20w M/ 24 IU intranasal Heightened sensitivity for biological motion
F
Kirsch et al. 2005 15w M 27 IU intranasal Reduced amygdala responsiveness towards negative emotional faces
Kosfeld et al. 2005 58b M 24 IU intranasal Increased trust in a monetary exchange task
Labuschagne et al. 2010 38w M 24 IU intranasal Reduced amygdala responsiveness towards fearful faces in GSAD patients compared to
controls
Marsh et al. 2010 50b M/ 35.2 mg intranasal Increased sensitivity in detecting happy facial expressions
F
Meinlschmidt and 2006 19b M 24 IU intranasal Reduces sensitivity to OT in men with early parental separation
Heim
Mikolajczak et al. 2010 60b M 32 IU intranasal Increase in trust towards reliable subjects, but not towards unreliable subjects
Mikolojczak et al. 2010 60b M 32 IU intranasal Increased trust in a non-monetary exchange task
Naber et al. 2010 17w M 24 IU intranasal Increased responsiveness and less hostility in fathers during interaction with their own
child
Norman et al. 2010 45b M/ 20 IU intranasal Reduced arousal ratings for social threatening pictures compared to non-social pictures
F
Perry et al. 2010 24w M 24 IU intranasal Changes in EEG signal during biological motion perception pointing at mirror-neuron
activation
Petrovic et al. 2008 23b M 32 IU intranasal Reduced conditioning sensitivity; reduced activation in the amygdala and the ACC
Pitman et al. 1993 30b M 20 IU intranasal Less facial muscle responses during combat imagery compared to placebo and AVP
Rimmele et al. 2009 44b M 24 IU intranasal Improved memory for faces compared to non-social stimuli
Savaskan et al. 2008 36b M/ 20 IU intranasal Improved memory for faces
F
Shamay-Tsoory et al. 2009 56w M/ 24 IU intranasal Increased envy and gloating over monetary gain or loss of other participant
F
Singer et al. 2008 20w M 32 IU intranasal Reduced amygdala responses to pain stimuli
Theodoridou et al. 2009 96b M/ 24 IU intranasal Higher ratings of trustworthiness and attractiveness of pictures of faces
F
Unkelbach et al. 2008 44b M 24 IU intranasal Facilitation of the recognition of positive sex and relationship words
Zak et al. 2007 68b M 40 IU intranasal Higher offers in the UG
Testosterone
Aarts and van Honk 2009 24w F 0.5 mg sublingual Increased motivation for action in general
Bos et al. 2010 16w F 0.5 mg sublingual Stronger responses of thalamus and cingulate cortex to infant crying
Bos et al. 2010 24w F 0.5 mg sublingual Decreased trust, specifically in high trusting humans
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 23

Table 1 (continued)

Author Year N Sex Administration Effect of administration


Eisenegger et al. 2010 121b F 0.5 mg sublingual Higher offers in the UG, after correction for ‘belief’ effects of testosterone
Hermans et al. 2006 20w F 0.5 mg sublingual Reduced startle responses
Hermans et al. 2006 20w F 0.5 mg sublingual Reductions in empathic facial mimicry
Hermans et al. 2007 20w F 0.5 mg sublingual Reductions in stress responses to aversive pictures
Hermans et al. 2008 12w F 0.5 mg sublingual Enhanced activation of amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex when looking at facial
expressions
Hermans et al. 2010 12w F 0.5 mg sublingual Enhanced nucleus accumbens responses during reward anticipation
van Honk et al. 2001 16w F 0.5 mg sublingual Enhanced cardiac responses to angry faces
van Honk et al. 2004 12w F 0.5 mg sublingual Increased risk taking on the IOWA gambling task
van Honk et al. 2005 16w F 0.5 mg sublingual Reductions in vigilance toward unseen fearful faces in a masked emotional stroop task
van Honk and 2007 16w F 0.5 mg sublingual Impaired recognition of negative facial emotional expressions
Schutter
Schutter and van 2004 16w F 0.5 mg sublingual Reduced cortico-sub-cortical connectivity as indexed by cross frequency EEG
Honk
Tuiten et al. 2000 8w F 0.5 mg sublingual Enhanced physiological and subjectively experienced sexual arousal
van Wingen et al. 2008 25 w F 0.9 mg intranasal More automated processing during memorizing of male faces in females
van Wingen et al. 2009 25w F 0.9 mg intranasal Enhancement of amygdala reaction to facial threat in middle-aged woman
van Wingen et al. 2010 25w F 0.9 mg intranasal Reduced connectivity between the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex
Zak et al. 2009 25w M 0.1 g in cutaneous gel Lower offers in the UG
Vasopressin
Ebstein et al. 2009 27 b M 20 IU intranasal Increased cortisol response during social stress
Pitman et al. 1993 28b M 20 IU intranasal More facial muscle responses during combat imagery compared to placebo and OT
Guastella et al. 2010 48b M 20 IU intranasal Enhanced encoding of happy and angry faces compared to neutral faces
Guastella et al. 2010 41b M 20 IU intranasal Enhanced recognition of sex-related words
Thompson et al. 2004 27b M 20 IU intranasal Enhanced facial muscle responses to neutral faces
Thompson et al. 2006 74b M/ 20 IU intranasal Enhanced competitive facial muscle responses in M; more affiliative responses in F
F
Zink et al. 2010 20w M 40 IU intranasal Attenuation of activity in amygdala-prefrontal network in response to fearful faces

response time towards fearful faces, which might indicate less be involved in directing the gaze towards fearful faces [77], and has
attention towards threat-related stimuli. therefore been proposed to be involved in the effect of OT on face
However, in contrast to the findings specific to positive social processing.
information, an opposite finding is reported in a study showing Modern brain imaging supports the above findings. The first
improvement of facial recognition by OT, but this time specifically published fMRI study using OT administration by Kirsch et al.
for angry and neutral faces and not for happy facial expressions [124] demonstrated an attenuating effect of OT on the amygdala
[169]. In contrast to these studies using facial pictures, Keri and in response to threatening faces and scenes. This finding was rep-
Benedek used biological motion [119]. OT administration led to licated and extended in a study by Domes et al. [56] who also in-
heightened sensitivity in detecting biological motion (a walking cluded happy faces in their design and found that OT also
character) compared to non-biological motion, indicating that the reduces amygdala reactivity towards these non-threatening facial
effect of OT on the detection of social stimuli is not restricted to fa- stimuli. However, a recent high resolution imaging study shows
cial pictures. Together, these studies show that OT facilitates recog- that OT can have a different effect on sub-regions of the amygdala
nition of social stimuli, specifically faces, compared to non-social dependent on the valence of the perceived stimuli [78]. In this
stimuli. So far only two studies have been performed which inves- study, Gamer et al. show that OT attenuated amygdala activation
tigate the behavioral effects of AVP on human social perception. In in the lateral and dorsal region in response to fearful faces, but at
these studies, it was found that AVP enhances the encoding of the same time increased activation in response to happy faces.
faces, but specifically for happy and angry facial expressions com- Thus OT’s effect on the amygdala does seem valence specific,
pared to neutral faces [87], pointing at increased attention towards although how this relates to the behavioral findings on social per-
emotionally significant stimuli, regardless of valence. A somewhat ception is currently not known. These imaging data do show that
comparable effect was found in the other study on AVP [88], the hormone decreases amygdala reactivity towards facial threat
wherein it was observed that AVP leads to faster detection of stimuli, leading to less fearful responding and hence more direct
sex-related words. It is however not clear whether AVP leads to gazing towards eye regions. Recently, a study has tested the
general faster detection, and in the case of sex-related words being hypotheses whether OT would specially reduce aversion to threat-
more salient, only differentiates for these items. ening faces [67]. In this study, emotional facial pictures were
The data so far, is not convincing as to whether these peptide paired with a monetary reward. Under placebo administration,
effects on facial perception are dependent on the valence of the participants avoid angry facial expressions, irrelevant of the re-
face, or whether it occurs during a very early stage of visual pro- ward. Under OT administration however, this avoidance towards
cessing, such as immediate detection. But more insight into the angry faces is reduced. This interpretation of reduced fearful
mechanism whereby OT facilitates facial recognition, is provided responding towards threat is congruent with research on the
by the finding that OT increases gazing towards the eye region of stress-reducing effects of OT [95], which demonstrates that OT re-
neutral faces [89]. Information of the eye region of the face is of duces cortisol responses and subjectively experiences anxiety,
great importance for correctly identifying the emotional expres- most effectively when in combination with social support.
sions of others (mind-reading), and is pivotal in human communi- The attenuating effects of OT on experienced stress, well-known
cation [14]. OT might enhance facial processing by increasing from animal studies described above, have been replicated in dif-
attention towards the eye region, thereby facilitating future recog- ferent paradigms in humans, e.g.; OT reduces the sensitivity for
nition. The amygdala, an important target of OT [105], is known to the conditioning of social stimuli with electric shocks [157], and
24 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

also reduces amygdala reactions in response to an electric shock tion to act, explaining the imaging data as well as the subjective
[177]. Reduced anxiety by OT at the level of the amygdala, specif- findings. Such different interpretations of the effect of T on social
ically for social stimuli, serves as a good model for explaining the behavior are however not mutually exclusive, but remain to be
findings on increased gazing towards eye regions. However, it experimentally parsed. The diversity of effects may point to a more
should be noted that in this study increased gazing was only found subtle role of T in social–emotional behavior, wherein the effects of
towards neutral faces. Furthermore, higher levels of social anxiety hormones interact with environment and experience [80,82]. This
correlate with increased gazing towards threatening faces, which view will be explained in more detail below.
seems somewhat at odds with the findings on OT [139]. Future
studies need to further investigate if increased gazing after OT 4.1.2. Social cognition and empathy
administration towards the eye region also holds for emotional fa- While the studies on the hormonal effects involved in social
cial expressions, and how this gazing relates to amygdala activa- perception are mostly limited to the effects on primary emotions,
tion and subjectively experienced threat. some studies have attempted to look at hormonal effects on empa-
A limitation of the research of OT effects on social recognition is thy and social cognition, such as mind-reading. Domes et al. [57]
that most data is only obtained from male subjects. The choice to measured the effect of OT administration on mind-reading, the
limit the subject group to males is mostly to avoid interactions ability to consciously infer the intentions of others, by using a task
with cyclic hormonal fluctuations in female participants. A recent developed to measure social impairments in people with autism
imaging study which investigated the effect of OT on face process- spectrum disorder [14]. The task consists of 36 pictures of eye re-
ing specifically in females found different activation patterns com- gions of faces with a specific emotional expression accompanied by
pared to data obtained from male subjects [58]. Interestingly, four possible answers among which participants have to choose
females showed more reactivity in the amygdala in response to the correct emotion (reading the mind in the eyes task; RMET).
fearful faces after OT as compared to placebo, but with regard to OT administration led to better performance on this task, specifi-
the finding of Gamer et al. [78], we cannot be confident that the cally for the difficult items in the set of 36 pictures. Apparently,
same sub-regions of the amygdala were affected in males and fe- OT increases the sensitivity for subtle facial cues, an effect that
males. Also, in females activation was enhanced by OT in the infe- may be a result of elevated visual scanning of these faces. A differ-
rior frontal gyrus (IFG) and the superior temporal gyrus (STG), both ent task was used in a recent publication on the effects of OT on
of which are involved in the evaluation and interpretation of com- emotion recognition. In this task, subjects had to indicate what
plex stimuli such as emotional expressions, mind-reading, and imi- emotion was shown in movies of neutral faces which slowly mor-
tation[203]. This finding indicates that the current knowledge on phed into a full-blown emotion, at several points in time. After OT
the effects of OT on the brain is biased by not taking into account administration subjects correctly identified happy emotional
the striking sexual dimorphism of the brain. The next challenge is expressions at lower levels of morphing, indicating higher sensitiv-
to elucidate the effects of OT on face processing in both sexes, and ity for positive emotional expressions in this task [132]. However, a
investigate the mechanism by which OT affects the different neural comparable study using only the eye region of morphing faces in a
regions involved in social processing. similar task setup, showed increased detection selectively for fear-
Considering the well-known role of OT in social bonding in ani- ful facial expressions [73]. Whether increased emotion recognition
mals, this hormone has been the primary focus of studies on hu- is selective or not, is thus still unresolved.
mans. Nonetheless, with regard to perceptions of emotional OT also increases the accuracy in judgment when people are
stimuli, a finding we recently published shows that T administra- asked to indicate how other people feel when discussing an emo-
tion in young, non-parental women enhanced neural responsive- tional event [16], indicating increased empathy. This effect was
ness towards infant crying in a neural circuit underlying however specific to participants scoring low on social competence.
maternal care [25]. Subjective ratings by the participants showed Thus OT seems to affect only those people in whom there is room
that after T administration the crying was experienced as less aver- for improvement and leaves socially proficient people unaffected.
sive, perhaps indicating increased propensity to care and act on the Even stronger indications for increased empathy by OT are pro-
crying. In pregnant women, T levels are known to gradually rise in vided using a comparable task in which participants have to judge
concert with progesterone and E, levels all of which rapidly decline emotional pictures. The data showed that after OT, the pictures are
after parturition, which might possibly prime the brain during experienced as more emotional by the participants, leaving ratings
pregnancy for parental care [75]. Male partners of pregnant wo- of the emotional state of the people depicted on the photos unaf-
men show hormonal fluctuations similar to those of pregnant wo- fected [106]. In this study thus, OT specifically increased affective
men, indicating a shared comparable endocrine substrate for empathy without changing cognitive empathy (mind-reading). In
parenting in both sexes [179]. Also, T levels in fathers increase the same study, participants performed a learning task with a so-
when listening to infant cries [74]. Based on these findings, and cial and a non-social condition after OT or placebo administration.
the animal research described above, we have proposed two mech- In this task, OT specifically facilitated social learning, confirming
anisms that could account for this increased propensity to care that OT seems to increase the sensitivity to social information.
after administration of T in young women [25]. One mechanism The underlying neural mechanism by which OT affects social
is conversion of T–E by aromatase, after which E could next in- cognition and empathy has not yet been investigated directly,
crease OT synthesis and subsequently increase caring behavior but fMRI studies using the RMET have shown that the task elicits
[40,45]. In mice, heightened conversion of T–E by aromatase has neural activation in several regions identical to the regions acti-
indeed been demonstrated to increase paternal care [189], and in vated by OT during face processing (e.g.; superior temporal sulcus:
humans OT administration leads to increased positive interaction STS; amygdala; inferior frontal gyrus: IFG) [4,58]. These regions are
between fathers and their children [140]. Another, more direct thus good candidates for the mechanism behind the hormonal
mechanism is that T would directly act on AVP synthesis, as AVP modulation of mind-reading.
has also been shown to be involved in maternal care [28]. While The effect of T on the conscious detection of emotion has also
this interpretation in terms of increased social bonding by T is cer- been investigated. Using a similar design as describe above, with
tainly appealing, it cannot be excluded that the increased respon- movies of gradually changing faces, threatening facial emotions
siveness induced by T reflects, more directly, an effect of T on (anger; fear; disgust) were detected at a later stage after T admin-
other affective responses to social challenges (see below). From istration compared to placebo [199]. Based on this study, it appears
this perspective T could lead to reduced fear but increased motiva- that T might reduce social cognition, and possibly also mind-read-
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 25

ing, which is opposite to the effect of OT which enhances this ad- the first player, but since these higher offers were more often not
vanced capacity. This finding is however based on only one study, accepted, it led to less financial gain in the OT group [219]. These
and more data on the effect of T on social cognition and empathy is findings demonstrate that in economic exchange, more trust can
definitely warranted. come with the price of losing valuable resources.
Such apparently non-adaptive effects of OT underscore the
4.1.3. Social interaction importance of social environments wherein these exchanges take
Social bonding is all about social interaction between individu- place. From an evolutionary perspective, anonymous monetary ex-
als, whereas the data on humans described above are obtained in change of resources is a highly unnatural situation, as trading inter-
isolation. These findings tap into the processes of interaction by fa- actions never occur in a social vacuum, a framework that is
cial expressions and are therefore of great value. But to validate increasingly common in modern at-a-distance investments. How
hormonal effects on social behavior, one needs to measure behav- the social environment interacts with the role of hormones on
ior during social interaction. An important line of research having behavior is clearly demonstrated in three recent studies
done so, involves the hormonal effects on interpersonal trust. Long [6,50,137]. In an adaptation of the trust game, subjects played sev-
term human social interaction such as relationships and coopera- eral rounds against subjects that were either explained to them as
tion are often based of reciprocal trust, and is therefore essential being reliable or unreliable [137]. OT increased trust when playing
for the establishment of social bonding [104]. In an economic ex- with reliable subjects, but did not affect the amount of trust in the
change task specifically developed to measure the amount of trust unreliable group. In yet another monetary game, which was de-
people give to others, OT was administrated [126]. In this experi- signed to promote cooperation, Declerck et al. show that OT in-
ment, participants were given an amount of monetary units creased cooperation in participants who socially interacted before
(MU’s) and were asked to entrust any amount of MU’s to another the game, but reduced cooperation in an anonymous version of
unacquainted player who they were randomly assigned to. The en- the game [50]. A different game was used by Alvares et al. [6],
trusted MU’s would be tripled by the experiment leader and upon who used a computer ball game with two virtual players, in which
receiving the tripled MU’s, the second player could choose to re- a ball could be tossed back and forth. The participant could however
turn any amount of MU’s to the first player. In this game, both be included in the game, or excluded by not getting the ball. While
players can profit if the first player entrusts more MU’s to the sec- OT had no effect on subjects that were excluded, the hormone did
ond player, at the risk of not getting the invested MU’s back. Alter- increase the motivation to play another round of the game when
natively, the first player can play safe by keeping his MU’s, thus subjects were included in the game, thus increasing the willingness
guaranteeing a smaller gain. Participants in the group receiving to engage in a social game after positive response [6]. These find-
OT entrusted a significantly higher amount of MU’s to the second ings, which show the strong interaction between environment
player compared to participants in the placebo group. 45% of the and hormonal effects, stress the importance of taking into account
OT group entrusted all MU’s to the trustee, compared to 21% in the ecological circumstances in which social behavior is measured.
the placebo group. This effect was specific to social trust, because To better understand the dynamics of human social cooperation
in a control task OT had no effect on the amount of given trust and associated hormonal influences, it is necessary to take into ac-
when participants were told that the returned MU’s would be count the situation wherein such behaviors evolved. Like many
based on change defined by a computer. A recent study has indi- mammalian species, humans are social animals, and the strong
cated that the increased trust extends beyond monetary invest- sociality probably came to a full growth when our ancestors
ments, as OT also increased trust when confidential information started living in groups in which mutual understanding and coop-
was at stake [138]. eration between non-kin became essential for survival [104].
The effect of OT on trust has also been investigated using sub- Group living was the context wherein the hormonal mechanisms
jective trustworthiness-ratings of unknown faces, which showed of social interaction acted for thousands of years, which might ex-
that OT also leads to higher ratings of trustworthiness [185]. Trust- plain why effects of hormones involved in human social interaction
worthiness-ratings correlate strongly with monetary investments, are so strongly modulated by social context. Three different studies
which concurs with the finding of OT influencing both the affective applying OT in different social settings clearly demonstrate the
(trustworthiness-ratings) and cognitive (monetary-investment) nature of this dynamic environmental–endocrine interaction. In a
aspects of the task [194]. OT thus leads to more trust towards unfa- study by Ditzen et al. [55] married couples were observed during
miliar individuals, but as Baumgartner et al. [17] show, this in- a 10 min discussion on an issue that led to conflict in the past. In
crease in trust comes with a cost. In a comparable paradigm as the couples receiving OT, partners showed significantly increased
described above by Kosfeld et al. [126], Baumgartner let people positive interaction and communication and showed a reduction
play several rounds of the trust game with different opponents. in stress compared to placebo, demonstrating increased social
Halfway through the experiment, participants were told that their bonding between the partners. OT also increased social bonding
opponents would no longer reciprocate trust, but keep the invested between fathers and their children, demonstrated in a study men-
money to themselves. Nonetheless, participants that received OT tioned above, where fathers were observer during playful interac-
perseverated in giving trust to others in the second half of the tion with toddlers [140]. In two sessions, fathers received either
experiment. This shows that OT increases trust, but can also lead placebo or OT, after which OT led to more responsive interaction
to non-adaptive levels of trust when people are treated unfairly. and less hostility towards their children, indicating increased
This ‘‘non-adaptive’’ effect of OT was also demonstrated in a parental bonding. In another study, conducted by Shamay-Tsoory
variation of the trust game: the ultimatum game. Unlike the trust et al. [175], it was importantly demonstrated how experimental
game, in the ultimatum game the participant is given an amount of manipulation can turn increased social bonding and trust, typically
MU’s and then proposes how to share this amount with a second promoted by oxytocin, into a negative counterpart: increased jeal-
player. The second player can accept the offer, or decline. In the ousy and gloating. In this experiment participants played a game
case of a decline, both players get nothing. The ultimatum game against an opponent where they received an amount of money
(UG) thus does not measure trust, because the dilemma of how based on chance. They received either more or less than their
to share the money is now strictly with the first player, who pre- opponent and then were informed of the differences. Hereafter,
sumably has to take into account the opinion of the second player they rated their feelings towards the other player. OT led to in-
knowing that unfair offers will probably be rejected, but can lead to creased jealousy when the other player received more money,
more gain if accepted. In this game, OT led to higher proposals by and increased gloating when receiving more money.
26 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

These findings suggest that increased social sensitivity by OT work. The first study which directly tested the effect of T adminis-
might strengthen cooperation and bonding when individuals are tration on responsiveness to faces showed increased heart rate
dependent on each other, but decrease social bonding and positive after T, compared to placebo, in response to angry facial expres-
regard when negative out-group attributions are emphasized, a sit- sions [201]. The angry facial expression serves as an important
uation where ‘‘dehumanization’’ is not uncommon in human threat signal in dominance encounters [147] and the increased
beings [209]. Importantly, increased in-group cooperation and heart rate caused by T in response to facial anger defensibly points
trust together with decreased cooperation and trust towards an at an enhanced predisposition to (aggressively) defend social sta-
out-group under OT administration, is exactly what is reported in tus [201].
a recent publication by De Dreu et al. [44]. In a series of economic One neuroimaging study gives general support to increased
exchange studies, they convincingly show that OT decreases coop- emotional responsiveness to directed threat after T administration,
eration with the out-group to protect in-group resources. but independent of emotion content. Van Wingen et al. [206]
From animal research it was already known that OT can lead to showed that in women T administration leads to enhanced amyg-
more affiliation as well as to more aggression, albeit OT is still gen- dala activation in response to emotional faces compared to non-
erally referred to as a pro-social hormone with only positive effects emotional stimuli. However, Hermans et al. [100] demonstrate that
on human and animal social interaction [126]. This positive bias in young women T increases activation of the amygdala, IFG, hypo-
with respect to OT hampers our understanding of its regulatory thalamus, and brainstem for angry faces compared to happy faces,
role in human behavior because aggression, envy, gloating, and regions which are all part of the rodent neural circuitry of domi-
in-group favoritism are also part of the properties OT has in human nance and reactive aggression [125,142]. Thus T strengthens neural
social behavior. It is these aspects of social behavior and the rela- responsiveness towards emotional stimuli, and even more towards
tion of these behaviors to the socio-sexual hormones which are de- stimuli with specific emotionally salient features, such as angry
scribed in the following section. faces. So far, only one neuroimaging study has investigated the role
of AVP in the perception of emotional faces. In contrast to T, AVP
4.2. Aggression and social challenge did not directly affect amygdala activation, but it did alter activa-
tion of a network involving amygdala and prefrontal brain regions
For ethical reasons one cannot investigate the effect of hor- [220]. A deactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex in the placebo
mones on aggressive behavior between humans in a controlled condition during the viewing of fearful faces was no longer appar-
experimental setting. Available data on the role of hormones in ent after AVP administration. Whether the increase in prefrontal
aggression is confined largely to correlational studies, typically brain activity by AVP, compared to placebo, is related to behavioral
using indirect measures to monitor aggression. Based on such cor- data showing increased encoding of emotional expressions by AVP
relational studies which show a link between AVP and lifetime [87], is currently not known. Another question to be addressed in
aggression, specifically in patients with antisocial personality dis- future research is how effects of T and AVP administration on the
order [39,72], and the extensive amount of animal research on brain are related, since they are neurobiologically linked [91,113].
aggression, researchers have attempted to causally link aggression Furthermore, increased neural responding to fearful and angry
to hormones in humans, especially T and AVP [133]. As said, full- faces could indicate increased motivation for flight (fear) or in-
blown aggression is difficult to measure, and researchers have creased intention to fight (anger, aggression) [196]. The data avail-
therefore sought more subtle measures indicating aggressive mo- able on the effect of T on fear processes indicates that T most likely
tives. As in most other animals, human aggression is relatively rare, increases inclinations to respond with aggression under provoca-
and human conflict is often solved without overt aggression. Inter- tion, but decreases fear. After T administration in humans, physio-
personal communication, especially by facial expressions and the logical stress responses such as the fear potentiated startle and
choice of words/prosody, plays an important role in avoiding esca- skin conductance response to aversive pictures are attenuated
lating conflict. [97,98]. Also, T reduces the attentional bias for unconsciously pre-
Responses to emotional faces have therefore often been used in sented fearful faces, a measure for selective attention to minimal
studies of human aggression and the field of social neuroendocri- danger cues [197]. These findings demonstrate that T reduces fear,
nology. It might however be unconstructive to explain the finding despite strengthening reactivity to emotionally salient informa-
of these studies in terms of aggression, because strictly speaking, tion. Reductions in fear induced by T however, concur with its pro-
aggression was not measured. In accordance with the animal liter- posed role in dealing with social challenges, as reduced fear should
ature wherein the ‘‘challenge-hypothesis’’ was put forward as a bet- enable one to better handle challenging situations.
ter explanation for the role of T in animal social behavior [212], we
argue here for a comparable interpretation of aggression-related 4.2.2. Social interaction
data gathered from humans. Scrutinizing the literature on the role A few studies have also attempted to investigate the role of AVP
of T in social behavior, Archer concluded that the literature fits well and T on interpersonal communication by measuring activity of fa-
within the framework of the challenge hypothesis, arguing that this cial muscles which are used when expressing either a happy or an-
hypothesis might very well apply to humans [9]. Here, we build on gry face. Thompson et al. used facial electromyography (EMG) in
this assumption and propose that hormones, especially T, are in hu- combination with nasal AVP administration to investigate the ef-
mans, involved in ‘‘socially challenging’’ situations that could lead fect of this hormonally modulated neuropeptide on human social
to aggression, but might also lead to increased sociality if the situ- communication [186,187]. In these studies, Thompson found in-
ation deserves a more moderated response. creased angry facial expressions towards same-sex neutral facial
expressions after AVP administration compared to placebo.
4.2.1. Social perception Although restricted to neutral faces, in line with the previous
Comparable to the studies on OT described above, and with re- observations this finding can be interpreted as a heightened
gard to the heavy dependence on facial communication in human aggressive or elevated dominant stance towards unfamiliar others.
interactions, facial pictures are often used in studies on the effect However, this finding only applied to males, as in females a com-
of T and AVP on human social challenges. Studies seeking to clarify pletely different pattern of muscle activation was apparent [187].
aggression have often explained responses towards angry faces in In contrast to the males, females showed reduced angry facial
terms of aggression, but as mentioned, these findings can just as expressions toward happy and angry faces after AVP administra-
well be explained in terms of a social-challenge conceptual frame- tion, and happier facial expressions towards neutral facial expres-
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 27

sions. The authors propose that these different findings for both intention or motivation to behave aggressively, the effect of these
sexes demonstrate an increased general stress response by AVP hormones on motivation might better account for some of the find-
which subsequently leads to different coping styles in males and ing in the studies described above.
females. An indication for an increased stress response by AVP in Several studies on the effect of T on human motivation have
males after social threat was indeed found by Ebstein et al. [62]. been performed. The effect of T on sexual motivation was demon-
Sexual differences in stress coping is proposed in a model by Taylor strated in a study by Tuiten et al. [190] in which female partici-
et al. [183]. Taylor et al. argue that under stress, males are prone to pants show increased physiological as well as subjective arousal
fight or flight behavior, while females respond by affiliating with after T administration in response to erotic visual stimuli. Further-
other members of the same sex and caring for offspring, namely more, Aarts and van Honk [1] measured subjective ratings on how
a ‘‘tend and befriend’’ response [183]. The model further suggests eager participants were to perform various kinds of (non-emo-
that both AVP and OT would be at the core of this evolutionary tional) activities, and found that T heightens the general motiva-
model for sex-specificity in response to stress. tion to perform the proposed activities. These findings concur
While this gender-specific interpretation of responding to stress with the idea of some form of increased neural or action respon-
is appealing, it does not explain why these differential effects occur siveness. Likewise, reduced averse subjective ratings towards in-
in response to neutral faces, and are absent in response to angry fant crying after T administration might reflect increased
faces. If the effect would reflect a stress response, would not an an- motivation to act (action preparation) on the crying [25].
gry face elicit more stress compared to a neutral face? Also, the T not only increases motivations to act, it also increases sensitiv-
studies report only on facial responses to same-sex stimuli. A re- ity to reward. T administration leads to more risk taking when play-
cent imaging-study administrating the AVP promoting hormone ing the IOWA gambling task [18], in which subjects can choose for
T, shows how neural responses towards faces after administration higher rewards at the risk of high punishments. Additionally, a re-
interacts with the sex of the facial stimuli [204]. In females, the cent neuroimaging study showed that T administration directly
pattern of activation towards female and male faces was shifted acts on the reward-seeking pathways during anticipation of reward
after T administration as compared to placebo, selectively increas- [96]. Thus T strengthens motivation by acting on the neural path-
ing the processing of male faces after T. Another study which ap- ways that mediate reward-seeking, which can lead to strengthened
plied EMG in combination with AVP administration tested the behavioral responses depending on the individuals’ needs and mo-
effect of both AVP and OT on facial muscle activation in trauma- tives e.g.; sexual reactivity in response to erotic visual stimuli, as
tized war-veterans [160]. In this study facial EMG was only mea- well as increased agonistic facial responses towards a threatening
sured in response to combat imagery, and the effects show face. Importantly, another recent study investigating the role of T
stronger facial muscle activity after AVP administration and re- in an economic game showed that depending on conditions, T can
duced activity after OT administration. Stronger emotional also lead to increased fairness [63]. In this study, subjects played
responding induced by AVP and reduced responding by OT during an UG after administration of T or placebo in a between subjects de-
imagined threat concurs with the aforementioned stress-reducing sign, and were afterwards asked whether they thought they had re-
properties of OT and enhanced emotional facial reactivity towards ceived T or placebo. Data showed that subjects who believed to
facial stimuli, or perhaps increases stress responding, after AVP. have received T gave lower offers to the second player, which con-
Another T administration study also using facial EMG by Her- forms to the stereotypical view of T being involved in aggressive
mans et al. [99], focused on facial mimicry, and might give a differ- behaviors leading these subjects to propose less fair offers. Crucially
ent view on some of the findings by Thompson et al. [187]. Facial however, subjects who actually received T gave higher (more fair)
mimicry refers to the innate capacity to automatically mirror facial offers compared to placebo. An explanation for increase in fairness
expression [54], and has recently received increased attention gi- by T is that higher offers in this UG subsequently led to higher rates
ven the popularity of the mirror-neurons. Mirror-neurons, located of acceptance of the offers and thus more profit. This demonstrates
in several loci in the cortex in humans and other primates, respond that although T leads to increased fairness, it might do so in the
to the performance of an action, as well as to the observation of the prospect of access to and control over resources. This shows that
same action by another individual [107]. Several authors have pro- T more likely acts on a motivational level without directly affecting
posed that this mechanism of internal mirroring promotes in- behavior, as it would be highly unlikely that T increases fair behav-
creased understanding of the intentions of others, and underlies ior at one’s own cost.
shared emotions, serving as an evolutionary foundation for mutual This might partly explain why another study, which also stud-
understanding and empathy e.g. [107,123]. Hermans et al. showed ied the effect of T in the UG, came to a different conclusion after
that T administration reduces facial mimicry in women, and there- finding lower offers in the T conditions [218]. In this study, the
by shows a potential mechanism that underlies the disruptive ef- investigators did not control for belief effects of T and also tested
fects of T on emotion recognition (see Section 4.1.2.). subjects twice, both in the T as well as in the placebo condition,
Reduced mimicry as such, might also explain the reduced facial which might have led to differential effects due to learning. How-
expressions by females during the perception of emotional faces ever, more research is necessary before definitive conclusions on
after AVP administration [187]. Instead of increased affiliation un- these issues can be reached.
der stress as proposed by Thompson et al., reduced mimicry might The findings that T leads to protected resources and economi-
account for some of the effects. cally wiser decisions in economic exchange tasks, seems opposite
Together, the findings on the effects of hormone administration to the economically unwise effects of OT, e.g.; unconditional trust
on emotional responses to faces reveal a complex mechanism of in- towards unreliable opponents [17]. In the light of higher trust in-
creased neural and physiological responses toward social challenge duced by OT, and the role OT and T play in animal social behavior,
(threatening faces), but variability between the different emotional Bos et al. predicted that T might have an opposite effect on trust.
expressions and strong sex differences, comparable to the effects The authors show that T indeed decreases trust in humans by
seen in the research on social bonding and OT. Importantly, this reducing trustworthiness ratings towards unfamiliar faces, as com-
line of research demonstrates that hormonal effects on social pared to placebo [26]. Not only does T reduce trust, they also show
behavior are not unidirectional, and that interpretation of findings that this effect is specific to people who are generally high-trusting
needs careful analysis of both individual as well as environmental towards others. Higher ratings of trustworthiness in the placebo
differences. Furthermore, based on animal data which indicate that condition were accompanied by a stronger reduction in trust fol-
AVP and T might not increase aggression per se, but rather the lowing T administration. When the group of participants was split
28 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

up into high and low-trusting groups, T strongly reduced trustwor- has been described. Based on animal neuroendocrine models of so-
thiness in the high-trusting group, but left the low-trusting group cial behavior, we now propose a model which extends to humans,
unaffected. Thus in people who are highly trustful, and therefore and which provides a framework for the literature described above.
more at risk of deceit, T adaptively reduced the level of trustwor- First, the neural underpinnings of hormonal actions are described;
thiness, possibly to protect them from potential harm [26,114]. thereafter a model is proposed on how these mechanisms translate
Taken together, the studies on the acute effects of T and AVP on to behavior. Finally, this model is discussed in relation to the cur-
human social–emotional behavior fit the animal literature, in that rent view of hormonal regulation of human social–emotional
the hormones are not implicated in the exertion of behavioral rep- behavior.
ertoires, such as directed aggression, but more by effects on the
underlying motivational stance; thereby the effects depend 5.1. Underlying neural mechanisms
strongly on environmental influence [8,80,212]. T reduces fear
and increases motivation for action towards socially rewarding Together with the evolution of steroids and neuropeptides and
stimuli and situations. Based on the few studies in which AVP is the conservation of their behavioral effects (see Section 2), it seems
administrated, no substantial conclusions can be made regarding evident that brain regions that mediate such effects have also been
the role of this hormone in human social–emotional behavior. So conserved. The evolutionarily recent neo-cortical expansions of
far, the few studies point in the direction that AVP increases emo- primate species, and especially humans, have drastically modified
tional responsiveness towards threat, perhaps in concert with T. the complexity of their behavioral repertoires [122,162], giving rise
to behavior such as complex social cognition and language, as well
as the foundations of ethics and morality [47,60]. The phylogenet-
4.3. Psychopathology
ically younger neo-cortical regions of the brain have often been as-
sumed to be primarily responsible for the emergence of ‘higher-
Based on the now abundantly demonstrated effects of OT on
order’ human behaviors, providing a rationale for distinguishing
bonding, trust, and mind-reading, and the effects of T on sexual
factors that control human and animal behaviors, thereby
arousal and fear reduction, some investigators have proposed pos-
strengthening the position of humans on some kind of a presumed
sible therapeutic effects of these hormones on psychopathology
‘scala naturae’. Neuroscience work during the past decades has
[59,153,195,198]. A promising therapeutic target for OT involves
started to challenge that view, especially at basic emotional and
the effects of OT on social bonding and social recognition, which
motivational levels, as more data became available showing that
has been the incentive to administrate OT to patients with social
the neural underpinnings of human complex behavior are often
deficiencies such as people with autism spectrum disorder (ASD).
embedded in evolutionary ancient emotional circuits [37,46,83].
Based on correlational findings, a connection of OT levels with
The recent findings on hormonal effects on complex social behav-
ASD had already been proposed [36,172]. The first experimental
ior reviewed here further strengthen the idea that human sociality
study showed that in a group of 15 ASD patients, OT administration
derives from evolutionary ancient brain and hormonal
significantly reduced repetitive behaviors, a core feature of ASD
mechanisms.
[102]. In another study on ASD patients, OT administration in-
One important neural structure in social behavior [5] among
creased memory for the emotional content of speech [101]. Also,
others, which is also an important hormonal target, is the amyg-
mind-reading (as measured with the RMET; see Section 4.1.2.) im-
dala. The amygdala is critically involved in vigilance and the detec-
proved in ASD patients after OT administration [85]. Furthermore,
tion of relevant stimuli, such as threat [43], and is targeted by both
in a group of 13 ASD patients, OT increased social learning in a dig-
OT and AVP [105]. Interestingly, OT and AVP have direct antagonis-
ital ball-throwing game [7], confirming a potential therapeutic role
tic effects on the amygdala, whereby OT inhibits and AVP excites
for OT with regard to ASD. Not only for ASD has the therapeutic po-
output to the brainstem regions such as the periaqueductal gray
tential of OT been demonstrated, a recent neuroimaging study in
[105], which serve as mechanisms for the exact opposite effects
patients with generalized social anxiety disorder (GSAD) showed
these neuropeptides have on fear responses easily monitored in
that amygdala reactivity towards fearful faces was dampened by
animal research (i.e. increased reactivity by AVP and reduced reac-
OT [128]. Moreover, this effect was confined to the patient group
tivity by OT) [49]. Furthermore, the actions of these social peptides
and was not demonstrated in the control group.
at the amygdala level are under steroidal influence. E directly pro-
Despite these promising results, effects of peptide administra-
motes activation of OT producing neurons [38], while T has compa-
tion are only short lasting (around 1.5 h), which might limit their
rable effects on AVP production and neural activities [45].
therapeutic potential. An attempt to test the effect of OT on treat-
Increased aggression in rats after AVP administration into the
ment outcome in patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD) by
amygdala has been shown to depend on T, providing evidence
Guastella et al. [86] was unsuccessful. OT administration did lead
for the interaction of these hormones at the level of the amygdala
to improvement of subjectively experienced exposure during ther-
[125]. Thus OT and AVP have opposite effects on amygdala-connec-
apy sessions, but did not generalize to treatment outcome. This
tivity with brainstem regions, either directly or by interacting with
suggests that there might be some room for optimism as there ap-
steroid hormones T and E. Besides these interactions where ste-
pears to be potential in the use of the peptide OT in treating psy-
roids affect neuropeptide synthesis, these steroids, E and T, also
chopathology, but it’s success is currently limited by its mode of
interact with each other. Aromatase, which is highly prevalent in
administration. The challenge however for pharmaceutical devel-
the human amygdala as well as in other limbic regions [20,182], al-
opment, is discovery of longer-lasting non-peptide congeners that
lows fast conversion of T–E (see Section 2). While these steroids
can lead to longer lasting effects, and also altered ways of central
can have rapid effects of their own, E and T can also differentially
administration which may lead to more enduring effects of pep-
affect neuropeptide synthesis [12,40]. Although detailed descrip-
tides on the central nervous system.
tion of these mechanisms is beyond the scope of this review (see
for elaborate reviews on peptide and steroid mechanisms:
5. A model for endocrine regulation of human social–emotional [38,40,45,129]), it is important to see how intimately such steroid
behavior neuropeptide interactions can globally shift social–emotional
responsiveness, especially through modulation of the amygdala.
In the previous section, the existing literature on the acute ef- The amygdala naturally does not stand alone; it is part of a vast
fects of steroids and peptides on human social–emotional behavior neural network involved in emotion processing, and strongly con-
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 29

nected with several other brain regions such as the thalamus, the mechanisms evolved to adaptively and flexibly adjust behavior to
PVN of the hypothalamus, the septum and the medial periaqu- the environment [161]. In challenging environments wherein an
eductal regions of the brainstem. It is also strongly connected with individuals are exposed to social and other threats, T will reduce
the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), the ventral part of the prefrontal fear and strengthen the motivation to act by activating the sympa-
cortex involved in emotional evaluation and with strong modulat- thetic nervous system, and promote assertive, ‘‘courageous’’ act-
ing influence on the amygdala [11,127] which allows higher men- ing-out behaviors, especially toward these threats. T acts in
tal processes to regulate emotionality. The OFC is also connected to concert with AVP, which by shifting away from complex stimulus
several other regions involved in more complex forms of emotion processing enables better coping with threat. In a safe environ-
processing such as the anterior cingulate gyrus and the superior ment, E and OT shift the balance towards parasympathetic activa-
temporal gyrus [79,174], regions which are implicated in trustwor- tion, initiating reduced stress and more local, domestic and
thiness assessment, as well as empathizing and mind-reading friendly activities. By inhibiting amygdala output to brainstem re-
[93,176,214]. gions, E and OT give rise to more cognitive control by prefrontal
Within this interactive cognitive-affective network, hormones neural regions, thereby facilitating social bonding through the di-
can, by directly modulating amygdala activity, shift neural output rect effects of OT on dopamine reward-seeking systems during so-
towards other brain regions. Exactly this has been found in neuro- cial interaction. After stress, OT will facilitate reduction of anxiety
imaging studies investigating the effects of T and OT on emotion- and the HPA stress response to reestablish homeostasis. Indeed, a
processing. Kirsch et al. [124] found reduced connectivity between study investigating the acute effect of E on heart rate found evi-
the amygdala and the brainstem after OT administration, in con- dence for increased parasympathetic tone after E administration
trast to van Wingen et al. [205] who showed reduced connectivity [115]. Together with the well-documented soothing and stress-
between amygdala and OFC after T administration, accompanied reducing effects of OT, and the above described finding of fear
by increased connectivity with thalamic regions. Increased connec- reduction by T, this model provides a framework for the role of ste-
tivity with thalamic regions might very well be caused by T acting roids and neuropeptides in human social behavior.
on AVP, and can be seen to be congruent with a T administration
study which showed increased activation of the brainstem and 5.2. On how these mechanisms translate to behavioral studies
hypothalamus [100]. This exact mechanism of T acting on AVP
needs further scrutiny, as the only neuroimaging paper so far on The present review of literature on hormonal and neuropeptide
AVP affects on the human brain did not show a direct effect on regulation of human social behavior has mainly focused on T and
the amygdala, but alterations in prefrontal regions connected to OT. Only seven studies are currently available which investigated
the amygdala [220]. Nonetheless, another indication for the pro- the role of acute AVP administration on human social–emotional
posed shift induced by T comes from an electro-encephalography behavior, and only two studies targeted the role of E (Table 1). Of
(EEG) study, which shows that T decreases the coupling between the two studies on E, one investigated a very basic component of
oscillations arising from cortical and sub-cortical neural regions the CNS, while the other study only looked for effects on mood
[173]. An EEG study with OT administration on the other hand, using a questionnaire [170]. When attempting to measure the sub-
showed changes in frequency bands suggesting increased activa- tle effects of hormones on social behavior by self-report measure, it
tion of mirror-neuron circuits during perception of biological mo- may not be surprising that effects are meager as hormones may tap
tion [156]. This increase in mirror neural circuits might explain into implicitly driven aspects of behavior that cannot easily be cap-
increased detection of biological motion, as reported earlier [119]. tured by self-reporting. Thus, compelling evidence for acute effects
The model (Fig. 1) by which the neuropeptides and steroids can of hormone E on human social behavior is currently lacking, and
modulate social behavior may reflect how the underlying brain the currently available data on AVP is also not yet convincing. This
has however, more to do with the quantity of the data instead of
the results that were obtained; more studies are needed to estab-
lish any concrete results. In contrast to E and AVP, there is consid-
erable amount of data showing effects of T and OT on human social
behavior (Table 1). These findings show that the adaptive endo-
crine mechanisms evolved millions of years ago, and still exert ef-
fects in today’s ‘‘highest’’ species.
STS Studies on the effects of hormones on behavior, in humans as
well as in other animals, mostly follow the logic of first administer-
ing the hormone and then monitoring effects on subsequent
Tha
Sep behavior. Unfortunately, these studies often do not (perhaps can-
ACC not) adequately consider the role of the (social) environment
OFC wherein for the past millions of years these social hormones came
to regulate diverse behaviors. We know from animal research that
hormone surges often occur depending on the social contexts
A
Bst
wherein animals are situated, and that these surges can be strongly
PVN modulated by the environmental differences (e.g.; season of the
OT/E year in annual breeders) and by animal characteristics (e.g.; gre-
garious versus competitive animals, dominance status, previous
T/AVP wins and losses in fights) [8,80,82]. This strong environmental
interaction demonstrates that the effects of these hormones are
Fig. 1. Neural model for the effects of steroids and neuropeptides on brain not simply unidirectional, because some hormones can serve dif-
connectivity. By acting on the amygdala, T and OT can shift output patterns towards ferent purposes in different environments, whereby slight differ-
the brainstem or prefrontal brain regions respectively. T does so by acting on AVP, ences in context can yield different behavioral effects. Reviewing
OT possible in concert with E. (A = amygdala; Tha = thalamus; ACC = anterior
cingulate cortex; Sep = septum; PVN = paraventricular nucleus; Bst = brainstem;
the literature on humans, this dynamic relation between hormonal
OFC = orbitofrontal cortex; STS = Superior Temporal Sulcus; OT/E = Oxytocin/Estro- effects and social contexts seems to apply to humans as well. Thus
gen; T/AVP = Testosterone/Arginine Vasopressin). for the studies on E, OT, T, and AVP, we need to take into account in
30 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

which social environments these steroids and neuropeptides most on the social environment and perhaps less on the subjective influ-
likely served their functions in behavior. From this evolutionary ences of individual self-serving motives, as it increases bonding
perspective, and based on the model described above (Section 5.1), behaviors, concentrated rather strictly in the proximate social
the findings on humans remain to be embedded in dynamic frame- environment. This is reflected in experiments showing increased
works that can yield differential predictions depending on environ- trust [126], partner bonding [55], cognitive and affective empathy
mental context. [57,106], and also in findings which show increased in-group
In our estimation, T and AVP are important in environments favoritism [44], as well as jealousy and gloating [175] in an envi-
that demand action, and especially when the individual is con- ronment in which competition is emphasized.
fronted with social challenges. T surges will adaptively increase Phylogenetically, meta-cognitive capacities such as mind-read-
vigilance and the motivation to act, either by the effect of T itself ing and conscious emotion detection lie a step beyond social recog-
or by acting on AVP. T does so by reducing fear and initiate ap- nition and social bonding. On these social cognitive behaviors, T
proach motivation through acting on reward-seeking pathways and OT demonstrate opposite effects whereby OT facilitates
in the presence of desired goals, irrespective of whether the goal mind-reading and emotion recognition, and T inhibits these capac-
is aggression (dominance), sex, or monetary gain. The function of ities. These opposite effects of T and OT seem to fit with the shift
AVP when confronted with challenge might very well be shifting these hormones induce at the neural level [105,124,205], leading
towards less complex stimulus processing, thereby increasing the to more self-serving affective reactivity by T, and more to social
focus to deal with threat. In contrast, E and OT function more dis- affective-cognitive behaviors by OT.
tinctly in safe environments where there is no direct need for vig- The model as presented in Fig. 2 integrates all the literature on
orous action. But when dealing with threat, OT has an important the acute effects of the steroids and peptides E, T, AVP and OT on
function to reinstate homeostasis. E and OT enhance parasympa- human social–emotional behavior, and can serve as a framework
thetic nervous system activity, promoting calming behaviors and for future research on the neuroendocrinology of human sociality.
increased provision of care, especially for infants. In primate evolu- Thus, it needs to be emphasized that the model as described here is
tion, with the shift towards cooperative breeding in human ances- incomplete by definition as it is restricted to the aforementioned
try, this increased caring response extended to family and group hormones, and does not include many other hormones and pep-
members [103,104]. OT also reduces fear, and facilitates social tides that might be relevant for an overall understanding of so-
bonding by increasing social proximity. cial–emotional behavior. Also, the scarce literature on E and AVP
A shift toward more social-cognitive neural networks in certain limits the scope of the model. Nonetheless, it can contribute as a
species, gives rise to increased empathy and higher-order social starting point for future research and an invocation to generate
cognition. The other side of this emphasis on increased care and more complex and inclusive hypotheses. The model builds upon
bonding with infants or in-group members is the increased empha- the current literature by seeking to incorporate an important role
sis this shift brings to how one responds distinctly to in- and out- for environmental interaction with hormone administration ef-
groups. It can bring forth more aggression towards intruders in ro- fects. Future research should directly test the environmental mod-
dent mothers [27], but also more in-group favoritism [44], as well ulations of the effect of hormones on human social behavior in
as increased jealousy and gloating in humans [175]. The differ- experimental settings to validate these proposed dynamic rela-
ences between the sexes with respect to hormone manipulations tions, as well as related ideas of autonomic balances and imbal-
can also be illuminated by this framework, in line with the tend- ances that result from social states [161,172].
and-befriend hypothesis for the female stress response [183]. The
dominant role of females in providing care for infants and securing
their survival has left its trace in the neuroendocrinology of social 5.3. Correct translation, lessons for the future
behavior. Although in the case of extreme threat, females as well as
males are inclined to fight or flight, the female stress response also Translational research, which in the past decades attempted to
carries a strong caring component, wherein stress can lead to in- validate neuroendocrine models based on animal literature in hu-
creased bonding and affiliation among group members, a response mans, has often yielded inconclusive data. Many hormone admin-
which is less evident in males. Perhaps this sexual diversity in istration studies testing the acute effects of OT and AVP were
stress coping can help explain the differences in male and female performed before the emergence of affective-social neuroscience,
brain responses to OT administration described above. and most attempted to investigate the effect on memory process-
Although both OT and T can reduce fear in humans [98,124], ing, with rather weak or no effects [48,69].
they seem to do so via different mechanisms: T and AVP promote In hindsight, these findings might not be surprising, as these
the ability to detect and cope with threat, whereas OT promotes peptides exert their functions in neural regions of the brain in-
the search for proximity and friendly interaction with others. This volved in emotional and affective processing, not in declarative
may help explain the findings that both T and OT increase atten- memory formation [129]. From these studies, as well as others that
tion towards faces [78,100]. Increased attention in the case of T left out the motivational and emotional aspects on which these
is selective to angry faces, for these signal threat. With high OT, hormones act, it had been concluded or assumed that human
such a selective bias does not seem to apply, as OT more generally behavior is no longer under hormonal control, but that evolution
increases attention towards social stimuli, and when confronted had led the human brain to become emancipated from hor-
with faces this translates to paying more attention to the eye re- monal-affective controls to more cognitively driven motives
gions of the face [89]. [41,122]. Although it is certainly true that we are not under the
During social interactions, effects of the steroids and peptides sway of some kind of hormonal dictatorship, largely dependent
no doubt increase in complexity as the complexity of environ- on hormonal levels as some simpler organisms are, based on the
ments and subjects’ ideas about their environments start to play current review we conclude that human behavior still has substan-
a more substantial role [63]. Overall, T, perhaps by AVP, increases tial and important endocrine control components. Although the
the motivation for actions which hold rewarding properties for neural substrates of reactive emotional behavior humans share
the individual. This can lead to more risk taking [200], reduced with other mammals [152] are, in humans, under enhanced top-
empathy [99], as well as to increased fairness if this fairness leads down cortical control, the studies reviewed here nonetheless show
to bigger gains [63], contextualized, no doubt, by the subjective that our ancestral hormones still have a firm grip on human mind
motives of the individual. OT promotes a greater directed focus and behavior.
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 31

OT/E
T/AVP
▼ fear (T) fear ▼

▲ sensitivity for reward homeostasis ▲

Threat ▼ punishment sensitivity social bonding ▲ Social


& Reward Ingroup
▼ complex processing (AVP) social cognition ▲

▼social cognition ingroup favourism ▲

Fig. 2. Heuristic model for the effects of steroids and peptides on human social–emotional behavior. T and AVP are important in environments that demand action, increasing
vigilance and the motivation to act. In contrast, E and OT function more distinctly in safe environments where there is no direct need for vigorous action. E and OT enhance
parasympathetic nervous system activity, promoting calming behaviors and increased provision of care, especially for infants.

Increased communication between cortical and sub-cortical behaviors, and how these mechanisms interact with the environ-
neural structures by the prefrontal expansion in primates has in- ment, remains a big challenge. Our proposed model is simply an at-
duced a shift towards increased behavioral flexibility compared tempt to provide a heuristic framework upon which new
to more emotional reflexive behavior [5,60]. However, as demon- hypotheses can be formed and tested. In our view, the acknowledg-
strated by imaging studies on humans, these cortical and sub-cor- ment that evolutionary ancient biology still underlies human social
tical shifts are hormonally modulated (see Section 5.1.). It is behavior, is of pivotal importance for how we, humans, see our-
exactly these shifts in the patterning of neural activation, which selves in relation to ourselves and our fellow animals [19].
underlie the effects on hormones in human social behavior.
When judging the effects of acute hormone administration, one Acknowledgments
has to bear in mind that the methods of application in the case of
humans are also a major limitation when compared to animal re- The work in this paper was supported by a Utrecht University
search. Modest effects on human behavior after applying hormonal (High-Potential Grant), the Hope for Depression Research Founda-
nasal spray might not be surprising. In animal studies wherein sub- tion (HDRF: RGA #9-015), and the Netherlands Society of Scientific
stances are directly injected in the brain (e.g.; intra-cerebroventric- Research (Brain and Cognition #056-24-010) to JvH. We thank Si-
ular injection; ICV), behavioral effects are typically more robust. mon Reader for constructive feedback on the manuscript and Den-
The aim for the following decades thus, should be to develop meth- nis Hofman for help with figure preparation.
odologies and build models which can increase our understanding
of the biology underlying all social–emotional behaviors, while
References
simultaneously rendering justice to the complexity of these very
same biological underpinnings [151]. [1] H. Aarts, J. van Honk, Testosterone and unconscious positive priming increase
human motivation separately, Neuroreport 20 (2009) 1300–1303.
[2] R. Acher, Molecular evolution of biologically active polypeptides, Proc. Roy.
Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 210 (1980) 21–43.
6. Conclusion [3] R. Acher, J. Chauvet, The neurohypophysial endocrine regulatory cascade:
precursors, mediators, receptors, and effectors, Front. Neuroendocrinol. 16
Direct translations from selected biological and behavioral (1995) 237–289.
[4] R.B. Adams, N.O. Rule, R.G. Franklin, E. Wang, M.T. Stevenson, S. Yoshikawa,
markers, easily quantified, to brain networks that control complex
M. Nomura, W. Sato, K. Kveraga, N. Ambady, Cross-cultural reading the mind
processes such as social bonding or aggression that can barely be in the eyes: an fMRI investigation, J. Cogn. Neurosci. (2009) 97–108.
studied with current technologies, can lead to misleading holistic [5] R. Adolphs, The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge, Annu. Rev.
ideas, comparable to those that Hippocrates advanced more than Psychol. 60 (2009) 693–716.
[6] G.A. Alvares, I.B. Hickie, A.J. Guastella, Acute effects of intranasal oxytocin on
2000 years ago, in which he deemed the levels of bodily humors subjective and behavioral responses to social rejection, Exp. Clin.
directly responsible for diverse human behavior or illness. We can- Psychopharmacol. 18 (2010) 316–321.
not yet do justice to the true neural complexities that underlie men- [7] E. Andari, J.R. Duhamel, T. Zalla, E. Herbrecht, M. Leboyer, A. Sirigu, Promoting
social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders,
tal and behavioral functions of the brain-body continuum of Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA (2010) 4389–4394.
individual animals, not to mention those that arise from inter-group [8] R.A. Antunes, R.F. Oliveira, Hormonal anticipation of territorial challenges in
dynamics. Nonetheless, an increasing number of studies on the hor- cichlid fish, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106 (2009) 15985–15989.
[9] J. Archer, Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge
monal regulation of human social behavior do show that steroids hypothesis, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30 (2006) 319–345.
and neuropeptides are causally implicated in human behavior. [10] A.P. Arnold, R.A. Gorski, Gonadal steroid induction of structural sex
Behaviors that were formerly thought of as largely cognitively med- differences in the central nervous system, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 7 (1984)
413–442.
iated and free from endocrine underpinnings, are proving to be not [11] J. Bachevalier, K.A. Loveland, The orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit and self-
so. However in humans, such hormonal regulations are far from regulation of social–emotional behavior in autism, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
reflexive or automatic, as the effects strongly interact with individ- 30 (2006) 97–117.
[12] J. Balthazart, G.F. Ball, Is brain estradiol a hormone or a neurotransmitter?,
uals’ environments and subjective states, and interpersonal dynam-
Trends Neurosci 29 (2006) 241–249.
ics. Thus human social and emotional behaviors are not under strict [13] J. Balthazart, M. Taziaux, K. Holloway, G.F. Ball, C.A. Cornil, Behavioral effects
hormonal constraints [122], but the recent studies summarized of brain-derived estrogens in birds, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1163 (2009) 31–48.
here do demonstrate that human behavior is importantly influ- [14] S. Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, J. Hill, Y. Raste, I. Plumb, The ‘‘reading the
mind in the eyes’’ test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults
enced by steroid and neuropeptide systems. Disentangling how with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, J. Child Psychol.
these, and other, neuroendocrine mechanisms influence social Psychiatry 42 (2001) 241–251.
32 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

[15] A. Bartels, S. Zeki, The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love, [49] J. Debiec, Peptides of love and fear: vasopressin and oxytocin modulate the
Neuroimage 21 (2004) 1155–1166. integration of information in the amygdala, Bioessays 27 (2005) 869–873.
[16] J.A. Bartz, J. Zaki, N. Bolger, E. Hollander, N.N. Ludwig, A. Kolevzon, K.N. [50] C.H. Declerck, C. Boone, T. Kiyonari, Oxytocin and cooperation under
Ochsner, Oxytocin selectively improves empathic accuracy, Psychol. Sci. 21 conditions of uncertainty: the modulating role of incentives and social
(2010) 1426–1428. information, Horm. Behav. 57 (2010) 368–374.
[17] T. Baumgartner, M. Heinrichs, A. Vonlanthen, U. Fischbacher, E. Fehr, [51] Y. Delville, K.M. Mansour, C.F. Ferris, Testosterone facilitates aggression by
Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in modulating vasopressin receptors in the hypothalamus, Physiol. Behav. 60
humans, Neuron 58 (2008) 639–650. (1996) 25–29.
[18] A. Bechara, H. Damasio, D. Tranel, A.R. Damasio, Deciding advantageously [52] A.C. DeVries, W.S. Young 3rd, R.J. Nelson, Reduced aggressive behaviour in
before knowing the advantageous strategy, Science 275 (1997) 1293–1295. mice with targeted disruption of the oxytocin gene, J. Neuroendocrinol. 9
[19] M. Bekoff, Minding Animals: Awareness, Emotions, and Heart, Oxford (1997) 363–368.
University Press, New York, 2002. [53] M. Di Simplicio, R. Massey-Chase, P.J. Cowen, C.J. Harmer, Oxytocin enhances
[20] A. Biegon, S.W. Kim, D.L. Alexoff, M. Jayne, P. Carter, B. Hubbard, P. King, J. processing of positive versus negative emotional information in healthy male
Logan, L. Muench, D. Pareto, D. Schlyer, C. Shea, F. Telang, G.J. Wang, Y. Xu, J.S. volunteers, J. Psychopharmacol. 23 (2009) 241–248.
Fowler, Unique distribution of aromatase in the human brain: in vivo studies [54] U. Dimberg, M. Thunberg, K. Elmehed, Unconscious facial reactions to
with PET and [N-methyl-11C]vorozole, Synapse 64 (2010) 801–807. emotional facial expressions, Psychol. Sci. 11 (2000) 86–89.
[21] I.F. Bielsky, L.J. Young, Oxytocin, vasopressin, and social recognition in [55] B. Ditzen, M. Schaer, B. Gabriel, G. Bodenmann, U. Ehlert, M. Heinrichs,
mammals, Peptides 25 (2004) 1565–1574. Intranasal oxytocin increases positive communication and reduces cortisol
[22] A. Blume, O.J. Bosch, S. Miklos, L. Torner, L. Wales, M. Waldherr, I.D. levels during couple conflict, Biol. Psychiatry 65 (2009) 728–731.
Neumann, Oxytocin reduces anxiety via ERK1/2 activation: local effect [56] G. Domes, M. Heinrichs, J. Glascher, C. Buchel, D.F. Braus, S.C. Herpertz,
within the rat hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus, Eur. J. Neurosci. 27 Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of
(2008) 1947–1956. valence, Biol. Psychiatry 62 (2007) 1187–1190.
[23] R.M. Bluthe, J. Schoenen, R. Dantzer, Androgen-dependent vasopressinergic [57] G. Domes, M. Heinrichs, A. Michel, C. Berger, S.C. Herpertz, Oxytocin improves
neurons are involved in social recognition in rats, Brain Res. 519 (1990) 150– ‘‘mind-reading’’ in humans, Biol. Psychiatry 61 (2007) 731–733.
157. [58] G. Domes, A. Lischke, C. Berger, A. Grossmann, K. Hauenstein, M. Heinrichs,
[24] J. Born, T. Lange, W. Kern, G.P. McGregor, U. Bickel, H.L. Fehm, Sniffing S.C. Herpertz, Effects of intranasal oxytocin on emotional face processing in
neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain, Nat. Neurosci. 5 women, Psychoneuroendocrinology 35 (2010) 83–93.
(2002) 514–516. [59] Z.R. Donaldson, L.J. Young, Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of
[25] P.A. Bos, E.J. Hermans, E.R. Montoya, N.F. Ramsey, J. van Honk, Testosterone sociality, Science 322 (2008) 900–904.
administration modulates neural responses to crying infants in young [60] R.I. Dunbar, S. Shultz, Evolution in the social brain, Science 317 (2007) 1344–
females, Psychoneuroendocrinology 35 (2010) 114–121. 1347.
[26] P.A. Bos, D. Terburg, J. van Honk, Testosterone decreases trust in socially naive [61] K. Ebner, O.J. Bosch, S.A. Kromer, N. Singewald, I.D. Neumann, Release of
humans, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107 (2010) 9991–9995. oxytocin in the rat central amygdala modulates stress-coping behavior and
[27] O.J. Bosch, S.L. Meddle, D.I. Beiderbeck, A.J. Douglas, I.D. Neumann, Brain the release of excitatory amino acids, Neuropsychopharmacology 30 (2005)
oxytocin correlates with maternal aggression: link to anxiety, J. Neurosci. 25 223–230.
(2005) 6807–6815. [62] R.P. Ebstein, S. Israel, E. Lerer, F. Uzefovsky, I. Shalev, I. Gritsenko, M. Riebold,
[28] O.J. Bosch, I.D. Neumann, Brain vasopressin is an important regulator of S. Salomon, N. Yirmiya, Arginine vasopressin and oxytocin modulate human
maternal behavior independent of dams’ trait anxiety, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. social behavior, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1167 (2009) 87–102.
USA 105 (2008) 17139–17144. [63] C. Eisenegger, M. Naef, R. Snozzi, M. Heinrichs, E. Fehr, Prejudice and truth
[29] O.J. Bosch, I.D. Neumann, Vasopressin released within the central amygdala about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour, Nature 463
promotes maternal aggression, Eur. J. Neurosci. 31 (2010) 883–891. (2010) 356–359.
[30] M.B. Bryan, A.P. Scott, W. Li, Sex steroids and their receptors in lampreys, [64] M. Engelmann, Vasopressin in the septum: not important versus causally
Steroids 73 (2008) 1–12. involved in learning and memory – two faces of the same coin?, Prog Brain
[31] A. Burri, M. Heinrichs, M. Schedlowski, T.H. Kruger, The acute effects of Res. 170 (2008) 389–395.
intranasal oxytocin administration on endocrine and sexual function in [65] M. Engelmann, R. Landgraf, C.T. Wotjak, The hypothalamic-neurohypophysial
males, Psychoneuroendocrinology 33 (2008) 591–600. system regulates the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis under stress: an
[32] H.K. Caldwell, H.J. Lee, A.H. Macbeth, W.S. Young III, Vasopressin: behavioral old concept revisited, Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25 (2004) 132–149.
roles of an ‘‘original’’ neuropeptide, Prog. Neurobiol. 84 (2008) 1–24. [66] M. Engelmann, C.T. Wotjak, K. Ebner, R. Landgraf, Behavioural impact of
[33] H.K. Caldwell, W.S. Young III, Oxytocin and vasopressin: genetics and intraseptally released vasopressin and oxytocin in rats, Exp. Physiol. 85 Spec
behavioral implications, in: R. Lim (Ed.), Handbook of Neurochemistry and No (2000) 125S–130S.
Molecular Neurobiology, Springer, New York, 2006, pp. 573–607. [67] S. Evans, S.S. Shergill, B.B. Averbeck, Oxytocin decreases aversion to angry
[34] A. Campbell, Attachment, aggression and affiliation: the role of oxytocin in faces in an associative learning task, Neuropsychopharmacology 35 (2010)
female social behavior, Biol. Psychol. 77 (2008) 1–10. 2502–2509.
[35] C.S. Carter, Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love, [68] M. Febo, M. Numan, C.F. Ferris, Functional magnetic resonance imaging
Psychoneuroendocrinology 23 (1998) 779–818. shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother–pup bonding
[36] C.S. Carter, Sex differences in oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for during suckling, J. Neurosci. 25 (2005) 11637–11644.
autism spectrum disorders?, Behav Brain Res. 176 (2007) 170–186. [69] G. Fehm-Wolfsdorf, J. Born, Behavioral effects of neurohypophyseal
[37] H.A. Chapman, D.A. Kim, J.M. Susskind, A.K. Anderson, In bad taste: evidence peptides in healthy volunteers: 10 years of research, Peptides 12 (1991)
for the oral origins of moral disgust, Science 323 (2009) 1222–1226. 1399–1406.
[38] E. Choleris, N. Devidze, M. Kavaliers, D.W. Pfaff, Steroidal/neuropeptide [70] R. Feldman, A. Weller, O. Zagoory-Sharon, A. Levine, Evidence for a
interactions in hypothalamus and amygdala related to social anxiety, Prog. neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: plasma oxytocin
Brain Res. 170 (2008) 291–303. levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother–infant
[39] E.F. Coccaro, R.J. Kavoussi, R.L. Hauger, T.B. Cooper, C.F. Ferris, Cerebrospinal bonding, Psychol. Sci. 18 (2007) 965–970.
fluid vasopressin levels: correlates with aggression and serotonin function in [71] C.F. Ferris, R.H. Melloni Jr., G. Koppel, K.W. Perry, R.W. Fuller, Y. Delville,
personality-disordered subjects, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55 (1998) 708–714. Vasopressin/serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control
[40] C.A. Cornil, G.F. Ball, J. Balthazart, Functional significance of the rapid aggressive behavior in golden hamsters, J. Neurosci. 17 (1997) 4331–4340.
regulation of brain estrogen action: where do the estrogens come from?, [72] S.O. Fetissov, J. Hallman, I. Nilsson, A.K. Lefvert, L. Oreland, T. Hokfelt,
Brain Res 1126 (2006) 2–26. Aggressive behavior linked to corticotropin-reactive autoantibodies, Biol.
[41] J.P. Curley, E.B. Keverne, Genes, brains and mammalian social bonds, Trends Psychiatry 60 (2006) 799–802.
Ecol. Evol. 20 (2005) 561–567. [73] M. Fischer-Shofty, S.G. Shamay-Tsoory, H. Harari, Y. Levkovitz, The effect of
[42] R. Dantzer, G.F. Koob, R.M. Bluthe, M. Le Moal, Septal vasopressin modulates intranasal administration of oxytocin on fear recognition, Neuropsychologia
social memory in male rats, Brain Res. 457 (1988) 143–147. 48 (2010) 179–184.
[43] M. Davis, P.J. Whalen, The amygdala: vigilance and emotion, Mol. Psychiatry 6 [74] A.S. Fleming, C. Corter, J. Stallings, M. Steiner, Testosterone and prolactin are
(2001) 13–34. associated with emotional responses to infant cries in new fathers, Horm.
[44] C.K. De Dreu, L.L. Greer, M.J. Handgraaf, S. Shalvi, G.A. Van Kleef, M. Baas, F.S. Behav. 42 (2002) 399–413.
Ten Velden, E. Van Dijk, S.W. Feith, The neuropeptide oxytocin regulates [75] A.S. Fleming, D. Ruble, H. Krieger, P.Y. Wong, Hormonal and experiential
parochial altruism in intergroup conflict among humans, Science 328 (2010) correlates of maternal responsiveness during pregnancy and the puerperium
1408–1411. in human mothers, Horm. Behav. 31 (1997) 145–158.
[45] G.J. de Vries, Sex differences in vasopressin and oxytocin innervation of the [76] A.B. Fries, T.E. Ziegler, J.R. Kurian, S. Jacoris, S.D. Pollak, Early experience in
brain, Prog. Brain Res. 170 (2008) 17–27. humans is associated with changes in neuropeptides critical for regulating
[46] F.B. de Waal, Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of social behavior, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102 (2005) 17237–17240.
empathy, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 (2008) 279–300. [77] M. Gamer, C. Buchel, Amygdala activation predicts gaze toward fearful eyes, J.
[47] F.B.M. de Waal, Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans Neurosci. 29 (2009) 9123–9126.
and other Animals, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, 1996. [78] M. Gamer, B. Zurowski, C. Buchel, Different amygdala subregions mediate
[48] D. de Wied, Neuropeptides in learning and memory processes, Behav. Brain valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans, Proc. Natl.
Res. 83 (1997) 83–90. Acad. Sci. USA 107 (2010) 9400–9405.
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 33

[79] H.T. Ghashghaei, H. Barbas, Pathways for emotion: interactions of prefrontal [108] T.R. Insel, The neurobiology of affiliation: implications for autism, in:
and anterior temporal pathways in the amygdala of the rhesus monkey, K.R.S.R.J. Davidson, H.H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences,
Neuroscience 115 (2002) 1261–1279. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2003, pp. 1010–1020.
[80] E.D. Gleason, M.J. Fuxjager, T.O. Oyegbile, C.A. Marler, Testosterone release [109] T.R. Insel, The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of
and social context: when it occurs and why, Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30 oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior, Neuron 65 (2010) 768–779.
(2009) 460–469. [110] T.R. Insel, L.J. Young, Neuropeptides and the evolution of social behavior,
[81] J.L. Goodson, A.H. Bass, Social behavior functions and related anatomical Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 10 (2000) 784–789.
characteristics of vasotocin/vasopressin systems in vertebrates, Brain Res. [111] T.R. Insel, L.J. Young, The neurobiology of attachment, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2
Brain Res. Rev. 35 (2001) 246–265. (2001) 129–136.
[82] J.L. Goodson, D. Kabelik, Dynamic limbic networks and social diversity in [112] A.E. Johnson, The regulation of oxytocin receptor-binding in the ventromedial
vertebrates: from neural context to neuromodulatory patterning, Front. hypothalamic nucleus by gonadal-steroids, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 652 (1992)
Neuroendocrinol. 30 (2009) 429–441. 357–373.
[83] J.D. Greene, L.E. Nystrom, A.D. Engell, J.M. Darley, J.D. Cohen, The neural bases [113] A.E. Johnson, C. Barberis, H.E. Albers, Castration reduces vasopressin
of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment, Neuron 44 (2004) 389– receptor-binding in the hamster hypothalamus, Brain Res. 674 (1995) 153–
400. 158.
[84] A.J. Guastella, D.S. Carson, M.R. Dadds, P.B. Mitchell, R.E. Cox, Does oxytocin [114] R.T. Johnson, S.M. Breedlove, Human trust: testosterone raises suspicion,
influence the early detection of angry and happy faces?, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107 (2010) 11149–11150.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 34 (2009) 220–225 [115] D. Kaya, S. Cevrioglu, E. Onrat, I.V. Fenkci, M. Yilmazer, Single dose nasal
[85] A.J. Guastella, S.L. Einfeld, K.M. Gray, N.J. Rinehart, B.J. Tonge, T.J. Lambert, I.B. 17beta-estradiol administration reduces sympathovagal balance to the heart
Hickie, Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with in postmenopausal women, J. Obstet. Gynaecol. Res. 29 (2003) 406–411.
autism spectrum disorders, Biol. Psychiatry 67 (2009) 692–694. [116] K.M. Kendrick, Oxytocin, motherhood and bonding, Exp. Physiol. 85 Spec No
[86] A.J. Guastella, A.L. Howard, M.R. Dadds, P. Mitchell, D.S. Carson, A randomized (2000) 111S–124S.
controlled trial of intranasal oxytocin as an adjunct to exposure therapy for [117] K.M. Kendrick, E.B. Keverne, C. Chapman, B.A. Baldwin, Microdialysis
social anxiety disorder, Psychoneuroendocrinology 34 (2009) 917–923. measurement of oxytocin, aspartate, [gamma]-aminobutyric acid and
[87] A.J. Guastella, A.R. Kenyon, G.A. Alvares, D.S. Carson, I.B. Hickie, Intranasal glutamate release from the olfactory bulb of the sheep during
arginine vasopressin enhances the encoding of happy and angry faces in vaginocervical stimulation, Brain Res. 442 (1988) 171–174.
humans, Biol. Psychiatry 67 (2010) 1220–1222. [118] K.M. Kendrick, E.B. Keverne, M.R. Hinton, J.A. Goode, Oxytocin, amino acid
[88] A.J. Guastella, A.R. Kenyon, C. Unkelbach, G.A. Alvares, I.B. Hickie, Arginine and monoamine release in the region of the medial preoptic area and bed
vasopressin selectively enhances recognition of sexual cues in male humans, nucleus of the stria terminalis of the sheep during parturition and suckling,
Psychoneuroendocrinology (2010) (Epub ahead of print). Brain Res. 569 (1992) 199–209.
[89] A.J. Guastella, P.B. Mitchell, M.R. Dadds, Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye [119] S. Keri, G. Benedek, Oxytocin enhances the perception of biological motion in
region of human faces, Biol. Psychiatry 63 (2008) 3–5. humans, Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 9 (2009) 237–241.
[90] A.J. Guastella, P.B. Mitchell, F. Mathews, Oxytocin enhances the encoding of [120] E.B. Keverne, J.P. Curley, Vasopressin, oxytocin and social behaviour, Curr.
positive social memories in humans, Biol. Psychiatry (2008). Opin. Neurobiol. 14 (2004) 777–783.
[91] T.M. Han, G.J. De Vries, Organizational effects of testosterone, estradiol, and [121] E.B. Keverne, F. Levy, P. Poindron, D.R. Lindsay, Vaginal stimulation: an
dihydrotestosterone on vasopressin mRNA expression in the bed nucleus of important determinant of maternal bonding in sheep, Science 219 (1983) 81–
the stria terminalis, J. Neurobiol. 54 (2003) 502–510. 83.
[92] C. Heim, L.J. Young, D.J. Newport, T. Mletzko, A.H. Miller, C.B. Nemeroff, Lower [122] E.B. Keverne, F.L. Martel, C.M. Nevison, Primate brain evolution: genetic and
CSF oxytocin concentrations in women with a history of childhood abuse, functional considerations, Proc. Biol. Sci. 263 (1996) 689–696.
Mol. Psychiatry 14 (2009) 954–958. [123] C. Keysers, V. Gazzola, Towards a unifying neural theory of social cognition,
[93] G. Hein, R.T. Knight, Superior temporal sulcus – it’s my area: or is it?, J Cogn. Prog. Brain Res. 156 (2006) 379–401.
Neurosci. 20 (2008) 2125–2136. [124] P. Kirsch, C. Esslinger, Q. Chen, D. Mier, S. Lis, S. Siddhanti, H. Gruppe, V.S.
[94] C.A. Heinlein, C. Chang, The roles of androgen receptors and androgen- Mattay, B. Gallhofer, A. Meyer-Lindenberg, Oxytocin modulates neural
binding proteins in nongenomic androgen actions, Mol. Endocrinol. 16 (2002) circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans, J. Neurosci. 25 (2005)
2181–2187. 11489–11493.
[95] M. Heinrichs, T. Baumgartner, C. Kirschbaum, U. Ehlert, Social support and [125] J.M. Koolhaas, T.H.C. Van Den Brink, B. Roozendaal, F. Boorsma, Medial
oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to amygdala and aggressive behavior: interaction between testosterone and
psychosocial stress, Biol. Psychiatry 54 (2003) 1389–1398. vasopressin, Aggressive Behav. 16 (1990) 223–229.
[96] E.J. Hermans, P.A. Bos, L. Ossewaarde, N.F. Ramsey, G. Fernandez, J. van Honk, [126] M. Kosfeld, M. Heinrichs, P.J. Zak, U. Fischbacher, E. Fehr, Oxytocin increases
Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal BOLD response trust in humans, Nature 435 (2005) 673–676.
during reward anticipation in healthy women, Neuroimage 52 (2010) 277– [127] M.L. Kringelbach, E.T. Rolls, The functional neuroanatomy of the human
283. orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology,
[97] E.J. Hermans, P. Putman, J.M. Baas, N.M. Gecks, J.L. Kenemans, J. van Honk, Prog. Neurobiol. 72 (2004) 341–372.
Exogenous testosterone attenuates the integrated central stress response in [128] I. Labuschagne, K.L. Phan, A. Wood, M. Angstadt, P. Chua, M. Heinrichs, J.C.
healthy young women, Psychoneuroendocrinology 32 (2007) 1052–1061. Stout, P.J. Nathan, Oxytocin attenuates amygdala reactivity to fear in
[98] E.J. Hermans, P. Putman, J.M. Baas, H.P. Koppeschaar, J. van Honk, A single generalized social anxiety disorder, Neuropsychopharmacology 35 (2010)
administration of testosterone reduces fear-potentiated startle in humans, 2403–2413.
Biol. Psychiatry 59 (2006) 872–874. [129] R. Landgraf, I.D. Neumann, Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the
[99] E.J. Hermans, P. Putman, J. van Honk, Testosterone administration reduces brain: a dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide
empathetic behavior: a facial mimicry study, Psychoneuroendocrinology 31 communication, Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25 (2004) 150–176.
(2006) 859–866. [130] M.M. Lim, L.J. Young, Neuropeptidergic regulation of affiliative behavior and
[100] E.J. Hermans, N.F. Ramsey, J. van Honk, Exogenous testosterone enhances social bonding in animals, Horm. Behav. 50 (2006) 506–517.
responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in [131] P.D. MacLean, The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Paleocerebral Functions,
humans, Biol. Psychiatry 63 (2008) 263–270. Plenum Press, New York, 1990.
[101] E. Hollander, J. Bartz, W. Chaplin, A. Phillips, J. Sumner, L. Soorya, E. [132] A.A. Marsh, H.H. Yu, D.S. Pine, R.J. Blair, Oxytocin improves specific
Anagnostou, S. Wasserman, Oxytocin increases retention of social cognition recognition of positive facial expressions, Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 209
in autism, Biol. Psychiatry 61 (2007) 498–503. (2010) 225–232.
[102] E. Hollander, S. Novotny, M. Hanratty, R. Yaffe, C.M. DeCaria, B.R. Aronowitz, [133] P.H. Mehta, J. Beer, Neural mechanisms of the testosterone-aggression
S. Mosovich, Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with relation: the role of orbito-frontal cortex, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22 (2009)
autistic and Asperger’s disorders, Neuropsychopharmacology 28 (2003) 193– 2357–2368.
198. [134] G. Meinlschmidt, C. Heim, Sensitivity to intranasal oxytocin in adult men
[103] S.B. Hrdy, Evolutionary context of human development: the cooperative with early parental separation, Biol. Psychiatry 61 (2007) 1109–1111.
breeding model, in: L.A.C.S. Carter, K.E. Grossmann, S.B. Hrdy, M.E. Lamb, [135] A. Meyer-Lindenberg, Impact of prosocial neuropeptides on human brain
S.W. Porges, N. Sachser (Eds.), Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis, function, Prog. Brain Res. 170 (2008) 463–470.
The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2005, pp. 9–32. [136] G. Michels, U.C. Hoppe, Rapid actions of androgens, Front. Neuroendocrinol.
[104] S.B. Hrdy, Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual 29 (2008) 182–198.
Understanding, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, 2009. [137] M. Mikolajczak, J.J. Gross, A. Lane, O. Corneille, P. de Timary, O. Luminet,
[105] D. Huber, P. Veinante, R. Stoop, Vasopressin and oxytocin excite distinct Oxytocin makes people trusting, not gullible, Psychol. Sci. 21 (2010) 1072–
neuronal populations in the central amygdala, Science 308 (2005) 245– 1074.
248. [138] M. Mikolajczak, N. Pinon, A. Lane, P. de Timary, O. Luminet, Oxytocin not only
[106] R. Hurlemann, A. Patin, O.A. Onur, M.X. Cohen, T. Baumgartner, S. Metzler, I. increases trust when money is at stake, but also when confidential
Dziobek, J. Gallinat, M. Wagner, W. Maier, K.M. Kendrick, Oxytocin enhances information is in the balance, Biol. Psychol. 85 (2010) 182–184.
amygdala-dependent, socially reinforced learning and emotional empathy in [139] K. Mogg, M. Garner, B.P. Bradley, Anxiety and orienting of gaze to angry and
humans, J. Neurosci. 30 (2010) 4999–5007. fearful faces, Biol. Psychol. 76 (2007) 163–169.
[107] M. Iacoboni, M. Dapretto, The mirror neuron system and the consequences of [140] F. Naber, M.H. van Ijzendoorn, P. Deschamps, H. van Engeland, M.J.
its dysfunction, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7 (2006) 942–951. Bakermans-Kranenburg, Intranasal oxytocin increases fathers’ observed
34 P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35

responsiveness during play with their children: a double-blind within- care, Horm. Behav. 52 (2007) 114–121 (Horm. Behav. 52 (2007) 557–559;
subject experiment, Psychoneuroendocrinology 35 (2010) 1583–1586. author reply 560).
[141] E.E. Nelson, J. Panksepp, Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment: [172] J. Schulkin, Autism and the amygdala: an endocrine hypothesis, Brain Cogn.
contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine, Neurosci. Biobehav. 65 (2007) 87–99.
Rev. 22 (1998) 437. [173] D.J. Schutter, J. van Honk, Decoupling of midfrontal delta-beta oscillations
[142] R.J. Nelson, B.C. Trainor, Neural mechanisms of aggression, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. after testosterone administration, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 53 (2004) 71–73.
8 (2007) 536–546. [174] B. Seltzer, D.N. Pandya, Frontal lobe connections of the superior temporal
[143] I. Neumann, J.A. Russell, R. Landgraf, Oxytocin and vasopressin release within sulcus in the rhesus monkey, J. Comp. Neurol. 281 (1989) 97–113.
the supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei of pregnant, parturient and [175] S.G. Shamay-Tsoory, M. Fischer, J. Dvash, H. Harari, N. Perach-Bloom, Y.
lactating rats: a microdialysis study, Neuroscience 53 (1993) 65–75. Levkovitz, Intranasal administration of oxytocin increases envy and
[144] I.D. Neumann, The advantage of social living: brain neuropeptides mediate schadenfreude (gloating), Biol. Psychiatry 66 (2009) 864–870.
the beneficial consequences of sex and motherhood, Front. Neuroendocrinol. [176] T. Singer, B. Seymour, J. O’Doherty, H. Kaube, R.J. Dolan, C.D. Frith, Empathy
30 (2009) 483–496. for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain, Science
[145] G.J. Norman, J.T. Cacioppo, J.S. Morris, K. Karelina, W.B. Malarkey, A.C. 303 (2004) 1157–1162.
Devries, G.G. Berntson, Selective influences of oxytocin on the evaluative [177] T. Singer, R. Snozzi, G. Bird, P. Petrovic, G. Silani, M. Heinrichs, R.J. Dolan,
processing of social stimuli, J. Psychopharmacol. (2010) [Epub ahead of print]. Effects of oxytocin and prosocial behavior on brain responses to direct and
[146] S. Ogawa, V. Eng, J. Taylor, D.B. Lubahn, K.S. Korach, D.W. Pfaff, Roles of vicariously experienced pain, Emotion 8 (2008) 781–791.
estrogen receptor-alpha gene expression in reproduction-related behaviors [178] T.S. Sperry, D.W. Wacker, J.C. Wingfield, The role of androgen receptors in
in female mice, Endocrinology 139 (1998) 5070–5081. regulating territorial aggression in male song sparrows, Horm. Behav. (2009).
[147] A. Ohman, Face the beast and fear the face: animal and social fears as [179] A.E. Storey, C.J. Walsh, R.L. Quinton, K.E. Wynne-Edwards, Hormonal
prototypes for evolutionary analyses of emotion, Psychophysiology 23 (1986) correlates of paternal responsiveness in new and expectant fathers, Evol.
123–145. Hum. Behav. 21 (2000) 79–95.
[148] J. Panksepp, Brain opioids: a neurochemical substrate for narcotic and social [180] L. Strathearn, P. Fonagy, J. Amico, P.R. Montague, Adult attachment predicts
dependence, in: S. Cooper (Ed.), Progress in Theory in Psychopharmacology, maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues,
Academic Press, London, 1981, pp. 149–175. Neuropsychopharmacology 34 (2009) 2655–2666.
[149] J. Panksepp, Oxytocin effects on emotional processes: separation distress, [181] J.E. Swain, E. Tasgin, L.C. Mayes, R. Feldman, R.T. Constable, J.F. Leckman,
social bonding, and relationships to psychiatric disorders, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. Maternal brain response to own baby-cry is affected by cesarean section
652 (1992) 243–252. delivery, J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49 (2008) 1042–1052.
[150] J. Panksepp, Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal [182] K. Takahashi, M. Bergstrom, P. Frandberg, E.L. Vesstrom, Y. Watanabe, B.
Emotions, Oxford University Press, New York, 1998. Langstrom, Imaging of aromatase distribution in rat and rhesus monkey
[151] J. Panksepp, The neuro-evolutionary cusp between emotions and cognitions: brains with [11C]vorozole, Nucl. Med. Biol. 33 (2006) 599–605.
implications for understanding consciousness and the emergence of a unified [183] S.E. Taylor, L.C. Klein, B.P. Lewis, T.L. Gruenewald, R.A. Gurung, J.A. Updegraff,
mind science, Evol. Cogn. 7 (2001) 141–163. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-
[152] J. Panksepp, Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and flight, Psychol. Rev. 107 (2000) 411–429.
humans, Conscious. Cogn. 14 (2005) 30–80. [184] H. Thackare, H.D. Nicholson, K. Whittington, Oxytocin – its role in male
[153] J. Panksepp, Primary process affects and brain oxytocin, Biol. Psychiatry 65 reproduction and new potential therapeutic uses, Hum. Reprod. Update 12
(2009) 725–727. (2006) 437–448.
[154] J. Panksepp, B.H. Herman, T. Vilberg, P. Bishop, F.G. DeEskinazi, Endogenous [185] A. Theodoridou, A.C. Rowe, I.S. Penton-Voak, P.J. Rogers, Oxytocin and social
opioids and social behavior, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 4 (1980) 473–487. perception: oxytocin increases perceived facial trustworthiness and
[155] J. Panksepp, S.M. Siviy, L.A. Nomansell, Brain opioids and social emotions, in: attractiveness, Horm. Behav. 56 (2009) 128–132.
M. Reite, T. Fields (Eds.), The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation, [186] R. Thompson, S. Gupta, K. Miller, S. Mills, S. Orr, The effects of vasopressin on
Academic Press, New York, 1985, pp. 3–49. human facial responses related to social communication,
[156] A. Perry, S. Bentin, I. Shalev, S. Israel, F. Uzefovsky, D. Bar-On, R.P. Ebstein, Psychoneuroendocrinology 29 (2004) 35–48.
Intranasal oxytocin modulates EEG mu/alpha and beta rhythms during [187] R.R. Thompson, K. George, J.C. Walton, S.P. Orr, J. Benson, Sex-specific
perception of biological motion, Psychoneuroendocrinology 35 (2010) 1446– influences of vasopressin on human social communication, Proc. Natl. Acad.
1453. Sci. USA 103 (2006) 7889–7894.
[157] P. Petrovic, R. Kalisch, T. Singer, R.J. Dolan, Oxytocin attenuates affective [188] J.W. Thornton, Evolution of vertebrate steroid receptors from an ancestral
evaluations of conditioned faces and amygdala activity, J. Neurosci. 28 (2008) estrogen receptor by ligand exploitation and serial genome expansions, Proc.
6607–6615. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98 (2001) 5671–5676.
[158] S. Pierman, M. Sica, F. Allieri, C. Viglietti-Panzica, G.C. Panzica, J. Bakker, [189] B.C. Trainor, C.A. Marler, Testosterone promotes paternal behaviour in a
Activational effects of estradiol and dihydrotestosterone on social recognition monogamous mammal via conversion to oestrogen, Proc. Biol. Sci. 269 (2002)
and the arginine-vasopressin immunoreactive system in male mice lacking a 823–829.
functional aromatase gene, Horm. Behav. 54 (2008) 98–106. [190] A. Tuiten, J. Van Honk, H. Koppeschaar, C. Bernaards, J. Thijssen, R. Verbaten,
[159] R. Pietrowsky, C. Struben, M. Molle, H.L. Fehm, J. Born, Brain potential Time course of effects of testosterone administration on sexual arousal in
changes after intranasal vs. intravenous administration of vasopressin: women, Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57 (2000) 149–153 (discussion 155–146).
evidence for a direct nose-brain pathway for peptide effects in humans, [191] C. Unkelbach, A.J. Guastella, J.P. Forgas, Oxytocin selectively facilitates
Biol. Psychiatry 39 (1996) 332–340. recognition of positive sex and relationship words, Psychol. Sci. 19 (2008)
[160] R.K. Pitman, S.P. Orr, N.B. Lasko, Effects of intranasal vasopressin and oxytocin 1092–1094.
on physiologic responding during personal combat imagery in Vietnam [192] K. Uvnas-Moberg, Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social
veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder, Psychiatry Res. 48 (1993) 107– interaction and emotions, Psychoneuroendocrinology 23 (1998) 819–835.
117. [193] K. Uvnas-Moberg, The Oxytocin Factor: Tapping the Hormone of Love, Calm
[161] S.W. Porges, The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social and Healing, Da Capo Press, Camebridge, MA, 2003.
nervous system, Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42 (2001) 123–146. [194] M. van‘t Wout, A.G. Sanfey, Friend or foe: the effect of implicit
[162] S.M. Reader, K.N. Laland, Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain trustworthiness judgments in social decision-making, Cognition 108 (2008)
size in primates, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99 (2002) 4436–4441. 796–803.
[163] L. Remage-Healey, N.T. Maidment, B.A. Schlinger, Forebrain steroid levels [195] F. van der Made, J. Bloemers, W.E. Yassem, G. Kleiverda, W. Everaerd, D. van
fluctuate rapidly during social interactions, Nat. Neurosci. 11 (2008) 1327– Ham, B. Olivier, H. Koppeschaar, A. Tuiten, The influence of testosterone
1334. combined with a PDE5-inhibitor on cognitive, affective, and physiological
[164] U. Rimmele, K. Hediger, M. Heinrichs, P. Klaver, Oxytocin makes a face in sexual functioning in women suffering from sexual dysfunction, J. Sex Med. 6
memory familiar, J. Neurosci. 29 (2009) 38–42. (2009) 777–790.
[165] H.E. Ross, L.J. Young, Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social [196] J. van Honk, Neuroendocrine manipulation of the sexually dimorphic human
cognition and affiliative behavior, Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30 (2009) 534– social brain, in: E. Harmon-Jones, J.S. Beer (Eds.), Methods in Social
547. Neuroscience, Guilford Press, New York, 2009.
[166] D.R. Rubinow, P.J. Schmidt, Androgens, brain, and behavior, Am. J. Psychiatry [197] J. van Honk, J.S. Peper, D.J. Schutter, Testosterone reduces unconscious fear
153 (1996) 974–984. but not consciously experienced anxiety: implications for the disorders of
[167] R.M. Sapolsky, Testicular Function, Social rank and personality among wild fear and anxiety, Biol. Psychiatry 58 (2005) 218–225.
baboons, Psychoneuroendocrinology 16 (1991) 281–293. [198] J. van Honk, D.J. Schutter, Unmasking feigned sanity: a neurobiological model
[168] R.M. Sapolsky, Why Zebras don’t get Ulcers, Owl Books, New York, 2004. of emotion processing in primary psychopathy, Cogn. Neuropsych. 11 (2006)
[169] E. Savaskan, R. Ehrhardt, A. Schulz, M. Walter, H. Schachinger, Post-learning 285–306.
intranasal oxytocin modulates human memory for facial identity, [199] J. van Honk, D.J. Schutter, Testosterone reduces conscious detection of signals
Psychoneuroendocrinology 33 (2008) 368–374. serving social correction: implications for antisocial behavior, Psychol. Sci. 18
[170] L.A. Schleifer, A.J. Justice, H. de Wit, Lack of effects of acute estradiol on mood (2007) 663–667.
in postmenopausal women, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 71 (2002) 71–77. [200] J. van Honk, D.J. Schutter, E.J. Hermans, P. Putman, A. Tuiten, H. Koppeschaar,
[171] C. Schradin, Comments to K.E. Wynne-Edwards and M.E. Timonin 2007. Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and
Paternal care in rodents: Weakening support of hormonal regulation of the reward in healthy young women, Psychoneuroendocrinology 29 (2004) 937–
transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental 943.
P.A. Bos et al. / Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 33 (2012) 17–35 35

[201] J. van Honk, A. Tuiten, E. Hermans, P. Putman, H. Koppeschaar, J. Thijssen, R. [210] S.R. Wersinger, H.K. Caldwell, M. Christiansen, W.S. Young III, Disruption of
Verbaten, L. van Doornen, A single administration of testosterone induces the vasopressin 1b receptor gene impairs the attack component of aggressive
cardiac accelerative responses to angry faces in healthy young women, behavior in mice, Genes Brain Behav. 6 (2007) 653–660.
Behav. Neurosci. 115 (2001) 238–242. [211] R.J. Windle, N. Shanks, S.L. Lightman, C.D. Ingram, Central oxytocin
[202] R.E. Van Kesteren, A.B. Smit, R.P. De Lange, K.S. Kits, F.A. Van Golen, R.C. administration reduces stress-induced corticosterone release and anxiety
Van Der Schors, N.D. De With, J.F. Burke, W.P. Geraerts, Structural and behavior in rats, Endocrinology 138 (1997) 2829–2834.
functional evolution of the vasopressin/oxytocin superfamily: vasopressin- [212] J.C. Wingfield, R.E. Hegner, J. Dufty, M. Alfred, G.F. Ball, The ‘‘challenge
related conopressin is the only member present in lymnaea, and is hypothesis’’: theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion,
involved in the control of sexual behavior, J. Neurosci. 15 (1995) 5989– mating systems, and breeding strategies, Am. Nat. 136 (1990) 829.
5998. [213] J.T. Winslow, T.R. Insel, Social status in pairs of male squirrel monkeys
[203] F. Van Overwalle, K. Baetens, Understanding others’ actions and goals by determines the behavioral response to central oxytocin administration, J.
mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis, Neuroimage 48 (2009) Neurosci. 11 (1991) 2032–2038.
564–584. [214] J.S. Winston, B.A. Strange, J. O’Doherty, R.J. Dolan, Automatic and intentional
[204] G. van Wingen, C. Mattern, R.J. Verkes, J. Buitelaar, G. Fernandez, Testosterone brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces, Nat. Neurosci.
biases automatic memory processes in women towards potential mates, 5 (2002) 277–283.
Neuroimage 43 (2008) 114–120. [215] C.T. Wotjak, M. Kubota, G. Liebsch, A. Montkowski, F. Holsboer, I. Neumann,
[205] G. van Wingen, C. Mattern, R.J. Verkes, J. Buitelaar, G. Fernandez, Testosterone R. Landgraf, Release of vasopressin within the rat paraventricular nucleus in
reduces amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex coupling, Psychoneuroendocrinology response to emotional stress: a novel mechanism of regulating
35 (2010) 105–113. adrenocorticotropic hormone secretion?, J Neurosci. 16 (1996) 7725–7732.
[206] G.A. van Wingen, S.A. Zylicz, S. Pieters, C. Mattern, R.J. Verkes, J.K. Buitelaar, G. [216] K.E. Wynne-Edwards, C.J. Reburn, Behavioral endocrinology of mammalian
Fernandez, Testosterone increases amygdala reactivity in middle-aged fatherhood, Trends Ecol. Evol. 15 (2000) 464–468.
women to a young adulthood level, Neuropsychopharmacology 34 (2009) [217] K.E. Wynne-Edwards, M.E. Timonin, Paternal care in rodents: weakening
539–547. support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in
[207] A.H. Veenema, D.I. Beiderbeck, M. Lukas, I.D. Neumann, Distinct correlations rodent animal models of biparental care, Horm. Behav. 52 (2007) 114–121.
of vasopressin release within the lateral septum and the bed nucleus of the [218] P.J. Zak, R. Kurzban, S. Ahmadi, R.S. Swerdloff, J. Park, L. Efremidze, K.
stria terminalis with the display of intermale aggression, Horm. Behav. 58 Redwine, K. Morgan, W. Matzner, Testosterone administration decreases
(2010) 273–281. generosity in the ultimatum game, PLoS ONE 4 (2009) e8330.
[208] J.G. Veening, T. de Jong, H.P. Barendregt, Oxytocin-messages via the [219] P.J. Zak, A.A. Stanton, S. Ahmadi, Oxytocin increases generosity in humans,
cerebrospinal fluid: behavioral effects; a review, Physiol. Behav. 101 (2010) PLoS ONE 2 (2007) e1128.
193–210. [220] C.F. Zink, J.L. Stein, L. Kempf, S. Hakimi, A. Meyer-Lindenberg, Vasopressin
[209] A. Waytz, N. Epley, J.T. Cacioppo, Social cognition unbound, Cur. Dir. Psychol. modulates medial prefrontal cortex-amygdala circuitry during emotion
Sci. 19 (2010) 58–62. processing in humans, J. Neurosci. 30 (2010) 7017–7022.

También podría gustarte