Está en la página 1de 6

Journal of the Neurological Sciences 364 (2016) 110–115

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of the Neurological Sciences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jns

Know thyself: Exploring interoceptive sensitivity in Parkinson's disease


Lucia Ricciardi a, Gina Ferrazzano b, Benedetta Demartini c, Francesca Morgante d, Roberto Erro a,
Christos Ganos a,e, Kailash P. Bhatia a, Alfredo Berardelli b,f, Mark Edwards a,⁎
a
Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, UCL Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, UK
b
IRCCS Neuromed, Pozzilli (IS), Italy
c
Department of Psychiatry, San Paolo Hospital, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
d
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Messina, Messina, Italy
e
Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (UKE), Hamburg, Germany
f
IRCCS Neuromed Institute, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background: Although Parkinson's disease (PD) is defined by its motor symptoms, it is now well recognised that
Received 7 October 2015 cognitive, affective and emotion domains are also impaired. The pathophysiology of these disabling non-motor
Received in revised form 10 March 2016 symptoms (NMS) remains unclear; recently the involvement of limbic areas, including the insula, in the neuro-
Accepted 11 March 2016 degenerative process has been suggested to have a key role. These areas, and the insula in particular, are also been
Available online 12 March 2016
suggested as key regions for interoception; interoceptive sensitivity (IS) is a measure of the accuracy of percep-
tion of sensations from inside the body related to the function of internal organs.
Keywords:
Parkinson's disease
Objectives: To evaluate IS in PD patients by means of a well-established task: the heartbeat perception task. More-
Interoception over, we evaluated possible correlations between IS and psychological, affective and disease-related characteris-
Non-motor symptoms tics as well as fatigue perception in PD patients.
Emotion Methods: Twenty PD patients and 20 healthy subjects (HS) were included and underwent the heartbeat percep-
Fatigue tion task. An extensive evaluation of motor, non-motor, affective and emotion domains was carried out.
Results: PD patients showed lower IS than HS (0.58 ± 0.2 vs 0.72 ± 0.1; p = 0.04). PD reported higher scores in
scales assessing depression (Hamilton depression scale: 8.7 ± 5.8 vs 6.2 ± 7.5; p = 0.04); anhedonia (Snaith–
Hamilton Pleasure Scale: 26.8 ± 9.7 vs 15.4 ± 2.9; p = b 0.001) and apathy (Apathy Evaluation Scale: 35.8 ±
8.6 vs 27.8 ± 6.8; p = 0.008). No significant correlations were detected between IS and motor, non-motor, affec-
tive and emotion symptoms.
Conclusions: PD patients have reduced interoceptive sensitivity. Future studies are encouraged to evaluate the
importance of interoception in understanding the pathophysiology of affective/emotional symptoms in PD.
© 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction interoception: the representation and contextualisation of somatic and


visceral responses elicited by emotional stimuli. [3].
Parkinson's disease (PD) is far from just a motor disorder. The im- In PD many components of the emotional processing are altered, [4]
portance of non-motor symptoms has been increasingly recognised, however there is still a lack of clarity on the neurobiological basis of
particularly in how they significantly add to the burden of PD for pa- these deficits. Nevertheless, the role of alpha-synuclein deposition and
tients and impair quality of life. [1] Despite the clinical importance of subsequent degeneration of the insula has been highlighted, and sug-
NMS, the pathophysiology of these symptoms remains unclear. gested as an anatomical region where degeneration could explain a
Limbic areas including insula, amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex number of NMS, including depression, anxiety, apathy, anhedonia and
are key regions in emotional processing. All these regions, in particular fatigue. [5].
the insula, are involved in the process of interoception, the perception When studying these aspects of emotion, researchers face two main
of sensations from inside the body, including the perception of physical problems: first, studies are conducted in laboratory and clinical settings
sensations related to the function of internal organs. [2] It is proposed and these contexts significantly differ from real life, therefore the gener-
that subjective “feeling states” are dependent on the process of alizability of findings obtained from this research is limited. [6] Second,
the diagnosis of these symptoms relies mainly on self-report question-
naires, the validity of which can be called into question when assessing
⁎ Corresponding author at: St. George's University of London, London, UK. disorders characterized by an inability or an alteration in identifying and
E-mail address: medwards@sglu.ac.ukuk (M. Edwards). judging one's own affective and emotional states. [7] The difficulty in

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2016.03.019
0022-510X/© 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Downloaded from ClinicalKey.com at Rutgers University - NERL January 16, 2017.


For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L. Ricciardi et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 364 (2016) 110–115 111

evaluating affective and emotional disorders in PD patients has been major concurrent neurological, cardiac or psychiatric disorders were ex-
also demonstrated in several studies showing a lack of agreement be- cluded. Full demographical and clinical data are presented in Table 1.
tween patients and their caregivers when scoring the severity of these All subjects agreed to participate to the study and signed a consent
symptoms. [8–9]. form; the local institutional review board approved the study protocol.
For these reasons, it would be very useful to find an objective mea-
sure which would correlate to the severity of NMS such as fatigue per- 2.2. Materials
ception, affective and emotional symptoms, which could complement
and implement the assessment scales in the diagnosis and follow-up 2.2.1. Procedure
of these aspects of the disease. If deficits in interoceptive processing After arriving at the laboratory, all participants completed a ques-
might reflect disease-related insula degeneration and this might under- tionnaire regarding socio-demographic and clinical information (for pa-
lie a proportion of NMS, then measurement of interoceptive sensitivity tients). Items included age, educational level, medical history,
(IS) could be a useful biomarker to study. In healthy individuals, higher medications taken.
IS is associated with the experience of more intense emotional states For the patients, information on disease onset, progression and dura-
[10] and higher activation of brain areas thought to play a key role in tion and on dopaminergic therapy expressed in terms of Levodopa
emotional processing (insular cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, Equivalent Daily Dose (LEDD) [17] were collected. Patients underwent
ventro-medial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the somatosenso- a clinical evaluation including: assessment of motor severity and com-
ry cortex). [2]. plications, i.e. fluctuation and dyskinesia by means of the Unified
Moreover, the literature currently suggests an association between a Parkinson's disease rating scale (UPDRS) section III and IV [18]; disabil-
number of neuropsychiatric problems and reduced IS, including major ity and non-motor symptoms (NMS) [section II of the UPDRS and the
depression, somatoform disorder, functional neurological symptoms NMS scale (NMSS)]; [19] overall cognitive assessment by means of the
and eating disorders. A disturbed interoceptive awareness of the state section I of the UPDRS, and the MOCA. The experimental procedures
of the body has been hypothesized to be linked to symptoms of chronic and the self-report measures (see below) were conducted in a soft-
fatigue and chronic pain [11–13]. One explanation for this might be that lighted, sound-attenuated room. All patient evaluations were per-
a “noisy” interoceptive signal could result in an inaccurate perception of formed in their “best ON” state.
internal state, which might in turn generate symptoms such as fatigue
and pain in the absence of a specific cause. 2.2.2. Heart beat detection task
We hypothesized that due to the neurodegenerative processes in Participants were seated, with their wrists gently resting on the
limbic areas (involved in the interoceptive sensitivity) patients with band of a heart rate monitor, which was located on a table in front of
PD may present a disturbed IS which could make them prone to symp- them. The ‘Heartbeat Perception Task’ was performed according to the
toms such as pain, fatigue, emotional/affective disorders. Therefore, we protocol by Schandry. [20] Heart rate was recorded with a Polar wrist
aimed to evaluate using a validated and widely used measure of IS - the worn heart rate monitor (model RS 800 CX). [20] Participants were
heartbeat perception task – whether patients with PD would have dis- first asked to sit quietly and relax for 30 s, subsequently baseline heart
turbed IS compared to healthy subjects, and whether disturbed IS rate was recorded for 3 min before the task started in order to evaluate
could be related to the presence and severity of NMS, especially affec- differences in heart rate frequency. Participants were then asked to con-
tion/emotion symptoms, pain and fatigue perception. centrate on their heartbeats and to count them silently. They were not
permitted to take their pulse or to attempt any other physical manipu-
2. Methods lations, which could facilitate detection. There were three counting
phases lasting for 25 s, 35 s, and 45 s and separated by 30 s rest periods.
2.1. Participants The order of the phases was randomized between participants of each
group. An acoustic signal provided the ‘start’ and ‘stop’ indications of
We recruited 20 consecutive PD patients from the movement disor- each counting phase. After each stop signal, participants were asked to
der outpatient clinics of the National Hospital for Neurology and Neuro- report the number of counted heartbeats. Participants were aware nei-
surgery, London. Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of PD according to ther of the length of the counting phases, nor of their performance.
UK Brain Bank criteria, [14] with treatment and clinical condition stable
for at least 4 weeks prior to the study. Exclusion criteria were any major 2.2.3. Psychiatric, affective and emotion assessment
concurrent neurological, cardiac or psychiatric disorders; clinically sig- The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; 21) was used as a measure
nificant cognitive deficits or score b 26 on the Montreal Cognitive As- of alexithymia. The TAS-20 is the most commonly used self-report mea-
sessment (MOCA), [15] severe depression [diagnosed according to surement of alexithymia, [19] with demonstrated reliability and validi-
both the DMS-IV TR criteria and a Hamilton depression Scale ≥14. [16] ty. [22] The scale consists of 20 items rated on a 5-point scale, anchored
Twenty healthy individuals, matched for age and gender were also re- at ‘1 = strongly disagree’ to ‘5 = strongly agree’, with a total score rang-
cruited and served as a control group. Individuals with a history of any ing from 20 to 100. Three sub-scores can also be calculated but these
were not used in the current study due to the relatively small sample
sizes and related power issues. Higher scores indicate greater
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristic of PD patients.
alexithymia. A total score of above 61 is considered the cut off score
for alexithymia. [21].
Gender (M/F) 13/7 The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD) is the most used and
Age at onset 53.4 ± 10.9
validated depression scale overall and has been used in PD. [23] The
Disease duration 6.8 ± 3.3
UPDRS-I 2.1 ± 1.6 HAMD is an interview-based rating scale and consists of 17 items: 16
UPDRS-II 7.9 ± 4.3 question-based items and one observational item. The item scores
UPDRS-III 15.9 ± 7.2 range from 0 to 4 (symptom is absent, mild, moderate, or severe) or
UPDRS-IV 2.4 ± 3.4
0–2 (absent, slight or trivial, clearly present). The total score can range
H&Y 1.5 ± 0.5
LEDD (total) 560.6 ± 345.5 from 0 to 54. A cut-off score of 9/10 has been suggested for screening
NMSS 42.5 ± 20.0 depression in PD patients, and 14 as a cut-off for major depressive dis-
Abbreviation: F: Female; H&Y: Hoehn and Yahr scale; LEDD: levo-
order in PD. [16].
dopa equivalent daily dose; M: Male; NMSS: Non-motor symptoms The Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (Ham-A) is a 14-item clinician-
scale; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson's disease Rating scale. rated instrument designed to assess and quantify severity of anxiety

Downloaded from ClinicalKey.com at Rutgers University - NERL January 16, 2017.


For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
112 L. Ricciardi et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 364 (2016) 110–115

[24]. Each item is rated on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging 0 to 4, Table 2


with a total score range from 0 to 56, where a score b 17 indicates Clinical evaluation of affective and emotional profiles of study populations.

mild severity, a score between 18 and 24 indicates moderate severity PD (n = 20) HC (n = 20) p-Value
and a score between 25 and 30 indicates a moderate to severe anxiety. Gender 13 M 8M 0.7
[24]. Age 61.4 ± 9.8 56.5 ± 10.8 0.13
The Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) is a self-rated scale to identify MOCA 28.8 ± 1.4 28.3 ± 1.4 0.19
clinically significant apathy, it consists of 18 items scored on 4-point HB baseline 236.4 ± 46.2 220.6 ± 41.6 0.19
IS 0.58 ± 0.22 0.72 ± 0.14 0.04
Likert scale (not at all true, slightly true, somewhat true, very true).
TAS-20 total score 50.9 ± 11.4 44.7 ± 8.2 0.16
Total score ranges from 18 to 72 points (higher score indicates more se- TAS-20 DIF 13.4 ± 3.5 13.5 ± 12.7 0.05
vere apathy). The cut-off score ≥ 37 was generally used to divide apa- TAS-20 DDF 18.2 ± 4.9 14.7 ± 3.9 0.05
thetic from non-apathetic patients [25]. TAS-20 EOT 19.5 ± 4.2 20.7 ± 4.2 0.4
The Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS) [26] is a 14 item scale Ham-Dep 8.7 ± 5.8 6.2 ± 7.5 0.04
Ham-Anx 12.8 ± 9.4 7.9 ± 9.5 0.05
designed to assess anhedonia in different psychiatric conditions. Re- SHAPS 26.8 ± 9.7 15.4 ± 2.9 b0.001
cently Ameli and coworkers have modified the initial version of the Empathy quotient 39.8 ± 9.2 46.2 ± 7.4 0.09
scale to improve its utility [27]. Items are scored from 1 to 4 (1 = lots Apathy (AES) 35.8 ± 8.6 27.8 ± 6.8 0.008
of pleasure, 4 = no pleasure) creating a score ranging from 14 to 56. Fatigue SS 37.4 ± 14.2 29.6 ± 15.1 0.12
The Empathy Quotient (EQ) is a psychological self-assessment ques- Values are means ± SD. Significantly different changes (Mann-Whitney U test) are bold
tionnaire measuring empathy levels. The EQ contains 60 items of which typed. Abbreviations: AES: Apathy Evaluation Scale; FSS: Fatigue Severity Scale; Ham-
40 are clinically relevant and 20 are for distraction only. For each item Dep: Hamilton depression scale; Ham-Anx: Hamilton anxiety scale; HB: heart beat; IS:
Interoception sensitivity; MOCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SHAPS: The Snaith-
the possible answers are: strongly agree, slightly agree, slightly dis- Hamilton Pleasure Scale; TAS: Toronto Alexithymia Scale.
agree, or strongly disagree, with one point given if the empathic state-
ment is recognised mildly (slightly agree) or two points given it if is
endorsed strongly (strongly agree). The scored is from 0 (being the significantly higher IS compared to patients as measured by the heart-
least empathetic possible) to 80 (being the most empathetic possible) beat perception task (0.72 ± 0.1 vs 0.58 ± 0.2; p = 0.04) (Fig. 1).
[28]. PD patients had significantly higher scores compared to HC for
The Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS) is a self-administered fatigue rating Hamilton-depression total score (PD 8.7 ± 5.8; HC 6.2 ± 7.5; p =
scale [29], which reveals the functional impact of fatigue during the past 0.04); SHAPS total score (PD 26.8 ± 9.7; HC 15.4 ± 2.9; p ≤ 0.001)
week. The FSS, which consists of 9 questions, uses a 7 point Likert scale and AES (PD 35.8 ± 8.6; HC 27.8 ± 6.8; p = 0.008).
ranging from strongly disagrees (score: 1) to strongly agree (score: 7). A Spearman's correlation showed no relationship between IS and
total score of b 36 suggests that subjects may not be suffering from fa- Alexithymia (TAS-20 score), depression (Ham-Dep score), anxiety
tigue, instead a total score of 36 or more suggests fatigue. (Ham-Anx score), apathy (AES score), anhedonia (SHAPS score), empa-
thy (EQ score) or fatigue (FSS) (Fig. 2). No significant correlations were
2.3. Statistical analysis detected between IS and disease characteristics (disease duration, age
at disease onset, LEDD, UPDRS, and severity of NMS in general (as re-
The accuracy of heartbeat perception was calculated as the mean vealed by NMSS total and sub-scores)) (Supplementary online Table 1).
score of three heartbeat perception interval according to the following Moreover, since the patients enrolled reported significantly higher
transformation: scores compared to HC at assessment scales for depressive symptoms
(Han-dep) and apathy (AES), to evaluate the influence of depression
1=3∑½ð1−ðjrecordedheartbeats–countedheartbeatsj=recordedheartbeatsÞÞ: and apathy on IS we clustered the PD patients in: 1) PD with depression
(n = 14) and PD without depression (n = 6) taking the Ham-Dep
Using this formula, the interoceptive sensitivity score can vary be- score N 13 as cut-off [16]; and 2) PD-apathetic (n = 7) vs PD-no-
tween 0 and 1, with higher scores indicating smaller differences be- apathetic (n = 13) taking AES score N 37 as cut-off. Mann-Whitney U
tween recorded and perceived heartbeats (i.e. more accuracy, or test showed that there was no significant difference in IS between the
higher interoceptive sensitivity). All analyses were performed using
SPSS version 21. Mann-Whitney U test was used to evaluate differences
between groups. Gender differences between the two groups were
assessed using the Fisher-exact test.
Spearman's correlations were conducted to examine the possible re-
lationship between interoceptive sensitivity and Alexithymia (TAS-20
score), depression (Ham-Dep score), anxiety (Ham-Anx score), apathy
(AES score), anhedonia (SHAPS score), empathy (EQ score), fatigue
(FSS). In order to evaluate possible correlations between IS and disease
characteristics (disease duration, age at disease onset, LEDD, UPDRS,
and severity of NMS (as revealed by NMSS total and sub-scores),
Spearman's correlations were conducted on the PD patients group
only. Significance level was set at p b 0.05.

3. Results

Twenty PD patients (13 males, mean age 61.4 ± 9.8 years) and 20
healthy controls (HC) (8 male, mean age 56.5 ± 10.8) were included
in the study. Demographic and clinical data of the study populations
are shown in Tables 1 and 2.
Baseline heartbeats were not significantly different between PD pa-
tients and HC (3 min baseline measure: 236.4 ± 46.2 vs 220.6 ± 41.6, Fig. 1. Interoception sensitivity, assessed by means of the heartbeat perception task, is
p = 0.1). A Mann-Whitney U test revealed that healthy controls had a reduced in PD patients compared to healthy subjects.

Downloaded from ClinicalKey.com at Rutgers University - NERL January 16, 2017.


For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L. Ricciardi et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 364 (2016) 110–115 113

Fig. 2. Spearman correlation analysis showed that interoception sensitivity has no significant correlation with alexithymia (TAS-20 score, panel A), depression (HDRS score, panel B),
anxiety (HARS score, panel C), anhedonia (SHAPS score, panel D), apathy (AES score, panel E), and fatigue (FSS, panel F).

depressed and non-depressed groups of patients (p = 0.3) and between expressivity and empathy). Finally, we did not collect any measure of
the apathetic and non-apathetic ones (p = 0.9). self-report interoceptive sensitivity (such as questionnaires or patients
‘confident level) which could have allow us to do further speculations
4. Discussion such as exploring the dissociation between different dimensions of
interoception [34]. Indeed, very little is known about IS deficits in clini-
This is the first work showing that interoceptive sensitivity, as mea- cal populations and this is the first study in PD.
sured by a heartbeat perception task, is reduced in PD. We have demon- Recent studies using a heartbeat perception task have showed that
strated that PD patients have a lower IS compared to healthy IS is positively correlated with intensity of emotional experience. [2,
individuals. However, we found no correlation between IS and a range 35] PD patients exhibit significant deficits in non-verbal communica-
of self-report measures of affective status, fatigue and non-motor symp- tion, having difficulties in both producing and recognising emotional fa-
toms in general. cial expressions and affective prosody [4–6]. In a recent study a
Interoception is the body-to-brain axis of sensation concerning the correlation between deficits in facial emotional expression and recogni-
state of the internal body and the organs. It is a key process for “periph- tion in PD patients has been demonstrated [36], hypothesizing for emo-
eral” theories of emotion which suggest that the central representation tion a mechanism similar to the one described for action execution and
and perception of changes in bodily physiology are the basis for emo- observation based on mirror neurons [37]. This is in line with a study by
tional feeling states [30]. The insula is thought to play a key role in Wicker et al. [37–38] showing that the recognition of disgust may in-
this process. Indeed, the connections and activation profile of the insula volve simulation of interoceptive states. Recent studies have demon-
suggest that it integrates visceral and somatic input and forms a repre- strated that PD patients also show a lack of self-awareness for their
sentation of the state of the body [3,29]. Alpha-synuclein has been motor symptoms, including levodopa-induced dyskinesia [39]. One
showed to be highly deposited throughout the insula by stage 5 in PD could speculate that the lack of ability to generate or recognise the emo-
patients [32]. tional content of facial expression or speech and the lack of awareness of
While the deficit we found in IS in patients with PD would be consis- somatic symptoms could be related to the deficit of awareness of viscer-
tent with this degenerative process involving the insula, we did not find al and somatic input that we found using the heart beat detection task in
any correlation between the degree of deficit in IS and the severity of this study.
non-motor symptoms, specifically affective symptoms. We acknowledge our study limitations, first the small sample size;
Recent research has suggested separate functional roles for the ante- second, as we have already pointed out, the fact that our PD patients
rior and posterior insula in more cognitive/affective and viscero- did not have severe NMS; third, PD patients were more depressed and
sensory/somatosensory awareness, respectively [33] This could contrib- apathetic than healthy subjects, this does not allow to fully exclude a
ute to explaining our results, as there may be a dissociation between IS possible influence of depression and apathy on IS, although there was
as measured by the heartbeat perception task, and interoceptive pro- no correlation between IS and the score of the Hamilton-depression
cessing that is related more closely to affective/emotional function. scale. Finally, the fact that all patients' evaluations were performed in
There are other caveats to our results. First, our sample of PD patients their “best ON” state, thus not allowing us to evaluate how levodopa in-
did not have severe NMS, and this might have made difficult to show fluences IS.
a significant correlation between IS and severity of NMS because of In conclusion, our data demonstrate in PD patients a lower intero-
the lack of a group of patients with high score at NMSS. Second, reduced ceptive sensitivity, a measure which might be related to insular degen-
IS in PD could be related to other aspects of the disease which we have eration. This deficit is related neither to the presence and severity of
not measured or assessed in our protocol (such as emotional non-motor symptoms nor to any other disease characteristic we

Downloaded from ClinicalKey.com at Rutgers University - NERL January 16, 2017.


For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
114 L. Ricciardi et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 364 (2016) 110–115

measured in this study. Nevertheless, given the evidence of a link be- [3] A.D. Craig, How do you feel now? The anterior insula and human awareness,
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 (1) (2009 Jan) 59–70, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/
tween IS and emotional processing, the finding of reduced interoceptive nrn2555.
sensitivity in PD points the way to further studies in this area. [4] J. Peron, T. Dondaine, F. Le Jeune, et al., Emotional processing in Parkinson's disease:
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at http://dx. a systematic review, Mov. Disord. 27 (2012) 186–199.
[5] L. Christopher, Y. Koshimori, A.E. Lang, et al., Uncovering the role of the insula in
doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2016.03.019. non-motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease, Brain 137 (Pt 8) (2014 Aug)
2143–2154.
Funding statement [6] I. Sotgiu, M.L. Rusconi, Investigating emotions in Parkinson's disease: what we know
and what we still don't know, Front. Psychol. 4 (2013 Jun 10) 336.
[7] L. Ricciardi, B. Demartini, A. Fotopoulou, et al., Alexithymia in neurological
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in disease: a review, J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2015 Feb 6)
the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. appineuropsych14070169.
[8] A. Fleming, K.F. Cook, N.D. Nelson, et al., Proxy reports in Parkinson's disease: care-
giver and patient self-reports of quality of life and physical activity, Mov. Disord. 20
Competing interest statement (11) (2005 Nov) 1462–1468.
[9] A. McKinlay, R.C. Grace, J.C. Dalrymple-Alford, et al., Neuropsychiatric problems in
Dr. Ricciardi, Dr. Ferrazzano, Dr. Demartini, Dr. Erro, Dr. Ganos and Parkinson's disease: comparisons between self and caregiver report, Aging Ment.
Health 12 (5) (2008 Sep) 647–653.
Prof Berardelli report no disclosures. [10] S. Wiens, E. Mezzacappa, E.S. Katkin, Heartbeat detection and the experience of
Dr. Morgante is member of the advisory boards of UCB pharma, emotions, Cog. & Emotion 14 (3) (2000) 417–427.
Allergan and Lundbeck, has received honoraria from Allergan, [11] O. Pollatos, J. Füstös, Critchley HD, On the generalised embodiment of pain: how in-
teroceptive sensitivity modulates cutaneous pain perception, Pain 153 (8) (2012
Medtronic, UCB pharma, Lundbeck, Chiesi pharma, and serves in the ed- Aug) 1680–1686.
itorial board of Frontiers Neurology. [12] A. Tsay, T.J. Allen, U. Proske, et al., Sensing the body in chronic pain: a review of psy-
Prof Kailash P Bhatia: Funding for travel from GlaxoSmithKline, chophysical studies implicating altered body representation, Neurosci. Biobehav.
Rev. 52 (2015 May) 221–232.
Orion Corporation, Ipsen, and Merz Pharmaceuticals, LLC; serves on [13] Duschek S, Montoro CI, Reyes Del Paso GA. Diminished interoceptive awareness in
the editorial boards of Movement Disorders and TherapeuticAdvances fibromyalgia syndrome. Behav. Med. 2015 Oct 2:0.
in Neurological Disorders; receives royalties from the publication of Ox- [14] A.J. Hughes, S.E. Daniel, L. Kilford, et al., Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic
Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases, J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
ford Specialist Handbook of Parkinson's Disease and Other Movement Psychiatry 55 (3) (1992) 181–184.
Disorders (Oxford University Press, 2008); received speaker honoraria [15] Z.S. Nasreddine, N.A. Phillips, V. Bédirian, et al., The Montreal Cognitive Assessment,
from GlaxoSmithKline, Ipsen, Merz Pharmaceuticals, LLC, and Sun Phar- MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment, J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53
(4) (2005 Apr) 695–699.
maceutical Industries Ltd.; personal compensation for scientific adviso-
[16] A. Schrag, P. Barone, R.G. Brown, et al., Depression rating scales in Parkinson's
ry board for GSK and Boehringer Ingelheim; received research support disease: critique and recommendations, Mov. Disord. 22 (8) (2007 Jun 15)
from Ipsen and from the Halley Stewart Trust through Dystonia Society 1077–1092.
UK, and the Wellcome Trust MRC strategic neurodegenerative disease [17] C.L. Tomlinson, R. Stowe, et al., Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency
reporting in Parkinson's disease, Mov. Disord. 25 (2010) 2649–2653.
initiative award (ref. number WT089698), a grant from the Dystonia Co- [18] S. Fahn, R. Elton, Members of the UPDRS Development Committee, in: S. Fahn, C.D.
alition and a grant from Parkinson's UK (ref. number G-10). Marsden, D.B. Calne, M. Goldstein (Eds.), The Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating
Prof Mark Edwards is funded by a fellowship awarded by the Nation- Scale, Recent developments in Parkinson's disease, 2, Macmillan Health Care Infor-
mation, Florham Park, NJ, 1987 153-63,-293-304.
al Institute for Health Research and is supported by researchers at the [19] K.R. Chaudhuri, P. Martinez-Martin, R.G. Brown, et al., The metric properties of a
National Institute for Health Research University College London Hospi- novel non-motor symptoms scale for Parkinson's disease: results from an inter-
tals Biomedical Research Centre. He receives grant support from national pilot study, Mov. Disord. 22 (13) (2007 Oct 15) 1901–1911.
[20] R. Schandry, Heart beat perception and emotional experience, Psychophysiology 18
Parkinson's UK, the UK Dystonia Society, Bachmann Strauss Foundation. (4) (1981 Jul) 483–488.
He has received honoraria for speaking from The International Move- [21] R.M. Bagby, G.J. Taylor, J.D. Parker, The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-II:
ment Disorders Society and UCB Pharma. convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity, J. Psychosom. Res. 38 (1994)
33–40.
[22] G.J. Taylor, R.M. Bagby, J.D.A. Parker, The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–IV:
Authors' role reliability and factorial validity in different languages and cultures, J. Psychosom.
Res. 55 (2003) 277–283.
[23] M. Hamilton, A rating scale for depression, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23
1. Drafting/revising the manuscript for content, including medical writ- (1960) 56–62.
ing for content [24] M. Hamilton, The assessment of anxiety states by rating, Br. J. Med. Psychol. 32 (1)
(1959) 50–55.
2. Study concept or design [25] R.S. Marin, R.C. Biedrzycki, S. Firinciogullari, Reliability and validity of the Apathy
3. Analysis or interpretation of data Evaluation Scale, Psychiatry Res. 38 (2) (1991 Aug) 143–162.
[26] R.P. Snaith, M. Hamilton, S. Morley, et al., A scale for the assessment of hedonic
4. Acquisition of data tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale, Br. J. Psychiatry 167 (1) (1995 Jul)
5. Statistical analysis 99–103.
6. Study supervision or coordination [27] R. Ameli, D.A. Luckenbaugh, N.F. Gould, et al., SHAPS-C: the Snaith-Hamilton
pleasure scale modified for clinician administration, PeerJ 2 (2014 Jun 17),
LR: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6
e429.
GF: 4 [28] S. Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults
BD: 3, 4, 5 with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences, J.
FM: 1, 6 Autism Dev. Disord. 34 (2) (2004 Apr) 163–175.
[29] L.B. Krupp, N.G. LaRocca, J. Muir-Nash, et al., The fatigue severity scale. Application to
RE:1 patients with multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosus, Arch. Neurol. 46
CG:1 (10) (1989 Oct) 1121–1123.
KB:1 [30] C.G. Lange, W. James, The Emotions, Hafner publishing Co, New York/London,
1967.
AB:1 [32] H. Braak, C.M. Müller, U. Rüb, et al., Pathology associated with sporadic
MJE: 1, 2, 3, 6 Parkinson's disease–where does it end? J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 70 (2006)
89–97 Review.
[33] L.J. Chang, T. Yarkoni, M.W. Khaw, et al., Decoding the role of the insula in human
References cognition: functional parcellation and large-scale reverse inference, Cereb. Cortex
23 (3) (2013 March) 739–749.
[1] P. Martinez-Martin, C. Rodriguez-Blazquez, et al., NMSS Validation Group. The im- [34] S.N. Garfinkel, C. Tiley, S. O'Keeffe, et al., Discrepancies between dimensions of
pact of non-motor symptoms on health-related quality of life of patients with interoception in autism: implications for emotion and anxiety, Biol. Psychol. 114
Parkinson's disease, Mov. Disord. 26 (3) (2011 Feb 15) 399–406. (2016 Feb) 117–126.
[2] H.D. Critchley, S. Wiens, P. Rotshtein, et al., Neural systems supporting interoceptive [35] O. Pollatos, K. Gramann, R. Schandry, Neural systems connecting interoceptive
awareness, Nat. Neurosci. 7 (2004) 189–195. awareness and feelings, Hum. Brain Mapp. 28 (1) (2007 Jan) 9–18.

Downloaded from ClinicalKey.com at Rutgers University - NERL January 16, 2017.


For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L. Ricciardi et al. / Journal of the Neurological Sciences 364 (2016) 110–115 115

[36] L. Ricciardi, M. Bologna, F. Morgante, D. Ricciardi, B. Morabito, D. Volpe, D. Martino, [38] B. Wicker, C. Keysers, J. Plailly, et al., Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common
A. Tessitore, M. Pomponi, A.R. Bentivoglio, R. Bernabei, A. Fasano, Reduced facial ex- neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust, Neuron 40 (3) (2003) 655–664.
pressiveness in Parkinson's disease: a pure motor disorder? J. Neurol. Sci. 358 (1–2) [39] S. Pietracupa, A. Fasano, G. Fabbrini, et al., Poor self-awareness of levodopa-induced
(2015) 125–130. dyskinesias in Parkinson's disease: clinical features and mechanisms, Parkinsonism
[37] V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, et al., Action recognition in the premotor cortex, Relat. Disord. 19 (11) (2013 Nov) 1004–1008.
Brain 119 (Pt 2) (1996 Apr) 593–609.

Downloaded from ClinicalKey.com at Rutgers University - NERL January 16, 2017.


For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2017. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

También podría gustarte