Está en la página 1de 10

J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307

DOI 10.1007/s10803-012-1672-z

ORIGINAL PAPER

A Pilot Study on the Efficacy of Melodic Based Communication


Therapy for Eliciting Speech in Nonverbal Children with Autism
Givona A. Sandiford • Karen J. Mainess •

Noha S. Daher

Published online: 14 October 2012


Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012

Abstract The purpose of this study was to compare the Keywords Autism  Nonverbal  Children  Efficacy 
efficacy of Melodic Based Communication Therapy Treatment  Music
(MBCT) to traditional speech and language therapy for
eliciting speech in nonverbal children with autism. Partic-
ipants were 12 nonverbal children with autism ages 5 Introduction
through 7 randomly assigned to either treatment group.
Both groups made significant progress after treatment. The According to the Center for Disease Control and Prevention
MBCT group progressed significantly in number of verbal (2012), the prevalence of autism in the United States of
attempts after weeks 1 through 4 and number of correct America is one in 88 children, described as a 23 % increase
words after weeks 1 and 3, while the traditional group since the last report in 2009. With the number of children
progressed significantly after weeks 4 and 5. No significant with autism on the rise, the number of children with autism
differences in number of verbal attempts or number of who are nonverbal is also rising. It is estimated that 30–50 %
correct words were noted between groups following treat- of children with autism never develop functional speech
ment. A significant number of new words were heard in the (Pickett et al. 2009; Prizant and Wetherby 1993). Further-
home environment for the MBCT group (p = .04). Par- more, if a child with autism has not developed verbal speech
ticipants in the MBCT group had more imitative attempts by the age of five, it is generally agreed upon that the like-
(p = .03). MBCT appears to be a valid form of interven- lihood of him or her acquiring speech or language in the
tion for children with autism. future is significantly reduced (Charlop and Haymes 1994).
Though some research has indicated that speech and lan-
guage can be acquired after this critical age (Pickett et al.
2009) such evidence is limited in nature and often does not
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this clearly specify the methods used to promote such late
article (doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1672-z) contains supplementary speech/language acquisition (Pickett et al. 2009).
material, which is available to authorized users. Despite marked deficits in language development, there
is evidence to support that musical abilities in children with
G. A. Sandiford  K. J. Mainess  N. S. Daher
School of Allied Health Professionals, Loma Linda University, autism are often preserved (Altgassen et al. 2005; Brenton
Loma Linda, CA 92350, USA et al. 2008; Heaton 2009; Molnar-Szakacs and Heaton
K. J. Mainess 2012; Lai et al. 2012; Oimet et al. 2012). Additionally,
e-mail: kmainess@llu.edu research suggests abnormal interhemispheric asymmetry
N. S. Daher occurs in individuals with autism, with relatively lower
e-mail: ndaher@llu.edu than normal activation of the left hemisphere (Flagg et al.
2005; Herbert et al. 2005; Lazarev et al. 2010). Evidence
G. A. Sandiford (&)
has long supported the importance of the right hemisphere
c/o Karen J. Mainess at LLU SAHP, Communication Sciences
and Disorders Department, Loma Linda, CA 92350, USA of the brain in the processing of melody and rhythm
e-mail: silverpenlight@hotmail.com; gsandiford03a@llu.edu (Berman 1981; Callan et al. 2006; Hoffman 2008; Lo and

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307 1299

Fook-Chong 2004; Mostafa et al. 1989; Ono et al. 2011; intonation therapy in and of itself resulted in the increase in
Park et al. 2011; Szirony et al. 2007), while the left language since other factors such as maturation of the child
hemisphere of the brain is commonly associated with lan- and the introduction of traditional therapy prior to the
guage comprehension and use in the majority of individ- adapted melodic intonation therapy may have played a role
uals (Callan et al. 2006; Knecht et al. 2000; Pujol et al. in eventual language acquisition. It should also be noted
2002). If the right hemisphere in ASD is relatively spared, that the subject was under the age of 5, an age at which
it may be the case that right hemisphere functions such as acquisition of language is still considered probable.
rhythm and melody can serve as areas of strength and Another study of a 6-year-old nonverbal female with
sources of compensatory activity. autism found that the subject imitated a trombone by
The corpus callosum, which joins both hemispheres and grunting (Hoelzley 1993). The clinician continuously sang
allows for transfer of information between hemispheres, is utterances to the subject in tone with the trombone. The
often impaired in children with autism (Gozzi et al. 2012; subject moved from grunting to singing words to finally
Hardan et al. 2009; Keary et al. 2009; Shukla et al. 2010). singing phrases. After a year of treatment the subject was
However, studies conducted on the corpus callosum indi- observed to speak the utterances that had been sung to her
cate that it can be strengthened by exposure to music prior (Hoelzley 1993). Again, use of a single subject does not
to the age of seven in typically developing individuals allow for generalization of these results to the general
(Schlaug et al. 1995, 2009). Taking into account the population. Though this study was performed on an older
musical strengths of children with autism (Altgassen et al. subject, the lack of a control group makes it difficult to
2005; Brenton et al. 2008; Heaton et al. 2008; Heaton determine whether other factors such as maturation were
2009; Molnar-Szakacs and Heaton 2012; Lai et al. 2012; responsible for the improvement.
Oimet et al. 2012) as well as the overall benefits of music Another larger study utilized 50 subjects with autism
on the brain (Schlaug et al. 1995, 2009), it is clear that ages 3–5 (both low and high functioning). Subjects wat-
further research into music based interventions for treating ched 3 days of music treatment videos, speech treatment
individuals with autism is necessary. videos, or had no treatment. Improvements were seen in
Various forms of music therapy have been explored for both the speech and music treatment groups; however, low
children with autism in the past (Accordino et al. 2006; functioning individuals had the greatest gains with the
Finnigan and Starr 2010; Miller and Toca 1979; Starr and music treatments (Lim 2010). While this study does sup-
Zenker 1998; Wimpory et al. 1995). However these studies port the use of music as a viable source of treatment for
are often plagued with problems such as an insufficient young children with autism, it is unclear whether low
sample size rendering findings difficult to generalize to the functioning subjects were considered nonverbal. It should
population as a whole, poor study design making it difficult also be noted that the age range of children in this study
to determine what factors contributed most to the was 3–5, which is within the range for which normal
improvement of the subject, or poor description of therapy acquisition of verbal speech can still be expected. It can-
techniques resulting in an inability to reproduce the therapy not be known from this study whether older subjects who
in a clinical setting (Finnigan and Starr 2010; Lim 2010; are nonverbal would have made similar gains, particularly
Miller and Toca 1979; Hoelzley 1993). Additionally, these since the likelihood of acquiring language after the age of
studies were often completed on children below the age of 5 is commonly accepted as low (Charlop and Haymes
5, an age at which normal language acquisition is still 1994).
expected. Recent literature has addressed the theory of Auditory-
One such study tested a variation of melodic intonation Motor Mapping Training (AMMT), an intervention using
therapy (MIT) on a 3-year-old nonverbal male with autism music and rhythm to facilitate speech (Wan et al. 2010a, b,
(Miller and Toca 1979). The male subject received 1 year 2011; Wan and Schlaug 2010). In their 2011 proof of
of traditional therapy involving signed and verbal language concept study, researchers used a single-subject design to
with little to no improvement at which time adapted study six nonverbal children between the ages of 5 and 9.
melodic intonation therapy was attempted. The adapted For the purpose of the study, nonverbal was defined as
melodic intonation therapy made use of signed language as having no intelligible words. The clinician introduced a
well as an intoned stimulus. After treatment, the subject target high frequency word or phrase by singing while
was noted to use trained, imitative and spontaneous intoned simultaneously tapping out a matching rhythm and pitch on
verbal attempts that were observed to generalize to other a set of tuned drums five times a week over an eight-week
settings (Miller and Toca 1979). Despite the success of this period of time. Researchers found that subjects improved in
case study, the use of a single subject cannot be generalized their ability to articulate words and phrases over time. This
to the greater population, nor can it be determined without was noted to generalize to words not trained in therapy
the use of a control group whether the adapted melodic (Wan et al. 2011). While these results are promising and

123
1300 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307

continue to indicate the need for more music-based inter- Method


ventions, lack of a control group makes it difficult to
determine whether a cause-effect relationship exists Participants
between treatment and outcome and whether other factors
such as maturation of the child or the intensity of the Permission to conduct this study was obtained from the
particular intervention played a role in the outcome. Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Loma Linda Univer-
Melodic Based Communication Therapy (MBCT) is a sity (LLU) ensuring appropriate adherence to informed
novel treatment developed by the first author based on consent procedures and handling of all research data.
experiences within the field and a review of the literature, Participants were recruited from the southern California
which, in theory, proposes to make use of the presumed area using local media/newspapers, letters to paraprofes-
musical strengths of the child with autism in order to sionals, flyers in local clinics, hospitals, universities,
increase verbal output. MBCT differs from other music schools, contacting of local support groups, social networks
therapies such as AAMT in that it utilizes a standard and word of mouth. Participants were included in the study
melody for each target word. Standardization of the melody if they had a past diagnosis of autism as well as a clinical
is made possible by use of a pre-recorded melody. Each diagnosis of autism based on the Autism Diagnostic
melody is specific to its target word and differs signifi- Observational Schedule (ADOS). In addition to a diagnosis
cantly from the other melodies. In addition to a standard of autism, participants needed to be nonverbal. Nonverbal
melody for each target word, each step is clearly defined, as was defined as having an expressive vocabulary of no more
is the reinforcement schedule. Additionally, items used for than 10 words which were not used on a daily basis and
reinforcement are highly specific to each child. While having no functional speech. Participants also needed to be
AAMT and other musical therapies often make use of other between the ages of 5 and 7 years. Individuals were
instruments such as the trombone or tuned drums, which excluded from the study if they were receiving other lan-
can be difficult or expensive to obtain and may require a guage or articulation treatments or therapy at the time of
high level of musical ability or years of training, MBCT the study, were unable to regularly attend four 45 min
uses pre-recorded instrumentals to accompany the recorded sessions of therapy weekly for 5 weeks, had a history of
melody, allowing the clinician to press ‘‘play’’ on a com- severe hearing impairment, had severe visual impairment/
pact disc player to begin. Additionally, unlike the previ- blindness, had a diagnosis of an organic impairment of oral
ously mentioned therapies, which often, with the exception or laryngeal structures, or had a significant medical illness
of Wan et al. omit rhythm as a component of musical or condition which would prevent the child’s participation
therapy, pre-recorded melodies are initially accompanied in the treatment procedures. These conditions included, but
by rhythmic clapping by the clinician and child. Clapping were not limited to cerebral palsy, paraplegia, spina bifida,
has been utilized as a source of rhythm by other researchers uncontrolled seizures, dysarthria, and amputation of
in the past (Kelly 2001; Matsuyama 2005; Olander et al. arm(s). Twelve participants who resided in the Southern
2010). Additionally clapping has been found, when com- California area were included in the study, 11 males and 1
pared to other motor tasks, such as sequential finger female. The participants included 5 seven year olds, 3 six
movements and grasp release movements, to be a more year olds, and 4 five year olds. Per parent report, partici-
significant cortical activator (Kim et al. 2011) as well as a pants had all received a minimum of 1 year of speech
way of significantly improving the syllable production of therapy prior to entering the study with a range of
words in children with learning disabilities (Jackson et al. 1–6 years of therapy depending on the participant. Each
1997). participant had been previously seen by up to four speech
Because of the great push for evidence-based practice pathologists prior to entering the study. See Table 1 for a
(http://www.asha.org/policy), the need to explore whether description of the participants’ characteristics.
Melodic Based Communication Therapy is an effective
form of intervention for nonverbal children with autism Measures
through extensive research is apparent. This is a pilot study
on the efficacy of MBCT. The purpose of this study was to The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), a
compare the efficacy of Melodic Based Communication semi-structured, standardized assessment of communica-
Therapy (MBCT) to traditional therapy for eliciting speech tion, social interaction, and play consisting of four mod-
in nonverbal children with autism. For the purposes of this ules was used to verify the diagnosis of autism (Lord et al.
paper, efficacy was assessed by: number of verbal attempts, 1989, 1999, 2000; Overton et al. 2008). The second
number of correct words, number of new words reported by author, a speech language pathologist trained and certified
the parent in the home environment, and number of imi- to administer the ADOS, administered Module 1 to the
tative attempts. study participants with the assistance of graduate students

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307 1301

Table 1 Mean ± SD of descriptive data for the participants (N = 12)


Age in years Gender # words reported by parents ADOS (algorithm scores)
Communication Reciprocal social interaction Comm. ? social interaction total

5.83 ± 0.8 1 Female 2.3 ± 2.7 7.8 ± 2 11.4 ± 2 19.3 ± 3.3


(5.0–7.6) 11 Males (0–9) (4–10) (7–14) (16–24)
For ADOS communication score the maximum score is a 10. The higher the child’s score the greater the level of impairment. The cutoff score for
autism is 4. For reciprocal social interaction score, the maximum score is 14. The higher the child’s score the greater the level of impairment. The
autism cut off score is 7. For the combined score the maximum score is 24 and the autism cutoff is 12 (Lord et al. 1989)

in the Communication Sciences and Disorders program at by a list of their child’s top three reinforcers. The parent
LLU. survey was also used to tally the number of words reported
Outcome measures for this study were: number of verbal by the parent prior to and following the completion of
attempts, number of correct words, number of words treatment. Parents were asked to list the words he/she had
reported by the parent, and number of imitative attempts. In heard the child say in the home environment.
order to measure number of verbal attempts and number of Imitative attempts were tallied based on a review of the
correct words over time, a criterion referenced vocabulary video recordings from the first and final treatment sessions.
test developed by the first author for the purposes of this Any attempt to verbally imitate the clinician was scored as
study was given at baseline and the beginning of each one imitative attempt. Imitative attempts for the first 10
treatment week. The criterion referenced vocabulary test trials were tallied for every participant’s first and last
was given again at the close of the last treatment week. session.
Score sheets for baseline and weekly criterion referenced
vocabulary testing were used to document the participants’ Research Design and Procedures
responses over time using the International Phonetic
Alphabet (IPA). The criterion referenced vocabulary test A randomized control design was used in order to determine
was conducted as follows: the clinician would pull one of the effectiveness of the experimental treatment and control
the stimulus items from a bin and ask the participant, for external factors such as maturation of the participants and
‘‘What is this?’’ The participant’s response was then tran- the intensity of the administered therapy. Upon entering the
scribed verbatim using the International Phonetic Alphabet study, children were put into three groups according to their
(IPA). Clinicians completing the criterion referenced age, and then randomly assigned to one of two treatment
vocabulary test were not blinded to the type of therapy the groups. The two groups were: the traditional therapy group,
child was receiving, as they were the same clinicians which represented the standard therapy procedures the par-
completing the therapy. Clinicians were however blinded ticipant would receive in a private practice setting and the
as to the method used to score criterion referenced testing. Melodic Based Communication Therapy (MBCT) group,
Criterion referenced testing was videotaped and 40 % of which represented the experimental group. Participants who
the videos later reviewed at random by the first and second joined late were randomly assigned to either group (see
author separately in order to ensure for accuracy. Fig. 1 for a flowchart on the movement of participants
The first and second author scored criterion referenced through the study). Participants received 5 weeks of inter-
testing using the following procedures: verbal attempts vention, with four 45 min individual sessions a week.
which were close approximations to the target word such Twenty-five target words were chosen based on high
as/hæ/for/hænd/were scored as correct words. Verbal frequency words children typically use first (Appendix A).
attempts which were nonsensical and bore little resem- Twenty-five stimulus items were chosen to represent the
blance to the target word such as/owio/for/hænd/were tal- target words. Target words and stimulus items were the
lied under verbal attempts. See Appendix D for an example same for each group. The goal of therapy for each treat-
of the criterion referenced vocabulary testing score sheet ment group was to train the production of the twenty-five
and instructions used by the reviewers. target words. If the twenty-five target words were learned,
A parent survey was compiled based on the information then two word utterances incorporating the twenty-five
needed for inclusion/exclusion criteria as well as the target words were then trained (e.g. ‘‘kick ball’’).
information needed for a reinforcer assessment and Clinicians were first year graduate students in the
assessment of the child’s current vocabulary. The rein- Communication Sciences and Disorders program at LLU
forcer assessment was a modification of Sturmey’s rein- trained extensively to provide both traditional therapy and
forcer assessment (2008), which requires parents to list Melodic Based Communication Therapy under the super-
possible reinforcers with corresponding examples followed vision of the first and second authors. Treatment

123
1302 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307

Fig. 1 Sampling and flow of


Assessed for eligibility
participants through randomized
(n=15)
control rrial
Total including late joiners
(n=17)

Excluded for
Enrollment not meeting
criteria (n=3)

Grouped/Blocked into category by age


then Randomized (n=12) Joined 1 week
late (n=1)

Joined 2 weeks
Allocated to MBCT late (n=1)
Allocated to
group (n=6)
traditional group
Allocation (n=6) Allocated to MBCT Randomly
group after joining
assigned to
late (n=2)
either group
(n=2)

Week 1 (n=5) Week 1 (n=6)


Week 2 (n=5) Week 2 (n=7)
Week 3 (n=5) Week 3 (n=8)
Follow -up Week 4 (n=5)
Week 4 (n=8)
Week 5 (n=5)
Week 5 (n=7)

Total lost to follow Total lost to


up (n=1) follow up (n=1)

Analyzed (n=5) Analyzed Pre/Post measures (n=7)


Excluded (n=1) Excluded from analysis (n=1)
Reason: dropped after 3 Reason: serious illness
days
Analysis Analyzed weekly measures (n=5)
Excluded from weekly analysis due to late
Parent Report (n=3) joining (n=2)
Excluded due to no report
given (n=2) Parent Report (n=5)
Excluded due to no report given (n=2)

procedures for the traditional therapy consisted of using the Procedures for MBCT were similar to the traditional
child’s three primary reinforcers to train and reward correct therapy in that the same 25 words and stimulus items were
productions as well as attempts to name stimulus items. used. The child’s top three reinforcers were also used to
See Appendix E for a list of the reinforcers used in this provide rewards for correct responses and attempts during
study. The clinician stated the word clearly while holding therapy. The children were allowed to listen to a CD
up the stimulus item, then asked the child to repeat the recording of the word set to melody while the therapist
word after him/her. Acceptable cues were: phonemic cues, presented the stimulus item to the child simultaneously. No
manual manipulation of lips/articulators, visual cues for two melodies were the same. Therapy then progressed from
placement and verbal instructions about where and how to listening to a recording of the word set to melody, to hand
use articulators. Therapy progressed from having the child over hand clapping of the rhythm, to unison clapping of the
imitate nouns and verbs to asking the child to indepen- rhythm, to independent clapping of the rhythm, to inde-
dently name the items in response to the question: What is pendent clapping of the rhythm while singing to the
this? If the child was able to name the majority of the recording with the clinician, to singing with just the cli-
words independently, the procedures were repeated for two nician while clapping, to singing with just the clinician
word utterances such as ‘‘kick ball.’’ See Appendix B for a without clapping, to singing while the clinician mouthed
complete list of procedures used for traditional therapy. the word silently, to singing the word independently, to

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307 1303

answering the sung question, ‘‘What is this?’’ with the For the number of correct words, the participants in the
melodic version of the expected target word, to answering MBCT group had significant progress after week one (z =
the spoken question, ‘‘What is this?’’ with the expected -2.2, p = .03) and week three (z = -2.0, p = .05). For
target word. If the child was able to name the majority of participants in the traditional group, however, the number of
the words independently, then the same procedures were correct words increased significantly only after week four
followed for two word utterances. See Appendix C for a (z = -2.0, p = .04) and week five (z = -2.0, p = .04).
complete list of instructions used for MBCT. There was no significant difference, however, in number of
correct words between the two groups at the completion of
therapy (z = -0.2, p = .40) (see Table 3). Parents reported
Results a significant improvement in number of new words heard in
the home and other environments for participants in the
Data was summarized using descriptive statistics. A two- MBCT group (z = -2.0, p = .04). On the other hand, there
way mixed factorial ANOVA (2 groups 9 6 times) was were no significant changes in the number of new words
used to examine changes between the two groups over heard in the homes of the participants in the traditional group
time. The outcome measures for the two groups were (z = -1.6, p = .11). When between group comparisons
compared using the Mann–Whitney U Test at weekly were made, however, no significant difference was noted
intervals. Changes in outcome variables over time were between groups for number of new words heard in the home
assessed using Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test. Intraclass environment (z = -.75, p = .45) (see Table 3). For number
correlation coefficient was used to examine the inter-rater of imitative attempts, the participants in the MBCT group
reliability. Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the showed significantly greater overall gains than the traditional
proportions of males/females between the two groups. The group (z = -2.2, p = .03) (see Fig. 2).
level of significance was set at p \ .05. Reviewers demonstrated high agreement for all mea-
Fourteen participants were originally included in the sures taken. For number of verbal attempts (Intra class
study; however, one dropped out after a few days and the correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.96, p \ .001), for num-
other was not readily available for testing due to illness. As ber of correct words ((ICC) = 0.71, p \ .001) and for
shown in Fig. 1, the traditional group had 5 participants. The number of imitations (ICC = 0.96, p \ .001).
MBCT group had 7 participants. Two participants in the
MBCT group, however, did not receive the full 5 weeks of
therapy due to later enrollment in the study. All participants Discussion
received at least 3 weeks of therapy. Ten participants
received 5 weeks of therapy, five participants in the tradi- The aim of this study was to compare the efficacy of
tional group and five participants in the MBCT group (see Melodic Based Communication Therapy (MBCT) and
Fig. 1 for the flow of participants through each stage of the traditional therapy in eliciting speech in nonverbal children
experiment). No significant differences were found between with autism. Efficacy was evaluated by number of verbal
the two groups prior to therapy for age, number of verbal attempts, number of correct words, number of words
attempts, number of correct words, number of words repor- reported by the parent in the home environment and
ted by the parent, or ADOS scores (see Table 2). number of imitative attempts. While both therapies were
Both treatment groups made significant progress in found to be effective at the completion of the study, results
number of verbal attempts (F5,40 = 6.9, p \ .001), number suggest a possible faster rate of improvement for the
of correct words (F5,40 = 4.1, p = .04), and number of MBCT group as well as greater overall gains in verbal
imitative attempts following treatment (z = -2.5, p = .01). attempts and imitative attempts.
In the MBCT group number of verbal attempts increased These findings seem to lend further support to the
significantly after week one (z = -2.4, p = .02), week two positive effects of music-based interventions previously
(z = -2.0, p = .04), week three (z = -2.0, p = .04), and found by multiple researchers (Finnigan and Starr 2010;
week four (z = -2.2, p = .03). The participants in the tra- Lim 2010; Miller and Toca 1979; Hoelzley 1993; Wan
ditional group, however, had significant progress in number et al. 2011). Furthermore, parents within the MBCT group
of verbal attempts only after week four (z = -2.0, p = .04) reported a significant increase in number of both taught and
and week five (z = -2.1, p = .04). The difference between untaught words in the home environment, lending some
number of verbal attempts expressed by the traditional group support to other recent findings regarding the generaliza-
and number of verbal attempts expressed by the MBCT tion of skills taught using music to production of words not
group at the completion of therapy approached statistical taught during therapy (Wan et al. 2011).
significance, with the MBCT group displaying more verbal The faster rate of improvement for the MBCT group,
attempts overall (z = -1.4, p = .08). greater overall gains in verbal attempts and imitative

123
1304 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307

Table 2 Median (min, max) of participant baseline characteristics et al. 2005; Herbert et al. 2005; Lazarev et al. 2010), and
(N = 12) melody and rhythm are housed in the right hemisphere of
Traditional MBCT p value** the brain, as research has indicated, (Berman 1981; Callan
(n = 5) (n = 7) et al. 2006; Hoffman 2008; Lo and Fook-Chong 2004;
Mostafa et al. 1989; Ono et al. 2011; Park et al. 2011;
Male n=5 n=6 0.58***
Szirony et al. 2007) then it may be expected that individ-
Age* 5.8 (.8) 5.9 (.9) 0.93
uals with autism might show an affinity or preference for
Number of verbal attempts 11 (0, 22) 8 (3, 18) 0.75
tasks relating to music and rhythm. This potential affinity
Number of correct words 0 (0, 9) 3 (0, 5) 0.15
for tasks pertaining to music and rhythm may explain the
Number of words reported 5 (0, 8) 8 (0, 10) 0.34
by parent
immediate progress of those in the MBCT group where
Number of words imitated 3 (0, 9) 1 (0, 5) 0.43
melody and rhythmic clapping were employed daily. Fur-
thermore, research indicating an increase in fibers in the
ADOS communication 9 (6, 10) 7 (4, 10) 0.56
score corpus callosum in those exposed to music, potentially
ADOS social interaction 11 (7, 13) 12 (10, 14) 0.16 allowing for better transfer of information between hemi-
score spheres (Schlaug et al. 1995, 2009), may further explain the
ADOS total score 17 (16, 23) 20 (16, 24) 0.56 initial progress for the MBCT group, particularly as the
* Results are reported as mean (SD)
corpus callosum has been cited as an area of impairment in
children with autism (Gozzi et al. 2012; Hardan et al. 2009;
** Mann–Whitney U test
Keary et al. 2009; Shukla et al. 2010).
*** Fisher’s exact test
Although the MBCT group initially showed greater
MBCT melodic based communication therapy
progress, a plateau effect was noted after 4 weeks of
therapy resulting in an overall lack of difference between
Table 3 Median (min, max) differences [Difference = post–pre] treatment groups. The lack of a difference in overall
between groups in verbal attempts, correct words and words reported number of words and number of verbal attempts during
by parents over time by type of therapy for all subjects criterion referenced testing over the full 5-week period may
Traditional MBCT p value** indicate a need for a change in the target words or trained
melodies after a period of 3–4 weeks has passed in order to
Verbal attempts 2 (-1, 16) 12 (7, 22) 0.08
continue the desired result. Further research implementing
Correct words 5 (1, 6) 5 (-1, 22) 0.40
the use of new melodies after this period may be necessary
Words reported by parent 6 (2, 23) 13 (5, 51) 0.45 to examine the full benefits of MBCT. In addition, research
** 2 tailed test; MBCT melodic based communication therapy looking into initial treatment with MBCT followed by the
use of traditional speech and language therapy after week 4
may also be warranted to determine if using MBCT ini-
tially will produce a spike in the therapeutic success of
traditional speech and language therapy when traditional
therapy is used as a secondary treatment.
The finding that the number of verbal attempts in the
MBCT group at the close of treatment increased in a
manner approaching significance when compared to the
traditional group, while number of correct words did not,
was noteworthy. This may imply that melody and rhythm
may also be factors in increasing the number of intentional
non-word attempts a child with autism makes. Previous
research has addressed the importance of shaping such non-
word verbal attempts into words while treating children
Fig. 2 Median number of words imitated pre and post treatment by
type of treatment with autism (Dawson 1989; Newman et al. 2009). The lack
of a difference in number of correct words between groups
attempts as well as the significant improvement in number following treatment, may indicate the need to spend less
of words said in the home environment may have been due time reinforcing for verbal attempts and more time care-
to an increased motivation of the child to complete the fully shaping verbal attempts into actual words (e.g.,/e/ to /
therapeutic tasks when melody and rhythm were imple- be/ to / bed/) as suggested by Newman et al. 2009.
mented. If individuals with autism demonstrate right Both treatment groups showed progress in a population
hemisphere strengths, as multiple studies suggest (Flagg that is often difficult to treat. This may have been due to the

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307 1305

intensity of the treatment and the highly specific nature of benefit of music-based interventions for children with
the reinforcers. Reinforcers in general have been shown to autism who are nonverbal, there may be a need for speech
assist in the learning of many behaviors in children with language pathologists to implement MBCT and/or other
autism (Koegel et al. 2009, 2010; Sturmey 2008; Volkert music based therapies in clinical and public school settings.
et al. 2008). The importance of intensity of the interven- Additionally should research continue to support the ben-
tions may warrant further investigation. efits of music-based interventions for this population, uni-
versities may wish to offer more training in music-based
Limitations and Future Research interventions to students enrolled in their speech pathology
programs.
Sample size may have been a limitation of the study. While
studies on children with autism, particularly nonverbal Acknowledgments Funding from the Communication Sciences and
Disorders and Rehabilitation Sciences Departments at Loma Linda
children, are often small due to inherent difficulties in University supported this research. We are grateful to the families
recruiting, testing and treating this population, a larger who participated in this research and to the talented and patient Loma
sample size would have allowed for greater generalization Linda University Communication Sciences and Disorders graduate
of outcomes to the population at large and greater power students who were involved in the providing of therapy, Brittany
Masai, Jordan Shimamura, Ryan Forgette, Christine DerDanielian,
overall. Seventeen children were assessed for eligibility Jennifer Diaz, and Cassie Richardson. We also thank the Sachs
overall, however due to the specificity of the inclusion and Norton Clinic for the use of their speech therapy rooms for the
exclusion criteria only fourteen were included in the study, duration of the study. Special thanks to pediatric neurologist
two of whom were lost to follow up. Dr. Sarah Roddy and neuroscientist Dr. Ernie Schwab for their
invaluable advice on the neurology of the brain as well as their
Lack of follow-up was another limitation of this study. supportive role in planning and designing this study. Special thanks,
Although all parents initially provided the number of words as well, to statistician Dr. Grenith Zimmerman for her invaluable
heard at home as well as a list of those words, not all support in the design and planning phases of this study. Special thanks
parents at the close of the study reported this information. to musicians Alan-Pierre Eloi (Alleykat Entertainment Inc.) and
Shannon Hicks, for their role in making MBCT a reality. Special
In both groups, 2 parents failed to provide a follow up thanks to the first author’s family and friends for their support and
report on words heard in the home environment, providing prayers throughout the duration of this study. God is able.
a smaller sample size for comparison between groups.
Additionally while attempts were made to follow up
with participants a few months after the study to determine
the progress of the child over time via a survey, only 3 References
participants in the MBCT group responded and none in the
Accordino, R., Comer, R., & Heller, W. B. (2006). Searching for
traditional group responded. Due to the limited number of music’s potential: A critical examination of research on music
responses and lack of a response from participants in the therapy with individuals with Autism. Research in Autism
control group no analyses was completed. Spectrum Disorders, 1(1), 101–115.
While both therapies were shown to work in an intensive Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., & Williams, T. (2005). Pitch perception in
children with autistic spectrum disorders. British Journal of
one-on-one, 45 min, 4 days per week setting, research Developmental Psychology, 23(4), 543–558.
should be conducted comparing the benefits of MBCT to American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2005). Evidence-
that of traditional therapy at a lower intensity level. In based practice in communication disorders [Position Statement].
addition, with the rise in the cost of treatment and health- Available from www.asha.org/policy.
Berman, I. W. (1981). Musical functioning, speech lateralization and
care for children with autism, (Wang and Leslie 2010) the Amusias. South African Medical Journal, 59(3), 78–81.
further research into the effectiveness of using MBCT in Brenton, J. N., Devries, S. P., Barton, C., Minnich, H., & Sokol, D. K.
the home environment as conducted by a parent or family (2008). Absolute pitch in a four-year-old boy with autism.
member may be warranted. Pediatric Neurology, 39(2), 137–138.
Callan, D. E., Tsytsarev, V., Hanakawa, T., Callan, A. M., Katsuhara,
While speech therapy is often the norm for children with M., Fukuyama, H., et al. (2006). Song and speech: Brain regions
autism, little research supports its efficacy for this popu- involved with perception and covert production. Neuroimage,
lation (Weber and Newmark 2007). Preliminary findings of 31(3), 1327–1342.
this randomized control study suggest that both MBCT and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2012). Preva-
lence of Autism spectrum disorders–Autism and developmental
traditional speech therapy are effective for children with disabilities monitoring network, United States, 2008. Morbidity
autism. While results seemed to indicate both therapies and mortal weekly report (MMWR), Vol. 61(3).
were effective, MBCT appeared to provide more rapid Charlop, M. H. & Haymes, L. K. (1994). Speech and language
results as well as greater overall gains in verbal attempts intervention: Behavioral approaches. In. J. L. Matson (Ed.),
Autism in children and adults: Etiology, assessment, and
and imitative attempts. These findings may indicate the intervention (pp. 213–24). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
need for additional research into music-based interven- Dawson, G. (Ed.). (1989) Autism: Nature, Diagnosis, and Treatment.
tions. Should further research continue to support the New York: The Guilford Press.

123
1306 J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307

Finnigan, E., & Starr, E. (2010). Increasing social responsiveness in a Lo, Y. L., & Fook-Chong, S. (2004). Ipsilateral and contralateral
child with autism. A comparison of music and non-music motor inhibitory control in musical and vocalization tasks.
interventions. Autism, 14(4), 321–348. Experimental Brain Research, 159(2), 258–262.
Flagg, E. J., Cardy, J. E., Roberts, W., & Roberts, T. P. (2005). Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leventhal, B., DiLavore,
Language lateralization development in children with autism: P., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-
Insights from the late field magnetoencephalogram. Neurosci- generic: A standard measure of social and communication
ence Letters, 386(2), 82–87. deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of
Gozzi, M., Nielson, D. M., Lenroot, R. K., Ostuni, J. L. Luckenbaugh, Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.
D. A., Thurm, A. E., Giedd, J. N. & Swedo, S. E. (2012). Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism
Biological Psychiatry, [Epub ahead of print]. diagnostic observational schedule (ADOS). Western Psycholog-
Hardan, A. Y., Pabalan, M., Gupta, N., Bansal, R., Melhem, N. M., ical Services.
Fedorov, S., et al. (2009). Corpus Callosum volume in children Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H.,
with Autism. Psychiatry Research, 174(1), 57–61. Mahwood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation
Heaton, P. (2009). Assessing musical skills in autistic children who schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and
are not savants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 364(1522), 1443–1447. ders, 19(2), 185–212.
Heaton, P., Davis, R. E., & Happe, G. E. (2008). Exceptional absolute Matsuyama, K. (2005). Correlation between musical responsiveness
pitch perception for spoken words in an able adult with autism. and developmental age among early age children as assessed by
Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 2095–2098. the Non-Verbal Measurement of the Musical Responsiveness of
Herbert, M. R., Ziegler, D. A., Deutsch, C. K., O’Brien, L. M., Children. Medical Science Monitor: International Medical
Kennedy, D. N., Filipek, P. A., et al. (2005). Brain asymmetries Journal of Experimental and Clinical Research, 11(10),
in autism and developmental language disorder: A nested whole- 485–492.
brain analysis. Brain, 128(1), 213–226. Miller, S. B., & Toca, J. M. (1979). Adapted melodic intonation
Hoelzley, P. D. (1993). Communication potentiating sounds: Devel- therapy: A case study of an experimental language program for
oping channels of communication with autistic children through an autistic child. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 40(4),
psychobiological responses to novel sound stimuli. Canadian 201–2013.
Journal of Music Therapy, 1(1), 54–76. Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Heaton, P. (2012). Muic: A unique window
Hoffman, M. (2008). Isolated right temporal lobe stroke patients into the world of autism. Annals of the New York Academy of
present with Geschwind Gastaut syndrome, frontal network Sciences, 1252(1), 318–324.
syndrome and delusional misidentification syndromes. Behav- Mostafa, M., Kotby, M. N., Barakah, M., el-Sady, S., Allosh, T.,
ioral Neurology, 20(3), 83–89. Elshobary, A., et al. (1989). Dominant functions of right versus
Jackson, S. A., Treharne, D. A., & Boucher, J. (1997). Rhythm and the left hemisphere. Acta Otolaryngolica, 107(5–6), 479–484.
language in children with moderate learning difficulties. Euro- Newman, B., Reinke, D., & Ramos, M. (2009). Is a reasonable
pean Journal of Disorders of Communication: The Journal of the attempt reasonable? Shaping versus reinforcing verbal attempts
College of Speech and Language Therapists, London, 32(1), of preschoolers with autsim. The Analysis of verbal behavior, 25,
99–108. 67–72.
Keary, C. J., Minshew, N. J., Bansal, R., Goradia, D., Fedorov, S., Oimet, T., Foster, N. E., Tryfon, A., & Hyde, K. L. (2012). Auditory-
Keshavan, M. S., et al. (2009). Corpus callosum volume and musical processing in autism spectrum disorders: A review of
neurocognition in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental behavioral and brain imaging studies. Annals of the New York
Disorders, 39(6), 834–841. Academy of Sciences, 1252, 325–331.
Kelly, K. (2001). They’ve got rhythm. Clapping away attention Olander, L., Smith, A., & Zelaznik, H. N. (2010). Evidence that a
deficits? U.S. News & World Report, 130(15), 50. motor timing deficit is a factor in the development of stuttering.
Kim, M. J., Hong, J. H., & Jang, S. H. (2011). The cortical effect of Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(4),
clapping on the human brain: A functional MRI study. 876–886.
NeuroRehabilitation, 28(2), 75–79. Ono, K., Nakamura, A., Yoshiyama, K., Kinkori, T., Bundo, M.,
Knecht, S., Drager, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Floel, A., Kato, T., & Ito, K. (2011). The effect of musical experience on
et al. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance hemispheric lateralization in musical feature processing. Neuro-
in healthy humans. Brain, 123, 2512–2518. science Letters. [Epub ahead of print].
Koegel, L. K., Singh, A. K., & Koegel, R. L. (2010). Improving Overton, T., Fielding, C., & de Alba, R. G. (2008). Brief Report:
motivation for academics in children with autism. Journal of exploratory analysis of the ADOS revised algorithm: Specificity
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), 1057–1066. and predictive value with Hispanic children referred for autism
Koegel, R. L., Vernon, T. W., & Koegel, L. K. (2009). Improving spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
social initiations in young children with autism using reinforcers Disorders, 38(6), 1166–1169.
with embedded social interactions. Journal of Autism and Park, J. Y., Park, H., Kim, J. I., & Park, H. J. (2011). Consonant
Developmental Disorders, 39(9), 1240–1251. chords stimulate higher EEG gamma activity than dissonant
Lai, G., Pantazatos, S. P., Schneider, H., & Hirsch, J. (2012). Neural chords. Neuroscience Letters, 488(1), 101–105.
systems for speech and song in autism. Brain, 135(Pt.3), Pickett, E., Pullara, O., O’Grady, J., & Gordon, B. (2009). Speech
961–975. acquisition in older nonverbal individuals with Autism: A review
Lazarev, V. V., Pontes, A., Mitrofanov, A. A., & deAzevedo, L. C. of features, methods and prognosis. Cognitive and Behavioral
(2010). Interhemispheric asymmetry in EEG photic driving Neurology, 22(1), 1–22.
coherence in childhood autism. Clinical Neurophysiology, Prizant, B. M., & Wetherby, A. M. (1993). Communication in
121(2), 145–152. preschool autistic children. In E. Schopler, M. Van Bourgandien,
Lim, H. A. (2010). Effect of ‘‘developmental speech and language & M. Bristol (Eds.), Preschool issues in autism (pp. 95–128).
training through music’’ on speech production in children with New York: Plenum Press.
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Music Therapy, 47(1), Pujol, J., Lopez-Sala, A., Deus, J., Cardoner, N., Sebastian-Galles, N.,
2–26. Conesa, G., et al. (2002). The lateral asymmetry of the human

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43:1298–1307 1307

brain studied by volumetric magnetic resonance imaging. training as an intervention to facilitate speech output in non-
Neuroimage, 17(2), 670–679. verbal children with Autism: A proof of concept study. Public
Schlaug, G., Forgeard, M., Zhu, L., Norton, A., & Winner, E. (2009). Library of Science PLoS One, 6(9).
Training-induced neuroplasticity in young children. Annals of Wan, C. Y., Demaine, K., Zipse, L., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G.
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 205–208. (2010a). From music making to speaking: Engaging the mirror
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y., Staiger, J. F., & Steinmetz, H. neuron system in autism. Brain Research Bulletin, 82(3–4),
(1995). Increased corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuro- 161–168.
psychologia, 33, 1047–1055. Wan, C. Y., Ruber, T., Hohman, A., & Schlaug, G. (2010b). The
Shukla, D. K., Keehn, B., Lincoln, A. J., & Muller, R. (2010). White therapeutic effects of singing on neurological disorders. Music
matter compromise of callosal and subcortical fiber tracts in Perception, 27(4), 287–295.
children with autism spectrum disorder: A diffusion tensor Wan, C. Y., & Schlaug, G. (2010). Neural pathways for language in
imaging study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & autism: The potential for music based treatments. Special report.
Adolescent Psychiatry 49(12), 1269–1278.e2. Future Neurology, 5(6), 797–805.
Starr, E., & Zenker, E. (1998). Understanding autism in the context of Wang, L., & Leslie, D. L. (2010). Health care expenditures for
music therapy: Bridging theory and practice. Canadian Journal children with autism spectrum disorders in Medicaid. Journal of
of Music Therapy, 6, 1–19. the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
Sturmey, P. (2008). How to teach verbal behavior. Austin, TX: Proed 49(11), 1165–1171.
Inc. Weber, W., & Newmark, S. (2007). Complementary and alternative
Szirony, G. M., Pearson, L. C., Burgin, J. S., Murray, G. C., & Elrod, medical therapies for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and
L. M. (2007). Brain hemisphere dominance and vocational autism. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 54(6), 983–1006.
preference: A preliminary analysis. Work, 29(4), 323–329. Wimpory, D., Chadwick, P., & Nash, S. (1995). Brief report: musical
Volkert, V. M., Lerman, D. C., Trosclair, N., Addison, L., & Kodak, interaction therapy for children with autism: An evaluative case
T. (2008). An exploratory analysis of task-interspersal proce- study with two-year follow-up. Journal of Autism and Develop-
dures while teaching object labels to children with autism. mental Disorders, 25(5), 541–552.
Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 41(3), 335–350.
Wan, C. Y., Bazen, L., Baars, R., Libenson, A., Zipse, L., Zuk, J.,
Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2011) Auditory-motor mapping

123

También podría gustarte