Está en la página 1de 12

The Evolution of the Domestic

Early Dog

Animal domestication, commonly considered a human innovation,


can also be describedas an evolutionaryprocess

Darcy F.Morey

within the past 12,000 or decision, as is commonly thought, or, presumption that those ends were
so
Sometime years, most of humankind began rather, is best modeled strictly as an sought, at least in rudimentary form,
to experience a profound shift in life? evolutionary process. by people of the past. According to this
style. Stone Age hunters and gatherers Those who explain domestication as view, people turned to the domestica?
of wild foodstuffs started to cultivate a rational decision suggest that people tion of plants and animals when increas?
plants and raise animals for their own recognized the potential benefits of es inhuman population or environmen?
use. A landscape full of wild grasses, bringing animals and plants under tal changes reduced the availability of
woolly mammoths and sabertooth cats control. The assumption is that people wild foods. Given these pressures, peo?
gave way to giant-eared corn, fat cattle, intentionally sought to raise, cultivate ple invented or otherwise made a deci?
toy poodles and many other new and manipulate organisms in ways sion to experiment with domestication,
species.
For reasons that remain ob? that enhanced their economically use? though not necessarily as a well-orga?
scure, the shift happened rapidly, by ful properties. In contrast, in the evolu? nized plan.
evolutionary standards, and in the tionary view, the behavior, diets and, Theories that assume intentionality,
mere space of a few thousand years, later, the physiology and morphology however, may be rootedmore in the bi?
different domestic animals and plants of certain animals changed from that ases ofmodern culture than in any ob?
appeared independently in several of theirwild counterparts in response jectivemeasure. Life in the 20th centu?
parts of the world. to the selection pressures of a new eco? rywithout domesticates is virtually
The archaeological record indicates domestic association to so it is tempting to
logical niche?a unimaginable us,
that humankind's best friend?the do? with human beings. This view holds, presume that people who lived with?
mestic dog, Canis familiaris?was likely first, that knowing the intentions of out domesticates during the late Pleis?
also its first.Consequently, I think of prehistoric people is beyond the abili? tocene and early Holocene surely
dogs as the pioneers of an evolution? ties ofmodern science. Second, and of wished to improve their lives.
ary radiation that had radical effects on greater importance, knowledge of peo?
the composition of the earth's biota ple's rational intentionswould not pro? Figure 1. Relationships between Stone Age
and on theway people live. As such, vide a scientific explanation for the people and wolves set the stage for dog do?

dogs are an appropriate focal point for process of domestication. mestication. People and members of the dog
an ongoing debate about the origins family have had a long association, as these

and nature of animal domestication. Domestication as Human Design ll,000-to-12,000-year-old remains attest. A
skeleton from either a dog or a wolf
Central to this discussion is the issue Given the pivotal role of domestication puppy
can be seen under the human skeleton's left
of intentionality?whether domestica? in shaping our present life-style,it is no
hand. These burials were discovered at Ein
tionmust be understood as a human surprise to find thatprehistorians have Mallaha in northern Israel and were origi?
argued vigorously about what domes? nally reported by zooarchaeologists Simon
tication really is,how itoriginated, and
Darcy F.Morey received his Ph.D. in Davis and Francois Valla. Early dog remains

anthropology in 1990 from theUniversity of why. Many classic definitions of the have been found at sites in other parts of the
Tennessee at Knoxville. His primary training is as concept focus on human subjugation of world, suggesting that dog domestication
an archaeologist, with a specialization in other organisms. In a commonly drawn may have taken place independently in dif?

zooarchaeology. His research


on
early scenario, people isolated individuals of ferent regions. Prehistorians have disagreed
domestication, the core of his doctoral project, grew a particular species from their wild about whether different animals were inten?
out of a desire to integrate technical training in domesticated by ancient people,
or
a
counterparts and then selectively bred tionally
zooarchaeology with long-standing interest in them to exaggerate desirable traits and whether domestication is another example of

evolutionary theory and its application to eliminate undesirable ones in a process


evolution driven by natural selection. The
sociocultural phenomena. He currently holds an author argues the latter, and proposes that
with theUniversity known as artificial selection. evolution is best viewed as the product
adjunctaffiliation of dog
Tennessee, where he teaches and does Such a scenario grows out of a com? of selection in a new ecological
occasionally pressures
research. Address: Department ofAnthropology, bination of common-sense reflection niche, in this case a domestic association
252 South Stadium Hall, University ofTennessee, on the conditions under which many with human beings. (Photograph by Alain
Knoxville, TN 37996-0720. modern domesticates live, with the Dagand.)

336 American Scientist, Volume 82

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1994 July-August 337

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A second theme, this one anthro
pocentric, also underlies theories that
Probable time of
earliest domestication
Domestic and probable place of domestication assume intentionality?that people ex?
species
before
(years present) ercise rational control of their collective
destiny. This perspective is appealing,
for itplaces people at the evolutionary
helm, charting the course from the
start. To borrow anthropologist David
Rindos's apt term, a "paradigm of con?
sciousness" is our conceptual anchor,
and from it stems the discussion of do?
mestication as invention, decision, idea
and so on.

Maybe the shift to economic reliance


on domestic species was in some sense
necessary, given human population
growth and environmental changes in
the Holocene. Maybe domestication
was indeed a strategy that prehistoric
people intentionally implemented.
Both propositions are debatable, but
my immediate objection stems from a
problem more fundamental than the
need forbetter data.
The human beings who participated
in the earliest domestic relationships
thousands of years ago are all dead.
They cannot tell us what was in their
minds or what they sought to accom?
plish. For early domestication, the data
required to evaluate scenarios based on
human intentionare, by definition, unat?
tainable. In otherwords, models thatex?
plain domestication thisway cannot be
empirically challenged, and on this ba?
sis alone, they are not scientificmodels.
The real issue iswhether it is neces?
sary to presume the intentions of pre?
historic people tomake sense of early
domestication. Over the years, some
scholars have attempted todescribe do?
mestication inmore mechanistic terms,
focusing on the implications of organ?
isms sharing space and resources in
symbiotic relationships. This approach,
however, has not led to a uniform per?
spective. In 1959, forexample, zoologist
Charles Reed characterized domestica?
tion as "beneficial mutualism." At
about the same time, in 1963, archaeol?
ogist F. E. Zeuner was using the term
"slavery" as a virtual synonym for
some cases of domestication. Neverthe?
less, such efforts can be viewed as the
foundation formore recent attempts to
model domestication as evolution.
Evolutionary perspectives differ from
anthropocentric approaches in several

Figure 2. Humankind's best friend was likely


also its first. This time line shows the esti?
mated times and probable places of origin for
several other important domesticates.

338 American Scientist, Volume 82

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ways. First, they do not restrictdomes? Wolves and late-Pleistocene hunters
tic relationships topeople. The complex and gatherers undoubtedly came into
symbiosis between ants and aphids is a contact regularly, since both were so?
handy example, and is even used inmy cial species who hunted many of the
dictionary to illustrate use of the term same prey items.Wolves are also op?
"domestication." Certain ants herd portunistic scavengers; theywere like?
aphids, providing protection in ex? ly to have been familiar with human
change for the sugary, honey-like liquid hunting practices and to have hung
they "milk" from the aphids. Second, around human settlements regularly.
domestic relationships involve two Let us then assume that the road to?
species. Focusing solely on the human ward domestication began when some
role indomestication ignores the evolu? wolf pups became incorporated into a
tionary stakes forparticipating animals human social and residential setting.
and plants. The ubiquity of dogs, forex? One could speculate endlessly about
ample, suggests theyhave profitedwell the conscious motivation people had
from the domestic arrangement. Their for taking on wolf pups. It seems suffi?
wolf ancestors, on the other hand, have cient, however, to note that different
a
been extirpated frommost of their for? people often kept wild animals for
of reasons without
merly vast range, and many subspecies variety attempting
are now extinct. From a Darwinian per? to achieve long-term domestication.
spective,wolves who took up residence Figure 3. Dogs served a number of economic Somewhere, at some time, one or
with people a few thousand years ago purposes in past human societies, the variety more adopted pups managed to sur?
made a smartmove?at least from to? of which makes it difficult to glean a primary vive to adulthood in the new setting.
day's vantage point.
benefit that people derived from the animals To have a chance in human society, the
during early domestication. This dog bone
animals minimally had to adjust to
Finally, an evolutionary perspective from Qeqertasussuk, a small island off of the
new social rules and to an altered diet.
discourages an assumption thatchanges west coast of Greenland, for example, was dis?
in an animal's size or shape during do? Socialization, according to studies
carded by people along with large quantities
mestication must be products of human of food debris almost 4,000 years ago. A series
conducted by J. P. Scott during the
selection. of cut marks on the bone indicate that the ani?
1950s and 1960s, is oest achieved early
mal from which it came was skinned or in a dog's life.Scott and his colleagues
Ancient Associations butchered. Another dog bone from the site
at the Jackson Laboratory in Bar Har?
If one is to eliminate rational intention had been fashioned into what Danish archae? bor,Maine, conducted long-term stud?
as a scientific explanation forearly ani? ologist Bjarne Gronnow describes as a needle ies of behavior and socialization in
case. Later arctic peoples used dogs to pull that the first few
mal domestication, one must conclude dogs and found
that the process originated with a nat? sleds, and some skulls from later sites in
weeks of a puppy's life are crucial for
Greenland and elsewhere in the Arctic bear
ural association between people and
marks that indicated blows to the head. (Pho?
forming primary social bonds with
thewild ancestors of dogs. Skeletal re? both people and other dogs. Not sur?
tograph by Geert Brovad.)
mains of early dogs from various ar? prisingly, wolves are similar. Several
chaeological sites around the world other studies have shown that young
place the beginnings of their domesti? wolf pups also form lasting bonds with
cation in the late Pleistocene era, possi? a process that becomes more
people,
bly as farback as 14,000 years ago. The difficult for the animals as theymature.
data therefore indicate that canid do? Bonding between people and wolves
mestication took place among people is facilitated by similarities in social
who still pursued a hunting-and-gath structure and in nonverbal modes of
eringway of life. communication. Wolves are organized
The ancestor of these early dogs can hierarchically and they communicate
be identified with confidence as the status through vocal, facial and postur?
wolf Canis lupus.This assertion rests on al displays of dominance or submis?
a growing body ofmolecular data and sion. These displays involvemany cues
isbuttressed by the strikingphysiologi? that are recognizable to people. Dogs
cal and behavioral similarities between use much the same repertoire of cues.
the species. It isnot currentlypossible to Wolves and dogs can also respond ap?
identifywhich subspecies of wolves propriately tomany human signals.
gave rise to domestic dogs (although Figure 4. Dog effigy vessel
was made by a Col It is clear that animals living within
new advances in comparative DNA ima artist The Colima, who inhabited western human settlements had to learn that
Mexico about 2,000 years ago, and some other subordinate status to dominant hu?
analyses to establish relatedness be?
Precolumbian inMesoamerica
tween species may soon change that). groups appar? mans was an inviolable rule. Some
ently used dogs as dietary fare, as did later wolves were undoubtedly more adapt?
For now, scholars simply recognize the
groups, such as the Aztec. According to a Span?
wolf as the dog's ancestral progenitor, able than others to human dominance,
ish observer at one Aztec market, 400 dogs were
and many people suspect that canid do? sold on a slow day. (Photograph used with per?
and those that did not follow the rules
mestication involved several wolf sub? mission from the Appleton Art Museum, were likely either killed or driven
species indifferentparts of theworld. Ocala, Florida.) away. Some of those that adjusted be

1994 July-August 339

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
came tolerated in human society?a
phenomenon thatmust have occurred
within many human settlements. Se?
lection forbehavioral compatibility in a
settingwith new social boundaries was
a strong force among founding domes?
tic populations.
From thebeginning of theirdomestic
life,wolf pups would also have had to
adjust to a different diet. Wild wolves
take almost all theirnutrition frommeat.
Adults oftenhunt cooperatively for large
prey items,which formodern wolves in?
cludes deer, caribou or moose. Young
wolves often accompany the adults and
learnhunting skills,but thatopportunity
would be lost towolf pups living in the
domestic setting. Instead, theywould
have needed to relymore on people to
share scraps of their own meals, a mix?
tureofmeat and plants. Adeptness at so?
liciting food from people was surely a
valuable skill. To supplement this diet,
wolf pups would have had to learn to
scavenge competitively and to hunt
small animals.
To maintain their toehold in the do?
mestic niche, the domesticated wolves
had to succeed in reproducing. One
could assume that a male might leave
the human setting and mate with wild
animals. If he were successful, the
progeny would be wild and would
therefore not help perpetuate domes?
ticpopulations.
Alternatively, the domestic setting
might have included a male and fe?
male whose progeny remained in the
human settlement. Although this sce?
nario successfully createsmore domes?
tic animals, it also creates a genetically
inbred population, which, in the long
run,weakens the gene pool of the do?
mestic population.
But a female surely had other op?
tions. A wild male thatwas unsuccess?
ful in breeding within a wild pack
might have found a domestic female an
easier target. The female would most
likely raise her offspringwithin the do

Figure 5. DNA sequence comparisons and


other lines of evidence allow scientists to es?
tablish the evolutionary relationships be?
tween members of the dog family. This

analysis suggests thatthegraywolf was the


immediate ancestor of the domestic dog. The
two species share so much genetic material
in common that some scientists have de?
scribed as gray wolves with a few ge?
dogs
netic alterations. The images do not depict
true size relations between the species. Time
is shown in millions of years ago. (Adapted
fromWayne, 1993.)

340 American Scientist, Volume 82

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
mestic setting, although a fewmight at?
tempt to return to thewild with their
pups. The continuation of the domestic
line requires that only some females
raise offspring in a domestic setting.
An irony here is that canid domesti?
cation might have foundered ifnot for
the role ofwild males finding alterna?
tive reproductive opportunities. Still,
their strategy ensured that the domes?
ticpopulation was not isolated geneti?
cally fromwild populations. Genetic
input fromwild wolves was probably
strong formany generations. Even to?
day dogs and wolves are capable of
mating and producing fertileoffspring.

Evolution in a Domestic Setting


The new population of domestic
wolves undoubtedly continued to ex?
pand. But at some point the animals be?
gan to change physically and behav
iorally, evolving toward the formwe
recognize today as the dog. Early dogs
conveniently exhibit consistent mor?
phological changes when compared
with wolves. Briefly (and not exhaus?
tively), dogs became smaller overall,
and the length of the snout became pro?
portionally reduced. The result was a
smaller animal with a shorter face, a
steeply rising forehead and proportion?
ally wider cranial dimensions. This
general pattern suggests that adult ani?
mals retained juvenile characteristics, a
phenomenon known as paedomorpho
sis. Paedomorphic dogs have a some?
what puppylike cranial morphology
when compared with adult wolves.
In seeking to explain this pattern,
many discussions presume thatdomes?
tic animals must change inways that
serve people. For example, some dis?
cussions suggest that people involved
in early canid domestication may have
found paedomorphic features endear?
Figure 6. Gray wolf was almost certainly the ancestor of the domestic dog.
ing and favored animals that retained
them. Similarly, it has been suggested
that people found smaller animals the possibility that some selection pres? tion.Different ecological circumstances
more manageable and favored them as sure other than human preference pose different selection pressures, and
well. Such suggestions appear reason? brought about the changes. the answers to these kinds of questions
able, especially because they reflect Specialists in life-history studies depend on the specific conditions faced
common biases inpeople's present-day have developed some tools forprobing by the animals.
choices for good household pets. But this issue. The life-history analyst fo? In addition to selection for social
these changes were taking place ubiq? cuses on the entire life cycle of an ani? compatibility, I propose that the condi?
uitously some 10,000 years ago, despite mal, especially how changes in timing tions faced by early domestic canids
tremendous variability in cultural and of developmental processes and impor? led to strong selection on reproductive
It seems tant life events can have consequences timing and body size. These selection
geographic settings. unlikely
that all these human groups would that impact reproductive success. Life pressures ultimately produced the
have selected forexactly the same traits history analysts might consider when smaller, paedomorphic animal known
in dogs. Surely, the consistent appear? and how often an animal should repro? as the dog.
ance of these traits in animals living duce, or how big and how fast it J.P. Scott,whose experimental work
within somany different cultures raises should grow, depending on its situa with dogs has already been noted, point

1994 July-August 341

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ed out that canid domestication may be
regarded as ecological colonization of a
new niche. Population models view the
hallmark of colonization as rapid popu?
lation growth. One reason for this is that
mortality becomes less dependent on
population density compared with more
stable conditions. Under these circum?
^
stances, a classical prediction of life-his?
torymodels is that selection should fa?
vor lowered age at first reproduction.
Increased fertilityis at a premium then,
and precocious maturation is a remark?
ably efficientway to achieve this.Evolu?
tionary theorypredicts that this change
should result in size reduction and pae
domorphosis in a descendant species,
owing to a truncationof the growth peri?
od. In such a case, both consequences are
only by-products of selection on repro?
ductive timing. It is tantalizing to note
thatwild wolves reach sexual maturity
at about the age of 2 years,whereas most
modern dog breeds achieve maturity be?
tween 6 and 12months. Unfortunately, it
isdifficult to know when in theirhistory
dogs started to reach sexual maturity
earlier, and the current observed ages
might justbe an artifactofmodern selec?
tive breeding programs.
A consideration of life-history stud?
ies also suggests that body size itself
was a likely target of selection. An ani?
mal's body size plays a crucial role in
defining its niche, and studies have
shown that adult size is correlated with
most life-history traits.Unfortunately,
causes can be difficult to disentangle
from effects.With early dogs, dietary
change had to be pronounced, and I
believe thisplaced smaller animals at a
distinct advantage, because of their
lower nutritional requirement. Admit?
tedly this idea is difficult to test, and

Figure 7. Natural selection may have brought


about many changes in the physiology and
overall body size of domesticated wolves and
led them to form a separate
eventually
species?the domesticdog. Skeletal remains
show that early dogs were smaller and that
adult dogs appeared juvenile in relation to
their wolf ancestors. Here a prehistoric adult
skull (center) is compared with an adult
dog
wolf skull (top) and a juvenile wolf skull.
The dog skull bears a strikingsimilarity to
the juvenile wolf skull and ismuch less sim?
ilar to the skull of the adult wolf. These

changes suggest that the developmental pro?


gram of the dog was altered in such a way
that itwould reach sexual maturity earlier
than itswolf ancestors, while other aspects of
its physical development were slowed down.
(The juvenilewolf skull is enlargedhere for
the sake of comparison.)

342 American Scientist, Volume 82

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
other factorswere probably involved.
Different lines of evidence at least sug?
gest that dogs took a very direct route,
genetically speaking, to get to smaller
sizes. Zoologist Robert K. Wayne of the
Zoological Society of London studied
DNA sequences inmodern canids and
concluded that dogs basically are
wolves, altered only by simple changes
in developmental timing and growth
rates. In related studies,Wayne also sug?
gested that reduced fetal growth rates
may be an important determinant of
adult size in small dogs. Simple changes
led to rapid size reduction in early dogs,
probably at the cost of problems in the
integration of different developmental
processes during growth. For example,
it is frequently observed that earliest
dogs often have crowded teeth, some?
times even overlapping each other in
jaws thatare not really big enough to ac? -?
commodate them efficiently.Overall, shorter longer
rapid size reduction with minimal genet? growthperiod
ic change suggests strong selection for
smaller size among early dogs. Figure 8. Size change is often a consequence of changes in growth rates and timing. This
Consistent size reduction clearly took schematic illustrates a hypothetical model to account for differences in size and morphology

place in the early evolution of the dog,


between the wolf and the dog. This model postulates that the descendant species grows more
in life and sooner than the ancestral species. In this way the
although causes are difficult topinpoint. slowly very early finishes growing
comes to resemble the juvenile form of itswolf ancestor when it reaches its full size.
But evolutionary theory also predicts dog
that the proposed developmental alter?
ations should produce paedomorphic where people were stillmaking their patterns stem from basic laws of bio
animals, and this requires a close look. living primarily through hunting and mechanics. For example, an elephant's
gathering at the time corresponding to mass could not be supported on geo?
Evolutionary Paedomorphs the age of the sample, and therefore metrically scaled-up mouse bones. To
It is one thing to note that the cranial where I had little reason to suspect sys? begin with, an elephant has to have
morphology of early dogs appears pae? tematic selective breeding. proportionally thicker leg bones. Shape
domorphic. It is quite another to argue Next, Imeasured crania from 222 changes shown by dogs could reflect
that this pattern sets them apart from modern wild canids representing four only thiskind of allometry.
other canids or reveals something im? species. These are, in descending order My analysis revealed some interest?
portant about evolution under domesti? of average size, the gray wolf, the red ing results. First, it turns out thatmost
cation.Other wild canids might also ap? wolf, the coyote and the golden jackal. dogs share snout-length proportions
pear paedomorphic when compared The wolves and coyotes are all North with comparably sized wild canids.
with wolves. Dogs are frequently de? American, from the continental United What sets dogs apart is not changes in
scribed as paedomorphic because mod? States or southern Canada. Based on the length of their snouts, but the
ern small breeds resemble juvenile cranial measurements, I determined width of their palates and cranial
forms of largerbreeds. But to have evo? thatmost of the prehistoric dogs inmy vaults. The cranialmorphology of dogs
lutionary significance, it is important to sample were roughly the size of golden isunique and does not conform to allo
determine whether prehistoric dogs jackals or the smaller coyotes. I did not metric patterns among wild canids.
were paedomorphic relative to theiran? have prehistoric samples of wild The issue then iswhether this cra?
cestral species, thewolves. canids and must assume thatmodern nial morphology reflects evolutionary
To tackle these problems, I armed a generally valid ap? paedomorphosis. To determine this, I
samples provide
with calipers and a notebook proximation of morphological varia? compared dog morphology with the
myself
and visited several American and Eu? tions in these species. morphology of its ancestral species,
museums tomeasure canid cra? Iwas interested in learn? the wolf, as it grows. If the dog is in
ropean particularly
nia. First, I tookmeasurements from 65 ing how several snout-length and cra? fact a paedomorphic wolf, Iwould ex?
adult prehistoric dog specimens from nial-width dimensions change in rela? pect to see the greatest similarities be?
archaeological sites, the vast majority tion to the overall length of the skull as tween dogs and juvenile wolves and
dated from between 3,000 and 7,000 one moves from large to small animals. less similarity between prehistoric
years ago. Three-quarters of the speci? Size changes in animals are almost in? dogs and adult wolves. Ideally, data
mens are from theUnited States, and evitably accompanied by patterned for answering this question would in?
the rest come from northern Europe. changes inproportions, a phenomenon clude cranial measurements from ju?
choice of sites known as allometry. Some allometric veniles of both species. Unfortunately,
My samples emphasized

1994 July-August 343

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
the archaeological record is not that Using the allometric approach considers only the ratio of snout
cooperative, and skulls of juveniles are again, I compared juvenile wolf pro? length to total skull length.
usually nothing more than a pile of portions with those of the adult wild When I plotted width-to-total-skull
fragile, fingernail-sized pieces. With? canids and prehistoric dogs. By plot? length proportions, I saw some inter?
out data from juvenile dogs, it be? ting snout-length measures against to? esting differences. Adult dogs are dis?
comes imperative to have data from tal skull length, I found that as a wolf tinct in these dimensions from all the
a wild canids. But adult dogs do re?
juvenile wolves, and this cause is not grows, its snout gets longer at rate adult
as hopeless. Imeasured skulls of 64 thatmirrors increasing snout length in semble one wild canid group: juvenile
modern juvenile wolves ranging in the adult wild canids. These plots wolves. Of all wild canid species, in?
age from a few weeks to several showed that all adult canid species, in? cluding adult wolves, the shape of
months, a sample that includes several cluding adult dogs, look something adult dog crania most closely resembles
North American subspecies. like scaled-down adult wolves, if one that of juvenile wolves. The issue is not
closed, but these data do support the
hypothesis that dogs represent a pae
domorphic formof theirwolf ancestors.
If that is true, it is possible that thedogs
evolved as the evolutionary model
would predict. Developmental changes
in these animals might have come
about as a response to selection pres?
sures in a new niche, and these changes
ultimately gave rise to a paedomorphic
form of the ancestral species.

Behavioral Paedomorphosis
Many adult dogs not only appear ju?
venile, they also act juvenile. They dis?
play a sort of behavioral paedomor?
phosis. Dogs routinely solicit attention,
play, grovel, whine, bark profusely and
otherwise exhibit behavior thatwolves
more or less outgrow as theymature.
Biologist Raymond Coppinger and lin?
guist Mark Feinstein describe dogs as
"stuck in adolescence." They also make
the important point that the essence of
tameness is the submissive, solicitous
behavior style of juveniles. This leads
to the question ofwhether physiologi?
cal and behavioral paedomorphosis
are interrelated.
Experiments directed by Russian ge?
neticist D. K. Belyaev cause one to sus?
pect that the answer is yes. Belyaev's
group implemented a strict selective
breeding program with silver foxes
froma commercial fur farm.Their work
sprung from the observation that al?
though a majority of captive foxeswere
aggressive or fearful around people, a
small number, about 10 percent, were
less so.More than 30 years ago Belyaev
began selectively breeding these calmer
individuals only with other such indi?
viduals, through successive generations.
Selection was forwhat Belyaev de?
scribed as domesticated behavior.
Figure 9. Behavioral alterations seem to accompany physical changes, so that dogs not only look
more juvenile than wolves; were shown to
The results after only about 20 gen?
they also act more juvenile. Behavior and physiology
be linked by breeding experiments conducted by Russian geneticist D. K. Belyaev. Belyaev and
erations were fascinating. Many foxes
his colleagues interbred foxes that responded well to people. After about 20 generations, foxes in the selected population now actively
from this lineage actively sought contact with people, whined and wagged their tails. Like sought contact with people. The foxes
many dogs, some tame foxes had drooping ears and erect tails, features thatwere decidedly ab? would lick people's hands and faces,
sent from the control fox population in this study. (Adapted from Belyaev 1979.) whine and wag their tails. Whereas

344 American Scientist, Volume 82

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Borzoi '
^^1^ lf\sL
9?nd0?S Salukl^^^^^:
\
bulldog
[^^^ "
V/^^ -^^_^y ^SSv^c ?^^^Ei^
; ??' ^hantoqi]ri;
^^bKII^^ ^^^^^^^^^ ^M^^ ^^^V

dofi^^j^ \.
7^ herding f' ' "

I mastiffs -^
^^^|^^^^^HH^^^^^^^B ?^^?^

Dane
Great

<^[^ ^^^^^^^^^

^ ."
European
spitz'?.??J:^/lhy ^M^^' V^r; ..
vv^-v;^;,'
?^^^

Figure 10.Differentgeographic subspecies ofwolves may have givenrise todifferentdog-breed groups.This model shows one interpretation
of theancestryofmodern dog breeds.Modern dog breeds are theresultof at least2,000years of breeding under human control,and no breed
isderived solely fromone geographic origin.Ultimately,DNA studiesmay help assess theaccuracyofmodels like thisone. The imagesdo not
depict the truesize relationsbetween breeds. (Adapted fromClutton-Brockand Jewell1993.)

wild foxes, like wild wolves, breed fox population and are absent inwild Scientists are still far from under?
once each females in the se? wolves as well.
only year, standing precisely how different fac?
lected population began a shift to? The experiments do not replicate, tors combined to produce the changed
wards more frequent receptivity,with even roughly, the conditions of animal whose bones begin to turn up
early
some
later-generation females capable domestication, but they show how in late Pleistocene archaeological sites.
of breeding twice each year. Domestic behavioris linkedtophysiolo?
strongly Several important factors, however, at
dogs regularly breed more than once gy. Strict selection for certain behavioral least seem to point in the same direc?
each year. Other changes in the select? traits can disrupt previously stable pat? tion.Whether focusing on social be?
ed population included a much longer terns of physiological or
development. havior, diet reproductive tactics, one
moulting time, drooping ears and erect Oddities of domestication, such as erect should find that the evolution of a
tails. These remarkably dog-like tails and drooping ears, make more smaller, paedomorphic canid during
changes were absent in the unselected sense in lightof this
work. domestication presents no surprise.

1994 July-August 345

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Beyond Dogs
A couple of years ago, a colleague com?
mented that Iwas fortunate tohave cho?
sen dogs as my subject, because my per?
spective would not hold up for other
cases of domestication. Naturally, I asked
why not. He answered that dogs were
but after that, the idea of domestica?
first,
tion was in place. People then had a
model, one they could apply to animals
of considerably greater economic impor?
tance, for example goats or cattle. The
domestication of such animals, my col?
league argued, would best be under?
stood as the product of people's pur?
poseful effortsto achieve thatgoal.
Applying the same logic toBelyaev's
experiments, we might just as well ex?
plain the evolution of modified, tame
foxes as a consequence of Belyaev set?
ting out to accomplish that. Such an ex?
planation is not scientificallymeaning?
ful.The mechanistic explanation begins
with the observation that foxes with
certain heritable traitsmated only with
foxes bearing similar traits through suc?
cessive generations.
The issue is not whether prehistoric
people engaged in behavior that led to
the domestication of goats or cattle.
They certainlymust have. The issue lies
with the presumption that the eventual
result?highly modified animals under
conscious human subjugation?ex?
plains the process that started those an?
^^^^j^^^^^^^^^^^^^S^j^^^k
^^^^^^ imals toward that end. Figuring out
what prehistoric people actually did
that contributed to the evolution of do?
mestic organisms is hard enough. To
presume theirpurposes, and then prof?
fer that as part of an explanation for
evolutionary change, is to flirtwith
mysticism.
To be fair,my colleague's argument
was undoubtedly reflects a broader tendency for schol?
Figure 11. Canid domestication helped along by the ability of the animals to
form strong social attachments to people. For many modern ars to treat dogs as a special case be?
dogs, social bonding is vital to
cause they are not
their individual well-being. This scene, familiar to dog enthusiasts, emphasizes that bonding perceived as eco?
is a two-way street.
nomically important and therefore
provided no compelling reason for
For a long time, early domestic dogs other thannatural. It isnot a process that people to have sought to domesticate
were consistently smaller compared distinguishes human factors from oth? them. Many societies, however, have
with wolves. In contrast,modern dogs ers in the environment.Natural selection made regular use of dogs as dietary
include breeds, such as theGreat Dane, is simply the statistical summation of the fare. In addition, dog skins have served
that are as large as or even larger than reproductive fates of organisms thatuse as clothing, and bones as raw material
wolves. Given the context of domesti? theirphysical and behavioral equipment for tools, and the living animals have
cation, only one set of circumstances is to compete forgenetic representation in often been used as beasts of burden or
likely to account for large dogs or can the next generation. Dogs are no excep? as hunting aids.
account for the size range of modern tion.Tameness and other traitswere the Ultimately, the present exercise is
breeds. That set of circumstances is se? currency of competition from the onset only a minor part of themuch larger is?
lective breeding under human control. of the domestic relationship, regardless sue of how to fithuman cultural evolu?
It is important to stress that a domes? ofwhether people had goals for the ani? tion into a scientific framework. Hu?
tic relationship does not mean thatnat? mals or were even aware of what man culture, not being genetically
ural selection has become something changes were unfolding. determined, iswidely assumed to su

346 American Scientist, Volume 82

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
persede the Darwinian processes that selves. Consider how difficult it is to Davis, S. J.M. 1987. The Archaeology ofAnimals.

explain how other organisms evolve. take even the seemingly small step of New Haven:Yale University Press.
Dunnell, R. C. 1982. Science, social science and
By extension, domestication is also fre? bringing domestic organisms under the common sense: The dilemma of
quently exempted from Darwinian Darwinian umbrella. We are a longway agonizing
modern archaeology. Journal ofAnthropolog?
models of evolution for the simple rea? fromknowing whether Dunnell is right. ical Research 38:1-25.
son that it arises in a human sociocul Hemmer, H. 1990. Domestication?The Decline
tural context. In a field hungry forgen? Acknowledgments of Environmental Appreciation. Cambridge,
uine theory,however, anthropologists This research has been supported by England: Cambridge University Press.
and archaeologists are currentlydebat? the JacobK. JavitsProgram of theU. S. McKinney, M. L. and K. J.McNamara 1991.

Department of Education, the Smith?


Evolution
ing the applicability ofDarwinian theo?
Heterochrony?The of Ontogeny.
New York: Plenum Press.
sonian the American-Scan?
ry to sociocultural evolution. Biologists
Institution,
D. F. 1992. Size,
shape, and develop?
should be keenly interested in this de? dinavian Foundation and theWenner Morey,
ment in the evolution
of the domestic dog.
Gren Foundation forAnthropological
bate, for in the exclusion of cultural Journal ofArchaeological Science 19:181-204.
evolution from the Darwinian model Research. I thankMichael Logan forof?
Morey, D. R and M. D. Wiant. 1992. Early
makes it irrelevant to a good portion of fering suggestions and helping locate Holocene domestic dog burials from the

lifeon thisplanet. sources for several illustrations inused North American Midwest. Current Anthro?
in this article. pology 33:224-229.
More than a decade ago, archeologist
Olsen, S. J. 1985. Origins of theDomestic Dog.
R. C. Dunnell suggested that ifarchae? Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Bibliography
ology should achieve itswidely pro? Belyaev, D. K. 1979. Destabilizing selection as a Rindos, D. 1984. The Origins ofAgriculture?An
fessed goal of becoming scientific, few factor in domestication. The Journal ofHered? Evolutionary Perspective. New York: Acade?
people would be pleased with the re?
mic Press.
ity 70:301-308.
sult. For one thing, there theories about Clutton-Brock, J. 1981. Domesticated Animals Roff, D. A. 1992. The Evolution of LifeHistories.
New York: Chapman and Hall.
cultural evolution would not be from Early Times. Austin: University of Texas
Press. Scott, J.P. and J.L Fuller. 1965. Genetics and the
grounded in human intention. Even if Clutton-Brock, J.,and P. Jewell. 1993. Origin and Social Behavior of theDog. Chicago: Universi?
we could document people's goals and
domestication of the dog. InMiller's Anatomy ty of Chicago Press.
intentions, they are phenomena to be of the Dog, 3rd edition, ed. H. E. Evans. Wayne, R. K. 1993. Molecular evolution of the

explained, not explanations in them Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders, pp. 21-31. dog family. Trends inGenetics 9:218-224.

This content downloaded from 75.99.61.90 on Wed, 12 Nov 2014 16:38:25 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

También podría gustarte