Está en la página 1de 6

B R I T I S H J O U R N A L O F P S YC H I AT RY ( 2 0 0 0 ) , 1 7 6 , 3 5 7 ^ 3 6 2

Catatonia in autistic spectrum disorders The Elliot House team tends to see chil-
dren and adults presenting complicated
diagnostic problems. The fact that the staff
LORNA WING and AMIT TA SHAH
members have a particular interest in cata-
tonia has also become known and probably
influences the pattern of referrals.

Criteria for catatonia


There are no generally accepted standard-
ised criteria for diagnosing catatonia. In
this study, a diagnosis of catatonia was
given when exacerbation of certain features
Background The clinical pictures of Catatonia is a term used to refer to a cluster
of behaviour occurred in sufficient degree
autistic spectrum disorders include of behavioural features. In its extreme
to interfere with movement and everyday
form, it is manifested as absence of speech
features described in catatonia. functions of self-care, education, occupa-
(mutism), absence of movement (akinesia)
tion and leisure. The essential features
Aims To examine the severe and maintenance of imposed postures (cat-
were:
alepsy). Lesser degrees of these impair-
exacerbation of the catatonic features of (a) increased slowness affecting move-
ments, and various other abnormalities of
autistic disorders in adolescence or early posture, movement, speech and behaviour, ments and verbal responses;
adult life, which occurs in some individuals. are also considered to be catatonic pheno- (b) difficulty in initiating and completing
mena (Bush et al, al, 1996; Joseph, 1992). actions;
Method A semi-structured interview Autistic spectrum disorders are charac-
(c) increased reliance on physical or verbal
schedule was used to collect information terised by a triad of impairments affecting
prompting by others; and
from parents or other care-givers the development of social interaction, com-
munication and imagination. This triad is (d) increased passivity and apparent lack of
concerning 506 referrals to a specialist motivation.
associated with a narrow, repetitive pattern
clinic for autistic spectrum disorders. of interests and activities and a range of Other abnormalities of behaviour often
Individuals with severe exacerbation of other abnormalities of language, movement associated were:
catatonic features were compared with a and behaviour (Wing & Gould, 1979),
(e) reversal of day and night;
same-age group of referrals withoutthis similar to the lesser manifestations of cata-
tonia (Leary & Hill, 1996; Wing, 1996). (f) Parkinsonian features: tremor, eye-
type of deterioration in skills and rolling, dystonia, odd stiff posture,
Some individuals have a severe exacerba-
behaviour. tion of these features in adolescence or freezing in postures, etc.;
adult life (Realmuto & August, 1991; (g) excitement and agitation; and
Results Seventeen per cent of referrals
Wing, 1996). The present study examines
aged15 or over had severe exacerbation of (h) increase in repetitive, ritualistic behav-
the frequency of this exacerbation among
iour.
catatonic features.They were significantly people with autistic disorders referred to a
more likely than the comparison group to diagnostic centre, and compares the charac-
teristics of referrals with and without this Clinical assessment
have had, before the onset ofthe change in
marked deterioration in behaviour. For those seen at Elliot House, information
behaviour, impaired language and was collected from an appropriate infor-
passivity in social interaction. mant, usually one or both parents, using
the Diagnostic Interview for Social and
Conclusions Catatonia is a later Communication Disorders (DISCO; further
METHOD
complication of autistic spectrum details available from the first author upon
disorders, which adds considerably to the Population studied request). This is a semi-structured interview
burden of caring.More research is needed During the period March 1991 to Decem- that enables the interviewer to obtain, in a
ber 1997, 506 children and adults with systematic fashion, details concerning the
to identify causes, neuropathology, and
autistic spectrum disorders were referred medical history and the pattern of develop-
early signs of vulnerability. to the Elliot House Centre for Social and ment from birth of a wide range of skills
Communication Disorders, which is the and atypical behaviour. It contains a sec-
Declaration of interest None. National Autistic Society's tertiary referral tion on associated psychiatric disorders
centre and accepts referrals from anywhere and another on catatonic phenomena.
in the UK or overseas. The majority (94%) When the assessments were carried out
of these were seen at Elliot House for diag- in other locations, the same procedures
nostic assessment. The remainder were seen were adopted as far as possible. In some
where they were currently living: in residen- cases, the diagnosis of an autistic spectrum
tial homes, hospitals or, in a few cases, their disorder had already been firmly estab-
parents' homes. lished. For these individuals, the purpose

357
W ING & S HA
AHH

of the consultation was to consider how to observation before the study began had subgroups were identified: those with no,
manage challenging behaviour, sometimes suggested that individuals with autistic impaired or deviant language (language-
including difficulties due to catatonic fea- spectrum disorders who were passive in impaired); and those with grammatical
tures. Relevant diagnostic information was their social interactions were particularly speech and an adequate or good vocabulary
collected from case notes, supplemented vulnerable to the development of severe (language not impaired).
by informants. For the 14 individuals exacerbations of catatonic features. For
(2.8%) about whose early history no infor- the individuals with catatonia, the rating
Type of residence
mant was available, as much information was based on information concerning be-
as possible was obtained from past case re- haviour before the catatonia began. For At the time when they were seen and as-
cords. In all cases, the DISCO was used as the comparison group, the rating was made sessed, individuals in the study were in four
the framework for recording details. for behaviour current at the time when they different types of residence: living indepen-
A study of interrater agreement has were seen by the Elliot House team. dently; at home with parents; in a small
recently been completed for the DISCO, residential home in the community; or in
with the participation of parents of children Psychological assessment a hospital ward.
who had not been seen at Elliot House. For Although associated cognitive deficits of
over 80% of all the developmental
developmental and any level of severity are very common, Statistical analysis
behavioural ratings made (515), kappa autistic spectrum disorders can occur in in- A one-way analysis of variance was used to
was equal to, or better than, 0.75 or, for dividuals of average or superior cognitive compare ages. Otherwise, Pearson's w2 tests
the few ratings where kappa was not appro- ability on formal intelligence tests (Rutter, were used.
priate, agreement was 90% or above 1970; Wing & Gould, 1979; Wing, 1996).
(further details available from the first Assessments of cognitive skills were carried
author upon request). out, using standardised tests appropriate RESULTS
for age and ability to cooperate. The sub-
Prevalence among referrals
Diagnosis jects were observed in structured and un-
structured situations. Because of the A total of 30 individuals with autistic
The autistic spectrum is similar to, but
difficulties of testing individuals with cata- spectrum disorders (6% of all referrals)
broader than, the category of pervasive
tonia, for this group the estimates of cogni- met the above criteria for catatonia. Their
developmental disorders in the tenth revi-
tive ability were based on performance ages when seen at Elliot House ranged from
sion of the International Classification of
before the onset of catatonia, using infor- 15 to 50 years. Table 1 shows the numbers
Diseases (ICD10) (World Health Organ-
mation obtained from case records and and percentages of these with catatonia
ization, 1993). The subgroups represented
interviews with parents or care-givers. For among all referrals, by age group when
among the individuals in the study were
the comparison group, the estimate was seen. All of those with catatonia were aged
Asperger syndrome, childhood autism
based on the findings when seen and 15 or above at referral. They represented
and atypical autism. The criteria used for
assessed by the Elliot House team. 17% of all referrals in that age range.
Asperger syndrome were those suggested
A further eight individuals (five males
by Ehlers & Gillberg (1993) because the
(one with Down's syndrome) and three
ICD10 criteria had been found to be Cognitive ability
females, aged 1036 years when seen) had
unsatisfactory (Leekam et al,
al, 2000). The On the results of assessments carried out as occasionally had problems crossing thresh-
ICD10 criteria for childhood autism and described above, individuals were classified olds and/or freezing during activity, but
atypical autism were used. When criteria into three groups: severe learning disability were not disabled enough to be included
for both autism and Asperger syndrome (IQ 049), mild learning disability (IQ 50 in the catatonic group. In two of these,
were present in one individual, the latter 69) and borderline, average or high ability the brief catatonic episodes had occurred
diagnosis was given. This is contrary to (IQ 70 and above). Because of the large dis-
ICD10 instructions but was more relevant crepancies between different areas of skill,
to the needs of the individuals concerned. Table 1 Ages when seen at Elliot House
which are characteristic of autistic disor-
ders, the estimates were approximations,
Social interaction but do give an indication of the overall level Age group Total seen With catatonia
of function. (years) n
One of the sections in the DISCO concerns n (%)
social interaction. Individuals in the study
could be assigned to one of three sub- Expressive language
1^4 90 ^ (^)
groups, based on the quality of their inter- Expressive language was assessed on stand- 5^9 159 ^ (^)
actions with others. The three categories ardised measures where possible, or on his- 10^14 82 ^ (^)
were `aloof ' (indifferent to social ap- tory and observation when testing was not
15^19 65 12 (17)
proaches, although physical contact might possible. As with other psychological
20^24 48 8 (17)
be enjoyed),
enjoyed), `passive' (accepting social ap- assessments, estimates of expressive lan-
25^29 19 3 (16)
proaches and following other's lead but guage in those with catatonia were based
not initiating any contact), and `active but on information concerning language before 30^34 20 5 (25)
odd' (making active social approaches the onset of the catatonia. For the compar- 35+ 23 2 (9)
but in an inappropriate manner) (Wing & ison group, the estimates were based on as- Total 506 30 (6)
Gould, 1979; Wing, 1996). Clinical sessments by the Elliot House team. Two

358
C ATATONI A IN
I N AU T I S T I C S P E C T RU M D I S O R D E R S

only in early or middle childhood. In one, case notes. From the onset of catatonia up According to the accounts given by re-
catatonic features were seen at school but to the time they were seen at Elliot House, latives or other carers, none had ever had
not at home. three individuals had experienced a slow the first-rank symptoms of schizophrenia.
but steady deterioration in mobility and All showed abnormalities of speech, but
practical skills. In 17, the course had been these were indistinguishable from those
Characteristics of the group
steady once the catatonia was established, found in autistic disorders. The quantity
with catatonia
while four had shown minor and six had of speech was in all cases markedly reduced
Age of onset shown major fluctuations in severity. or absent when catatonia was present.
The ages of onset of the current episode of
catatonia, obtained from the recollections Severity of the effect of catatonia
of informants, are given in Table 2. For on daily activities
Manifestations of catatonic behaviour
four individuals, the ages of onset could
The most common manifestations of cata- At the time of referral, the 30 individuals in
not be ascertained. For the rest, the major-
tonic behaviour seen in the group, each the catatonic group varied in the degree to
ity started the current episode between 10
affecting seven or more individuals, are which the catatonia interfered with their
and 19 years of age. Five individuals had
listed in Table 3. Most of the abnormalities participation in everyday activities, their
had brief episodes of slowness and freezing
of movement resulted in slowing or stop- mobility and their independence, as
during childhood, before age 10.
ping activities, but episodes of excitement follows.
and sudden impulsive actions also Twelve with the least severe symptoms
Type of onset occurred. Some could not stop actions once were mobile but were very slow in carrying
In 15 individuals, the onset was im- started. Incontinence resulted when the out all self-care activities, often freezing
mediately preceded by a period of very individual concerned did not initiate the during an activity and needing prompting
disturbed, often aggressive, behaviour. movement needed to reach the toilet. to begin moving again. They had difficulty
Eight individuals developed obsessive As shown in Table 3, 12 individuals crossing thresholds or demarcation lines,
compulsive symptoms before they became showed bizarre behaviour that could not but were able to take part in the daily
catatonic. Six of these were among those be classified under other headings. For ex- activities with much help from staff.
who had shown disturbed, aggressive ample, two individuals would never use Fourteen were more severely affected.
behaviour before the onset of catatonia. one arm and hand (the left in one case They were so slow, or so unable to cross
and the right in the other), although no thresholds, or so locked into one repetitive
physical reason could be found. One man activity, that their daily programme was
Possible precipitating factors
walked the same route to the same destina- severely impoverished.
Possible precipitating factors were sug- Four individuals were the most severely
tion each day in order to stand motionless,
gested for 13 individuals. These included affected. Their lives were completely dis-
staring for 2 hours at a spot where a build-
bereavement, pressure at school, lack of rupted by their symptoms. One stayed in
ing used to be before it was pulled down.
structure after leaving school, and lack of one room, rocking in a chair, and could
Others had occasional visual hallucinations
occupation. The individuals concerned did not leave the house unless carried out, as
or paranoid ideas that did not fit any parti-
not communicate their feelings about these stiff as a statue, completely covered by a
cular diagnosis.
events. The suggestion of a connection with blanket. One had to be half-lifted by two
the onset of the catatonia was made by the care-givers to enable him to cross any de-
Table 3 Most frequent manifestations of
parents or other care-givers. marcation lines, including cracks in pave-
catatonia
ments. One sat immobile unless physically
Course prompted, not even moving to empty her
Catatonic manifestations Feature
bladder. However, when taken out and
Information about any fluctuations in the present started on a route, she would walk for miles
catatonic state over time was obtained from n without a pause until prompted to stop.
parents or other carers and from available
The fourth was permanently confined to a
Essential criteria (slowness, difficulty 30 wheelchair, apart from one occasion when
T
Table
able 2 Ages of onset of catatonia initiating movements, needs his elderly father stumbled and nearly fell.
prompts, passive) Seeing this, the son leapt from his wheel-
Age group (years) n Odd gait 27 chair, helped his father to sit down and
Odd, stiff postures 19 then returned to immobility.
1^4 ^ None of the above variables was signif-
Freezing 17
5^9 ^ icantly related to autistic spectrum sub-
Impulsive acts 16
10^14 7 group, expressive language, level of ability
Difficulty crossing lines 16
15^19 16
Bizarre/psychotic 12
or quality of social interaction.
20^24 2
Sleep problems 10
25^29 1
Incontinence 10 Comparison with a group
Not known 4
Cannot stop actions 7
without catatonia
Total 30 Excited phases 7 The comparison group comprised 115 indi-
viduals with autistic spectrum disorders

359
W ING & S HA
AHH

whose age range (when they were seen at Table


Table 4 Characteristics of the catatonia and comparison groups
Elliot House) was the same as that of those
with catatonia (15 years and above), and Variable Catatonia group (n
(n30)
30) Comparison group (n
(n115)
115) P
whose clinical details were entered into a
computer file. Twenty-two individuals in n (%) n (%)
the same age range were excluded because
they were not in the computer file for Gender
administrative reasons, not due to any bias Male 28 (93) 91 (79)
NS
in their clinical pictures. The eight with Female 2 (7) 24 (21)
mild or previous catatonia mentioned Diagnostic subgroup
above were also excluded. Autism 11 (37) 32 (28)
Atypical autism 5 (17) 8 (7) NS
Age when seen at Elliot House Asperger syndrome 14 (47) 75 (65)
The age range of those with catatonia was Social interaction
1750 years (mean 24.6). For the compari- Aloof 5 (17) 18 (16)
son group it was 1560 years (mean 25.1). Passive 15 (50) 19 (17) 50.001
There was no significant difference between Active/odd 10 (33) 77 (67)
the groups. Level of cognitive ability
Severe learning disability 9 (30) 22 (19)
Gender Mild learning disability 9 (30) 19 (17) NS

There was an expected excess of males in Borderline/average 12 (40) 74 (64)


both groups, somewhat more marked among Expressive language
those with catatonia. The difference between Impaired 16 (43) 30 (26)
0.008
groups was not significant (see Table 4). Not impaired 14 (57) 85 (74)
Residence

Diagnostic subgroup Parents' home 9 (30) 70 (61)


Own home ^ (^) 16 (14)
In the group studied, catatonia occurred in 50.001
Care home 12 (40) 13 (11)
a smaller proportion of those with Asperger
syndrome than of those with childhood aut- Hospital 9 (30) 16 (14)
ism or atypical autism, but the difference
was not significant (see Table 4).
(w223.27,
including psychiatric hospitals (w 23.27, Factors involved in the
Quality of social interaction d.f.3,
d.f. 3, P50.001) (see Table 4). development of catatonia
Only 17% of the comparison group were Catatonia was seen more often in those who
passive when assessed, whereas half of Epilepsy had impaired expressive language, and
those with catatonia were passive before Four (13%) of those with catatonia and those who were passive in social inter-
the onset of the catatonic behaviour 25 (22%) in the comparison group had a action, before the onset of catatonia. Within
(w214.98,
14.98, d.f.2,
d.f. 2, P50.001) (see Table 4). history of one or more epileptic fits. The the group of those who developed catato-
difference between the groups was not nia, however, the number of catatonic
significant. features and the degree to which the
Level of cognitive ability
catatonia limited everyday activities had
In the group studied, catatonia was seen in
DISCUSSION no significant relationship to expressive lan-
a somewhat higher proportion of those
guage ability or type of social interaction.
with learning disabilities, but the difference Prevalence In the majority of individuals in this
between the groups was not significant (see
As previously noted, the number of indivi- study, the onset of severe catatonia
Table 4).
duals with catatonia, as defined above, occurred in the years from 10 to 19,
among Elliot House referrals was probably although a few had also had brief episodes
Expressive language unusually high. The proportion of indivi- of freezing, or difficulty crossing thresh-
The individuals with catatonia were more duals with this type of deterioration in olds, in childhood. Adolescence is asso-
likely to be language-impaired than the com- skills and behaviour should not, without ciated with physical and psychological
parison group (w (w26.95,
6.95, d.f.1,
d.f. 1, P0.008
0.008 further investigation, be considered as typi- stresses. However, in more than half of
with continuity correction) (see Table 4). cal of autistic disorders in general. It is also the group studied, no particular precipitat-
possible that the factors found to be signif- ing cause could be identified, although they
icantly associated with catatonia can be ex- may have experienced stresses that were
Type of residence plained by referral bias. Nevertheless, the idiosyncratic to the individuals concerned
More of the group with catatonia were comparisons between those with and those but were not evident to the care-givers.
currently in some form of residential care, without catatonia remain valid. Most adolescents with autistic disorders

360
C ATATONI A IN
I N AU T I S T I C S P E C T RU M D I S O R D E R S

do not develop catatonia, although they too


are likely to meet the same kinds of difficul-
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
ties associated with growing up. It is poss-
ible that some individuals have an & Catatonia develops in adolescence in a small proportion of individuals with autistic
inherent vulnerability to developing cata-
spectrum disorders.
tonia, which becomes overt in response to
stress. & Recognition of catatonia in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders is
necessary in order to institute appropriate management and care.

Autistic disorders, catatonia


& The presence of an undiagnosed autistic spectrum disorder should be considered
and related motor disorders in any individual presenting with catatonia.
The terms `catatonia' and `autistic
LIMITATIONS
spectrum disorder' refer to clusters of
behavioural features. They can each be & The prevalence rate of catatonia was based on referrals to a specialist clinic and is
associated with a range of physical and
likely to be higher than that for a total population.
psychological conditions. They are best re-
garded as effects of underlying causes and & The study was cross-sectional and does not provide information on future course
not as causal entities. They share the fuzzi- and prognosis.
ness inherent in concepts defined only by a
mix of behavioural features, even when op- & Profiles based on all clinical features were not examined in the catatonic and
erational criteria have been standardised, as comparison groups.
with the ICD10 classification. Neverthe-
less, they are useful concepts in clinical
practice. It is necessary to use appropriate
methods of management and treatment of
the overt behaviour when, as in the group LORNA WING, FRCPsych, Centre for Social and Communication Disorders, Bromley, Kent; AMITTA SHAH,
described here, a treatable underlying cause MSc, Leading Edge Psychology, Purley, Surrey
cannot be identified.
Correspondence: Dr Lorna Wing,Centre for Social and Communication Disorders, Elliot House,
Autistic conditions are developmental Bromley,Kent BR2 9HT
disorders present from birth or early in life.
The concept of catatonia was developed in (First received 15 June 1999, final revision 5 October 1999, accepted 12 October 1999)
relation to behaviour observed in adults.
However, there is a marked overlap of the
behavioural features of the two disorders.
For example, motor stereotypies, manner-
isms, rituals, mutism, echolalia and nega- simple disorders of dyskinesia and parkin- include the pictures resembling obsessive
tivism, among others, are described in sonism to the complex disturbances of cat- compulsive disorder (Rutter, 1985), Tour-
catatonia (Joseph, 1992; Bush et al, al, 1996) atonia, all of which can be related to the ette's syndrome (Realmuto & Main, 1982)
as well as in autistic spectrum disorders extrapyramidal system. Lishman (1997) de- and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
(Rutter, 1978, 1985; Wing, 1996). This scribed the clinical manifestations of ence- (Gillberg, 1992; Wing, 1996). The re-
overlap in the clinical pictures, as well as phalitis lethargica, the condition that was lationship between all these conditions
the findings reported here, raises the ques- pandemic towards the end of the First and autistic disorders suggests that they
tion of the nature of the relationship World War and the 1920s. Lishman noted have some aspect of their neuropathology
between the two disorders. that catatonic phenomena could occur, in common.
In this context it is relevant to note that and he mentioned all the behavioural
the manifestations of catatonia that were features listed in the present paper.
most common in the group described here The tendency to passivity in social Implications for care, treatment
overlap with those found in parkinsonism. interaction seen in some individuals before and research
Damasio & Maurer (1978) discussed the si- the onset of catatonia is, perhaps, a pre- Severe catatonia adds considerably to the
milarities between the motor phenomena of cursor of the lack of ability to initiate difficulties of caring for individuals with
autism and of parkinsonism. Rogers (1992) voluntary movement, which is typical of autistic spectrum disorders, as shown by
considered that a rigid division of motor catatonia and parkinsonism. However, the the numbers with catatonia who were in
disorders into `catatonic' (psychiatric) and findings do not corroborate the view of hospital placements. Admission was the
extrapyramidal (neurological) was not ap- Leary & Hill (1996) that autism is a result and not the cause of the catatonia.
propriate because of the high correlation purely motor problem. The social and cog- It is important for clinicians to be aware
between scores in the same individuals on nitive disabilities are as important as the of the possibility of catatonia when investi-
the relevant scales. He concluded that there motor aspects and cannot be explained gating reasons for deterioration in skills
seems to be a continuously distributed away. and behaviour occurring in adolescents
array of disorders of movement, volition Other disorders affecting movement and adults with autistic spectrum disorders.
and behaviour, ranging from the relatively that can co-occur with autistic disorders Conversely, the possibility of an underlying

3 61
W ING & S HA
AHH

autistic disorder should be considered if an Damasio, A. R. & Maurer, R. G. (1978) A neurological _ & August, G. (1991) Catatonia in autistic disorder; a

model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology,


Neurology, 35,
35, sign of co-morbidity or variable expression? Journal of
individual presents with catatonia. Possible
777^786. Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Disorders, 21,
21, 517^528.
physical or psychological causes should be
Ehlers, S. & Gillberg, C. (1993) The epidemiology of Rogers, D. (1992) Motor Disorder in Psychiatry:Towards a
investigated, and treated if found. In the
Asperger syndrome; a total population study. Journal of Neurological Psychiatry.
Psychiatry. Chichester: Wiley.
absence of a treatable cause, there is no Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Psychiatry, 34,
34, 1327^1350.
known cure for catatonia, but appropriate Rutter, M. (1970) Autistic children: infancy to
Gillberg, C. (1992) The Emmanuel Miller Memorial
care and environmental management can adulthood. Seminars in Psychiatry,
Psychiatry, 2, 435^450.
Lecture 1991: Autism and autistic-like conditions:
help to alleviate the problems for the indivi- subclasses among disorders of empathy. Journal of Child
_ (1978) Diagnosis and definition. In Autism: A
Psychology and Psychiatry,
Psychiatry, 33,
33, 813^842.
dual concerned and for their care-givers Reappraisal of Concepts and Treatment (2nd edn) (eds M.
(further details available from the first Joseph, A. B. (1992) Catatonia. In Movement Disorders Rutter & E. Schopler), pp. 1^25. New York:
York: Plenum.
in Neurology and Neuropsychiatry (eds A. B. Joseph &
author upon request). _ (1985) Infantile autism and other pervasive
R. R.Young),
R. Young), pp. 335^342. Oxford: Blackwell.
Subjects for future research include in- developmental disorders. In Child and Adolescent
Leary, M. R. & Hill, D. A. (1996) Moving on: autism Psychiatry: Modern Approaches (2nd edn) (eds M. Rutter
vestigation of the neuropathology, identifi-
and movement disturbance. Mental Retardation,
Retardation, 34,
34, & l. Hersov), pp. 545^566. Oxford: Blackwell.
cation of early signs of vulnerability to 39^53.
exacerbation of catatonic features, the role Wing, L. (1996) The Autistic Spectrum: A Guide for
Leekam, S., Libby, S.,
S.,Wing,
Wing, L., et al (2000)
of environmental stress, and methods of Parents and Professionals.
Professionals. London: Constable.
Comparison of ICD ^10 and Gillberg's criteria for
management and treatment. Asperger syndrome. Autism,
Autism, 4, 11^28. _ & Gould, J. (1979) Severe impairments of social

Lishman, W. A. (1997) Organic Psychiatry: The


Lishman,W. interaction and associated abnormalities in children:
Psychological Consequences of Cerebral Disorder (3rd epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and
REFERENCES Childhood Schizophrenia,
Schizophrenia, 9, 11^29.
edn), pp. 349^356. Oxford: Blackwell.

Bush, G., Fink, M., Petrides, G., et al (1996) Catatonia: Realmuto, G. M. & Main, B. (1982) Coincidence of World Health Organization (1993) The ICD ^10
I. Rating scale and standardised examination. Acta Tourette's disorder and infantile autism. Journal of Autism Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders:
Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
Scandinavica, 93,
93, 129^136. and Developmental Disorders,
Disorders, 12,
12, 367^372. Diagnostic Criteria for Research.Geneva:
Research. Geneva: WHO.

362

También podría gustarte