Está en la página 1de 7

British Journal of Audiology, 1997,31,345-351

A randomized, controlled trial of the efficacy of a


communication course for first time hearing aid

Graham J. Beynon, Frances L. Thornton2and Chris Poo1e2


Audiology Department, Addenbrookess Hospital, Cambridge CB2 2QQ and zAudiology
Department, Stoke Mandeville Hospital, Aylesbury HP218AL

(Received 25 October 1996 accepted in revised form I7 June 1997)

Abstract
Many centres include a communication course as part of their auditory rehabilitation. These usually take
the form of a small group and include discussion of the effects of hearing loss, use of the hearing aid, hear-
ing tactics and lip reading. To investigate the efficacy of such a rehabilitation programme a randomized,
controlled trial of a communication course was undertaken. All subjects were first time hearing aid users;
handicap was measured using the Quantified Denver Scale of Communication Function (QDS) at the time
of hearing aid fitting, and then 13 weeks later. All subjects had a hearing aid follow-up appointment, but the
treatment group (n = 22) also underwent a four-week communication course, while the control group
(n = 25) had no further rehabilitation. The reduction in handicap measured by the change in QDS was sig-
nificantly greater for the treatment group than for the control group (Mann Whitney U test, tiedp value
= 0.014). This indicates that such a communication course is efficacious in reducing handicap. Further
research is required to identify the populations that will benefit most from such a course.

Key words: communication course, auditory rehabilitation

Introduction While observational studies can indicate t h e


T h e ultimate goal of a n auditory rehabilitation expected outcome of a rehabilitation treatment,
programme is t o reduce t h e communication it remains unknown whether t h e outcome
handicap that hearing impaired people experi- observed can b e attributed t o the rehabilitation
ence. While the fitting of a hearing aid is seen as a provided o r is the natural history for the condi-
main feature of auditory rehabilitation, there are tion. Therefore, in t h e case of aural rehabilita-
many further techniques used including Lip-read- tion which includes a communication course it is
ing, listening training, hearing tactics, hearing aid unknown whether the observed outcomes were
orientation and education regarding hearing d u e t o t h e fitting of a hearing aid only and t h e
loss. Many centres in t h e UK offer these tech- subsequent learning curve in hearing aid use, o r
niques of rehabilitation in the form of a commu- whether the additional input from the communi-
nication course comprising a small group of cation course significantly affected t h e out-
hearing aid wearers meeting for a few weeks. comes. There is a need, therefore, for controlled
There is a need t o evaluate t h e efficacy of these evaluations of such rehabilitation (Wertz, 1993;
communication courses t o justify their existence. Wilson, 1993).
Previous studies o n t h e efficacy of auditory
Address for reprints: G.J. Beynon, Audiology Department, training rehabilitation have shown improvement
Box 94, Addenbrookess Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge
CB2 2QQ
on clinical tests but mixed evidence for reduction

0300-536419713 10345+06 $03.50/0 0 1997 British Society of Audiology


346 Graham J. Beynon et al.

in handicap. For example, improvement in per- group setting with discussion of the effects of
formance on tasks such as consonant or speech hearing loss, speech reading skills, methods to
recognition tests compared has been shown to be improve communication skills and use of assis-
significantly greater in a group undergoing tive devices. The affect on self-perceived handi-
auditory training compared to a control group cap was measured by the HHIE. This showed
receiving no rehabilitation (Walden et al., 1981; that the group receiving the additional rehabilita-
Rubinstein and Boothroyd, 1987). However, a tion had a significantly larger reduction in self-
study examining the effect of an individual four- perceived handicap.
week communication training programme, There is mixed evidence, therefore, for the effi-
which emphasized use of situational and linguis- cacy of acommunication course in reducing hear-
tic cues was not able to demonstrate any signifi- ing handicap. Courses concentrating on auditory
cant reduction in self-perceived handicap in a training aspects of rehabilitation have shown sig-
group of elderly adults compared with a control nificant improvement on clinical tests but not any
group (Kricos et al., 1992). This study used the carry over to reduction in handicap. Courses
Hearing Handicap Inventory for the Elderly utilizing group work and a wide range of informa-
(HHIE) as its measure of handicap (Ventry and tion and rehabilitative techniques appear to have
Weinstein, 1982). A further study also using the demonstrated significant reduction in hearing
H H I E (Kricos and Holmes, 1996) examined a handicap. This present study therefore proposed
controlled trial of active listening training. This to evaluate a communication course employing a
showed significant improvement in clinical tests wide range of rehabilitative techniques by means
of auditory function (speech in noise tests) for the of a prospective, randomized, controlled trial.
treatment group compared with the control
group; in addition a similar improvement was Method
shown on a scale of psycho-social status, which A prospective, randomized, controlled study of
attempts to quantify an individuals adjustment the efficacy of a communication course was
to having a hearing loss. However, these undertaken. The patient population selected was
improvements did not carry over to a reduction in patients being fitted with hearing aids for the first
hearing handicap as measured by the HHIE. time. This reduced the effect of other variables
Evidence for the efficacy of such rehabilitation such as length of time that hearing aids had been
on hearing handicap has been provided by worn, and amount of previous advice regarding
Smaldino and Smaldino (1988) who compared a use of the hearing aids. Only patients aged less
control group receiving standard hearing aid ori- than 80 years of age were included to avoid com-
entation to three experimental groups. The mon difficulties presented by the very elderly
experimental groups all received standard hear- (e.g. transportation, poor manual dexterity). The
ing aid orientation and were then randomized authors felt that such patients require more indi-
into three groups. The first group received addi- vidualized rehabilitation than is available in the
tional information about their individual cogni- course under investigation. Patients were ran-
tive learning style; the second group received this domized into two groups: treatment and con-
cognitive information and a four-week audio- trol. Randomization was by taking the last digit
logic rehabilitation programme; the third group of the patients hospital numbers and assigning a
received the audiological rehabilitation pro- group by whether the digit was an odd or an even
gramme only. The effect on self-perceived handi- number. This took place at the initial fitting of the
cap was examined using the Hearing hearing aid and thus avoided any possible selec-
Performance Inventory (Lamb et al., 1979), tion bias. After the initial fitting patients were left
which demonstrated that the two groups receiv- to wear the hearing aid for six weeks and then
ing audiologic rehabilitation had a significantly returned for a follow-up appointment. This
larger reduction in self-perceived handicap than appointment reviewed the success of the fitting
those receiving other forms of therapy. Another and the hearing aid was adjusted according to the
controlled trial of a group communication course patients reports. After the follow-up appointment
compared patients receiving a hearing aid fol- those patients assigned to the treatment group
lowed by a counselling based auditory rehabilita- entered a four week communication course, the
tion course with patients receiving a hearing aid content of which is described below. The control
only (Abrams et al., 1992). This course utilized a group received no further appointments.
Communication course for hearing aid users 347
For inclusion in the study the patients in the treat- hearing impairment. Instruction in the basic
ment group had to attend at least three of the four elements of lip-reading.
sessions of the course. The hearing loss of the Session 3: explanation and discussion of coping
patients was classified into mild (pure tone average strategies for better communication including
0.5-4 kHz <40 dBHL) and moderate (> 40 dBHL group discussion of finding solutions to com-
pure tone average 0.5-4 kHz 4 0 dBHL). munication problems.
The level of handicap experienced by the Session 4:further instruction in lip-reading and
patients was assessed using the Quantified Den- hearing tactics. Discussion of the stress and
ver Scale of Communication Function (QDS) anxiety that can be involved in communica-
(Schow and Nerbonne, 1980). (The original Den- tion and the use of relaxation techniques.
ver Scale of Communication Function was devel-
oped by Alpiner et al. (1971).) This was Results
administered at the time of the first fitting of the
This study evaluated hearing handicap by means
hearing aid, and then 13 weeks later after the endof a self-rated questionnaire at the time of hear-
of the communication course. The QDS was ing aid fitting, and then 13 weeks later, after the
developed to subjectively assess the communica- treatment group had completed a four-week
tion handicap experienced by hearing impaired communication course. Fifty-three patients were
adults with acquired hearing loss. It has four sub-
entered into the study: 26 into the treatment
scales covering the following areas: (1) self, group, and 27 into the control group. Six patients
which examines the affect of hearing impairment were dropped from the study, five from the treat-
on an individuals view of him- or herself and hisment group for failing to attend the requisite
or her attitudes to the hearing loss; (2) family,
three sessions, and one from the control group
which examines the affect of hearing impairment for failing to return the final questionnaire. There
on an individuals relationship with his or her were therefore 21 patients in the treatment
family; (3) sociallvocational, which examines group, and 26 in the control group. The treatment
the affect of a hearing impairment on an individ- group had 13 females and 8 males; the control
uals work role and common social environ- group had 14 females and 12 males. The average
ments; and (4) communication, which examines age was 68.8 years (range 47-80) for the treat-
the effect of a hearing impairment on communi- ment group, and 68.6 years for the control group
cation difficulties in a range of common situa- (range 50-80). The treatment group had 13
tions. There are 25 questions with each question patients with mild hearing losses and 9 patients
being answered using a five-point semantic dif- with moderate hearing losses; the same figures
ferential continuum ranging from agree to dis-for the control group were 18and 7 respectively.
agree. Scoring is from 1 to 5 for each question The mean handicap prior to hearing aid fitting
giving a total range from 0 (no handicap), to 120 for the two groups was 74.5 and 77.2 (scores out of
(maximum handicap). The internal reliability 120) for the treatment group and control group
and test-retest variability of the QDS has been respectively. Comparison of these pre-treatment
examined by Tuley et al. (1990) with the conclu- scores using a Mann-Whitney U test showed that
sion that it is a reliable and repeatable scale. they were not significantly different (tiedp value =
The communication course (performed by 0.47). The mode and inter-quartile ranges of the
author CP, Hearing Therapist) was a four-week pre-treatment handicap scores is given in Table 1;
course for groups of 5-7 people and covered the this table also shows the values for the four sub-
following areas: scales of the questionnaire. The average handicap
measured 13 weeks later, after hearing aid fitting,
Session 1: explanation of the anatomy and physi- follow-up and the treatment group having com-
ology of the ear; the nature of hearing impair- pleted the communication course, was 55.8 for the
ment; the effects of hearing impairment in treatment group and and 62.6 for the control group.
different situations. Comparison of these scores using a Mann-Whitney
Session 2: explanation of the benefits and disad- U test showed that they were not significantly dif-
vantages of a hearing aid; hearing aid mainte- ferent (tied p value = 0.1 1). The mode and inter-
nance; adjusting amplification in different quartile ranges of the post-treatment handicap
acoustic environments. Explanation of the scores are given in Table 1; this table also shows the
composition of speech and implications for values for the four subscalesof the questionnaire.
348 Graham J. Beynon et al.

Table 1. Handicap scores (QDS) pre- and post-treatment

Total Family Self Vocational Communication


T C T C T C T C T C

Pre-treatment
Mode 73 80 10 8 10 18 36 39 15.5 16
Inter-quartile range 13 29.25 5 6.5 7 5.5 9 20.5 5 3.25

Post-treatment
Mode 56 63 10 8 8.5 14 22.5 29 9 13
Inter-quartilerange 15 29.75 6 5 7 5.25 14 23 7 5
T = treatment group; C = control group

The change in handicap measured for each the 4 subscales. The change in total handicap
patient was examined for the treatment and con- score for the two groups was 18.7 for the treat-
trol group separately using a Mann-Whitney U ment group and 14.6 for the control group (total
test. This showed that the handicap measured score = 120). Comparison of these changes by a
after hearing aid fitting and follow-up for the con- Mann-Whitney U test showed that the change for
trol group was significantly lower than that mea- the treatment group was significantly larger than
sured prior to fitting (tied p value = 0.005). The that for the control group (tiedp-value = 0.014).
same comparison for the treatment group A histogram of these changes in total handicap is
showed that the handicap measured after hearing shown in Figure 1.
aid fitting, follow-up and the communication The change in handicap for the four subscales
course was significantly lower to that measured was compared between the treatment group and
prior to fitting (tiedp value <0.0001). control group using a Mann-Whitney U test. The
The change in handicap before and after treat- vocational and communication subscales showed
ment was calculated for each patient by subtract- significantly larger change in the treatment group
ing the handicap measured after the 13 week compared with the control group (tied p-value
period from that measured at the hearing aid fit- = 0.019 and 0.037 respectively). N o significant
ting. This was calculated for the total score and difference between the two groups was seen for

0 Treatment group
W Control group

Y
v)
C
0)
*
.I

m
a
u
0
&
0)
n
5
z

OS 3-10 1015 ISZJ 20.2s 2S.Y) %IS 3 W

Change in total handicap


Fig. 1. Change in QDS score for treatment and control groups
Communicationcourse for hearing aid users 349

Table 2. Change in handicap scores (QDS)

Total Family Self Vocational Communication


T C T C T C T C T C

Mode 17.5 14 0 0 2 2 10 6 5.5 4


Inter-quartilerange 9 4.5 1 2 3 2.25 I 7 3 4.25
T = treatment group; C = control group

the family and self subscales (tied p value = 0.56 Examination of the four subscales of the QDS
and 0.11 respectively). The actual changes in the indicates the areas in which the communication
subscales before and after treatment is shown in course reduced handicap beyond that of the
Table 2. hearing aid fitting itself. The reduction in handi-
The affect of age was analysed by examining the cap for the communication and vocational
correlation between the change in the total handi- subscales was significantly greater for the treat-
cap score and age for the two groups. For both the ment group. However for the self and family
treatment and the control group no significantcor- subscales the reduction was no different between
relation between these variables was seen (corre- the two groups. It may be inferred, therefore, that
lation coefficients were 0.06 and 0.16 for the the components of the communication course
treatment and control group respectively). influence patients in these first two areas but not
The effect of hearing loss was examined by in the later two. Revision of the content of the
comparing the change in handicap for the control course, such that views of oneself as a hearing
and treatment groups separately. Comparisons impaired person, and the relationships with ones
between mild and moderate hearing losses were family are discussed, may be of benefit. It may of
made using the Mann-Whitney U test and course also be that these areas are harder to reha-
showed no significant difference in the change in bilitate people in than in general communication
handicap for the different classification of hear- and socialhocational functioning.
ing loss for either the control group or the treat- The analysis did not include those patients
ment group (tied p value = 0.95 and 0.47 dropped from the study for failing to attend the
respectively). requisite three sessions as we wished to investi-
gate the change in handicap for only those who
Discussion had undergone the treatment. It must be recog-
This study evaluated the efficacy of a communi- nized that patients who fail to complete a com-
cation course by means of a randomized, con- munication course may well do so due to being
trolled trial. It can be seen that both the patients dissatisfied with it, and hence have a poor out-
in the control group and the treatment group had come. (In this case the reasons given by patients
a statistically significant reduction in reported were either lack of time or ill health.) It is usual to
handicap over the 13week period. This reduction examine the outcome for this type of study on the
in handicap for the control group indicates that basis of intention to treat; that is, including all
rehabilitation in the form of hearing aid fitting those assigned to the treatment group even if
and and a follow-up appointment does signifi- they fail to complete the treatment. This is not
cantly reduce hearing handicap; this finding has possible in the present study due to lack of data
been demonstrated in several studies (Newman on those who failed to complete the treatment.
and Weinstein, 1988;Abrams etal., 1992; Mulrow The possible effect these patients had on the out-
et al., 1992). However, comparing the change in come, however, can be analysed by assigning a
handicap for the two groups showed that the hypothetical outcome handicap score to them. If
reduction in handicap for the treatment group it is assumed that they would on average have had
was significantly greater than that for the control the same decrease in handicap as other members
group. Thus, while handicap was reduced for of the treatment group their post-treatment
both groups it was reduced by a greater amount handicap score can calculated by subtracting the
for the group that had additional rehabilitation in average handicap decrease observed for the
the form of the communication course. treatment group. This would increase the
350 Graham J. Beynon et al.

significance of the difference in handicap change therapy that involves the same amount of patient
between the treatment and control group (Mann- contact time as a communication course is hard to
Whitney U test, tiedp value = 0.004). However, if conceive. There are two issues involved in this
it is assumed that the communication course was criticism. First, the aim of this study is to examine
of no benefit to these patients then their handicap whether a communication course reduces handi-
reduction should on average be the same as the cap compared to hearing aid fitting only, rather
control group. If their post-treatment score is cal- than examining one rehabilitation technique ver-
culated by subtracting the average reduction in sus another. Therefore, one group having more
handicap for the control group, this gives a worst contact time than the other is an innate part of the
case scenario. This will reduce the significance of study and no defence need be made for this. The
the difference between the treatment and control second and more difficult area is whether the ben-
group but it still remains statistically significant efit reported by the treatment group is simply due
(Mann-Whitney U test, tied p-value = 0.019). to the patient feeling that more time and effort
Therefore, it can reasonably be estimated that has been put into their treatment. The use of a
the patients who failed to complete the communi- standardized questionnaire is the crucial element
cation course did not affect the significance of the in avoiding this halo effect as much as possible.
difference between the control group and treat- The questionnaire focuses attention on actual
ment group. real life situations and difficulties rather than
The present study adds to the evidence that a some global assessment of benefit (e.g. Do you
communication course does significantly reduce feel your hearing problems have decreased?) or,
hearing handicap compared to hearing aid fitting even worse, an assessment of how beneficial
alone. Clearly the questionnaire used to attempt patients felt their therapy was (e.g. Do you feel
to measure handicap will influence the outcome the communication course has been beneficial?).
of any study. However, the efficacy of communi- These later alternatives are clearly open to a
cation courses has now been demonstrated in halo effect due to greater contact time. There-
three controlled trials: each used a different ques- fore, while this criticism cannot be completely
tionnaire to measure self-perceived handicap refuted there is good evidence for the communi-
(Smaldino and Smaldino, 1988; Abrams et al., cation course having a direct effect on hearing
1992; present study). The common feature of handicap rather than any feel good factor due to
these studies is that they were performed in receiving a large amount of therapy.
groups and involved little formal auditory train- This study evaluated the benefit of a communi-
ing; neither the present study not that of Abrams cation course on first time hearing aid wearers.
et al. (1992) contained any auditory training; that These findings would suggest that first time hear-
by Smaldino and Smaldino (1988) had one of four ing aid wearers would benefit from inclusion in a
sessions focusing on auditory training. It may be, communication course. It is unlikely that many
then, that courses which use a broad range of departments have the resources for such a service
techniques, focus on solving difficulties in partic- and therefore having established the efficacy of
ular situations and allow group discussion of cop- such courses identification of those patients who
ing with a hearing loss, may prove more would benefit the most is required. This study did
beneficial than those only utilizing one auditory not find any trends for the reduction in handicap
training technique. Which components of such a to vary with either age or severity of hearing loss.
communication are responsible for the reduction However, the study only included patients up to
in handicap and which components may be omit- the age of 80, and with hearing losses that were
ted is a subject for further research. either mild or moderate; therefore further
The main criticism of this type of study is that research in these areas is required.
the observed reduction in handicap in the group
undergoing additional rehabilitation is due to a References
placebo effect. The reported benefit could simply Abrams HB, Hnath-Chisolm T, Guerreiro SM,
be due to the larger amount of contact time the Ritterman SI. The effects of intervention strat-
patient has received rather than the content of the egy on self-perception of hearing handicap.
communication course itself. This same criticism Ear Hear 1992; 13: 371-377.
is true of the studies mentioned above and is Alpiner JG, Chevrette W, Glascoe G, Metz M,
extremely difficult to avoid as a suitable placebo Olsen B. The Denver Scale of Communication
Communication course for hearing aid users 35 1
Function. Unpublished materials. University Schow RL, Nerbonne MA. Hearing handicap
of Denver, 1971. and Denver scales: applications,categories and
Kricos PB, Holmes AE. Efficacy of audiologic interpretation. J Acad Rehab Audiol 1980; 13:
rehabilitation for older adults. J Am Acad 66-77.
Audiol, 1996;7: 219-229. Smaldino SE, SmaldinoJJ. The influence of aural
Kricos P, Holmes A, Doyle D. Efficacy of a com- rehabilitation and cognitive style disclosure on
munication training program for hearing the perception of hearing handicap. J Acad
impaired elderly adults. J Acad Rehab Audiol Rehab Audiol 1988;21: 54-67.
1992;25: 69-80. Tuley MR, Mulrow CD, Aguillar C, Velez R. A
Lamb S, Owens E, Schubert E, Giolas T. Hearing critical reevaluation of the Quantified Denver
Performance Inventory. San Francisco: San Scale of Communication Function. Ear Hear
Francisco State University, 1979. 1990; 11:56-61.
Mulrow CD, Tuley MR, Aguilar C. Sustained Ventry IM, Weinstein BE. The Hearing Handi-
benefits of hearing aids. J Speech Hear Res cap Inventory for the Elderly: a new tool. Ear
1992;35: 1402-1405. Hear 1982;3: 128-134.
Newman CW, Weinstein BE. The hearing Walden BE, Erdman SA, Montgomery AA,
handicap inventory for the elderly as a mea- SchwartzDM, Prosek RA. Some effects of train-
sure of hearing aid benefit. Ear Hear 1988; 9: ing on speech recognition by hearing impaired
81-85. adults.J Speech Hear Res 1981;24: 207-216.
Rubinstein A, Boothroyd A. Effect of two Wertz RT. Adult-onset disorders. Am Speech
approaches to auditory training on speech Lang Hear Assoc 1993; 35: 38-39.
recognition by hearing impaired adults. J Wilson BA. How do we know that rehabilitation
Speech Hearing Res 1987;30153-161. works? Neuropsychol Rehab 1993;3: 1-4.

También podría gustarte