Está en la página 1de 13

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

Preface

Task conict, information processing, and decision-making: The


damaging effect of relationship conict
Frank R.C. de Wit a,, Karen A. Jehn b, Daan Scheepers c
a
Leiden University, Institute for Psychological Research, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands
b
Melbourne Business School, 50 Leicester Street, 3053 Carlton, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
c
Leiden University, Institute for Psychological Research, Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, P.O. Box 9555, 2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: A popular theoretical assumption holds that task-related disagreements stimulate critical thinking, and
Received 29 June 2011 thus may improve group decision making. Two recent meta-analyses showed, however, that task conict
Accepted 14 July 2013 can have a positive effect, a negative effect, or no effect at all on decision-making quality (De Dreu &
Weingart, 2003; De Wit, Greer, & Jehn, 2012). In two studies, we built upon the suggestion of both
Accepted by Richard Moreland
meta-analyses that the presence of relationship conict determines whether a task conict is positively
or negatively related to decision making. We hypothesized and found that the level of perceived relation-
Keywords:
ship conict during task conict (Study 1), and the actual presence (vs. absence) of relationship conict
Task conict
Relationship conict
during task conict (Study 2), increased group members rigidity in holding onto suboptimal initial pref-
Information processing erences during decision making and thus led to poor decisions. In both studies the effect of relationship
Decision making conict on decision making was mediated by biased use of information.
2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction Dreu and Weingart, the ndings of De Wit et al. showed that task
conict and group performance were more positively related
Many researchers and practitioners have argued and found that among studies where the association between task and relation-
task-related disagreements can stimulate critical thinking, and ship conict was relatively weak rather than strong.
thus may improve group decision making (e.g., Amason, 1996; These results suggest that groups are more likely to benet
Deutsch, 1973; Jehn, 1995; Nemeth, 1995; Schulz-Hardt, Brodbeck, from a task conict when it occurs in the absence (vs. presence)
Mojzisch, Kerschreiter, & Frey, 2006). In contrast to these ndings, of a relationship conict (e.g., Shaw et al., 2011). Yet meta-analyses
two meta-analyses of the intragroup conict literature found no can only suggest inferences at the study level, not at the group or
support for an overall positive association between task conict individual level, and so the two meta-analyses could not test this
(disagreements that follow from different task-related viewpoints) hypothesis directly, nor could they investigate the processes that
and group performance (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; De Wit, Greer, may underlie the damaging effect of relationship conict (cf. Lau
& Jehn, 2012). Although De Dreu and Weingarts initial meta-anal- & Cobb, 2010). To ll this void, we present here two studies of
ysis of 30 studies on intragroup conict even showed an overall how relationship conict impairs the link between task conict
negative association between task conict and group performance, and group decision-making. We propose that the presence of rela-
a more recent meta-analysis of 116 studies by De Wit, Greer, et al. tionship conict during a task conict has two important conse-
(2012) showed that overall, the association between task conict quences. First, it makes group members more likely to rigidly
and group performance is neither negative nor positive. Moreover, retain an initially preferred decision alternative. Second, it makes
De Wit et al.s results showed that the association between task group members process information in a biased manner, using
conict and group performance depends on moderating factors their own information during decision making, rather than the
such as the association between task conict and relationship con- information they could (or do) receive from other group members.
ict (disagreements that arise from interpersonal incompatibilities By examining the damaging effect of relationship conict on the
and include affective elements such as feeling friction and tension; linkages among task conict, information processing, and decision
Jehn, 1994). More specically, in line with earlier ndings by De making, our research claries one of the circumstances that may
help or hinder the potential benecial effect of task conict on
Corresponding author. group performance (e.g., Behfar & Thompson, 2007; De Dreu,
E-mail addresses: FWit@fsw.leidenuniv.nl (F.R.C. de Wit), K.Jehn@mbs.edu (K.A. 2008; De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; Jehn & Bendersky, 2003). In addi-
Jehn), Scheepersdt@fsw.leidenuniv.nl (D. Scheepers). tion, it extends experimental research on task conict and group

0749-5978/$ - see front matter 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.07.002
178 F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

decision making, which so far has neglected to examine the role of to an escalation of commitment and even positional bargaining,
relationship conict (e.g., Scholten, Van Knippenberg, Nijstad, & De in which parties argue for a certain task-related viewpoint as a goal
Dreu, 2007; Schulz-Hardt et al., 2006). Furthermore, our research in itself, regardless of any underlying interests (Fisher & Ury, 1981).
extends correlational research on the consequences of task and Relationship conicts that are not directly related to a task con-
relationship conict (e.g., Jehn, 1995) by experimentally inducing ict, but instead arise independently from that task conict, are
both task and relationship conict. likely to cause rigidity as well. When debates about more personal
matters create friction, negative emotions, and interpersonal ani-
mosity (all the ingredients of a relationship conict), these prob-
Conict and rigidity in decision making
lems can easily spill over to determine how group members
react to a task-related debate. The presence of a relationship con-
When group members work toward a common goal (e.g.,
ict, for instance, may encourage hostile interpretations of task-re-
designing a new product, or maximizing the return on investment),
lated viewpoints, thereby creating a self-fullling prophecy of
a task conict may arise when members have diverging task-re-
mutual hostility and conict escalation (Simons & Peterson,
lated viewpoints. Our research focuses on two different reasons
2000, p. 104). Hence, instead of approaching a task-related debate
why task conict and relationship conict can co-occur. First, there
with an open-mind, group members may become more competi-
may be misinterpretations of a task conict as a relationship con-
tive during a task conict and reduce their willingness to consider
ict. This can happen when disagreement about a task is taken too
and use the viewpoints of their fellow group members (e.g.,
personally, causing group members to feel that they also disagree
Janssen, Van De Vliert, & Veenstra, 1999).
on a more personal level. Second, an unrelated relationship conict
Indeed, ample research on dyadic and group conict suggests
can arise during a task conict, due to disagreements about diverg-
that competitive mindsets can lead to rigidity in the form of dis-
ing political or artistic preferences, or from incompatible personal-
tributive bargaining, derogation of counterparts, and a reluctance
ities. We propose that in both cases, the presence of relationship
to disconrm initial preferences (De Dreu, 2008; De Dreu,
conict is likely to cause increased rigidity during a task conict.
Weingart, & Kwon, 2000; Rubin, Pruitt, & Kim, 1994; Tjosvold,
Task conicts are easily misinterpreted as relationship conicts
1998; Toma & Butera, 2009). Given all this, we propose that during
because task-related viewpoints often become strongly intertwined
group decision-making, group members are more likely to rigidly
with group members self-views, and people quickly develop feel-
hold onto their initial opinions when they encounter a task conict
ings of ownership over their viewpoints (e.g., De Dreu & Van
in the presence (compared to the absence) of a relationship conict
Knippenberg, 2005). Self-verication theory suggests that scrutiny
(Hypothesis 1).
and/or rejection of task-related viewpoints can seem like a negative
assessment of the self (e.g., Swann, Polzer, Seyle, & Ko, 2004).
Hence, during task conict, group members may feel that they are Conict and biases in information processing
being criticized at a more personal level. As a result, task-related
debates can be perceived as personal attacks and misinterpreted as As shown in Fig. 1, one possible process through which group
relationship conicts (e.g., Fisher & Ury, 1981; Jehn, 1997; Simons members can become more rigid in holding onto their initial opin-
& Peterson, 2000; Torrance, 1957; Yang & Mossholder, 2004). ions, is that group members process information in a more biased
This misinterpretation of a task conict as a relationship conict manner when they encounter a task conict in the presence of
is likely to cause counterproductive cognitions and behaviors relationship conict (e.g., Janssen et al., 1999). For example, rela-
(Simons & Peterson, 2000). In response to attacks onto their self- tionship conict may cause group members to focus on informa-
views, individuals often become defensive (e.g., Baumeister, Smart, tion that they possess themselves and that supports their initial
& Boden, 1996) and make a shift from a cooperative mindset to- viewpoint, rather than on information that they receive from other
wards a more competitive mindset (De Dreu & Van Knippenberg, group members and that may possibly be inconsistent with their
2005). Although competitive mindsets may assist group members own viewpoint. As a result of this biased information processing,
in protecting and maintaining their self-concepts, they often come group members may be less likely to doubt their own initially pre-
at the expense of nding a mutually agreeable solution (e.g., ferred decisions and therefore hold onto these initial decisions
Deutsch, 1973). More specically, competitive mindsets may lead (e.g., Brodbeck, Kerschreiter, Mojzisch, & Schulz-Hardt, 2007).

Motivation to
Process
- Information -
Systematically

Presence (vs. Absence) Biased +


+ Rigidity in
of Relationship Conflict Information
Decision-making
during a Task Conflict Processing
+
+

The Appraisal of
the Task Conflict
as a Threat

Fig. 1. Effects of the presence (vs. absence) of a relationship conict during a task conict on information processing and decision-making.
F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189 179

As also shown in Fig. 1, two distinct processes could explain functioning, because the anxiety triggered by the conict narrows
why group members process task-related information in a biased group members elds of attention and reduces the number of
manner when they are experiencing relationship conict. First, in information channels that they use (e.g., Kamphuis, 2010; Staw,
line with the motivated information processing in groups model Sandelands, & Dutton, 1981). For example, research suggests that
(MIP-G; De Dreu, Nijstad, & van Knippenberg, 2008), group mem- anxiety causes individuals to hold onto initial viewpoints as well
bers might deliberately choose not to use ideas and information as to focus on information that corresponds to these viewpoints
from other group members, because the presence of a relationship (De Wit, Scheepers, & Jehn, 2012; Fischer et al., 2011; Kassam
conict reduces their motivation to process information systemat- et al., 2009; Staw et al., 1981). Given all this, we propose that dur-
ically during a task conict. Second, group members might process ing group decision-making, group members are less likely to pro-
information in a biased manner because they feel more threatened cess the information provided by other group members when
when a task conict co-occurs with a relationship conict, causing they encounter a task conict in the presence (compared to the ab-
everyone to focus primarily on information that is consistent with sence) of a relationship conict (Hypothesis 2).
their initial preferences (e.g., Fischer et al., 2011; Kamphuis, 2010; This biased information processing probably mediates the effect
Kassam, Koslov, & Mendes, 2009). of the presence (compared to the absence) of relationship conict
According to the MIP-G (De Dreu et al., 2008) group members on group members rigidity in group decision-making (Hypothesis
motivation to systematically process information is determined 3). That is, when group members fail to process information from
by their epistemic motivation as well as their social motivation. others, they forgo the opportunity to learn from information that
Epistemic motivation is dened as the willingness to expend effort may contradict their initial viewpoint. As a result, group members
to achieve a thorough and accurate understanding of the task at may be less likely to question their initial preferences and, hence,
hand. Social motivation is dened as an individuals preference be more likely to stick to decisions that are consistent with these
for particular distributions of outcomes for the self vs. others. initial preferences (e.g., Brodbeck et al., 2007).
The MIP-G model postulates that epistemic motivation drives the In line with the model depicted in Fig. 1, we propose that two
degree to which information is attended to, whereas social motiva- distinct factors, in turn, might mediate the effect of the presence
tion drives the kind of information that individuals attend to. Social (compared to the absence) of a relationship conict on biased
motivation, for example, may determine whether group members information processing. First, in line with the aforementioned re-
focus primarily on information supporting their preferred alterna- search on motivated information processing in groups (e.g., Schol-
tive or on information that could integrate different decision alter- ten et al., 2007), more biased information processing could be
natives (De Dreu et al., 2008). caused by reduced motivation to process information systemati-
The presence of relationship conict is likely to reduce group cally. Second, and in line with research on threat and biases in
members epistemic motivation. A recent meta-analysis, for exam- information processing (e.g., Fischer et al., 2011; Kassam et al.,
ple, showed that relationship conict is negatively related to group 2009), more biased information processing could be caused by
members commitment to the group (De Wit, Greer, et al., 2012). group members appraisal of the task conict as a threat.
Therefore, in the presence (vs. absence) of relationship conict,
group members might disengage from the group task and ignore
what other group members have to say. In addition to reducing Our research
epistemic motivation, the presence of relationship conict is also
likely to affect group members social motivation. In line with The aim of our research is to investigate whether group mem-
the MIP-G, the presence of relationship conict may elicit a more bers are indeed less likely to use the viewpoints provided by others
competitive social motivation and thereby reduce group members in their decision making, and more likely to be rigid during deci-
motivation to attend to information that would facilitate consen- sion making, when a task conict occurs in the presence (com-
sus and the integration of perspectives (e.g., Van Kleef & De Dreu, pared to absence) of a relationship conict. To examine
2002). Put differently, the presence of relationship conict may information processing and decision making, we asked participants
cause group members to be more concerned with winning the to work on a hidden prole task (see Stasser & Titus, 1985; Toma &
debate, instead of developing an accurate and deep understanding Butera, 2009). In a hidden prole task, some pieces of information
of the decision problems at hand (Fisher & Ury, 1981). needed for the decision are shared among group members whereas
As a result, in the presence of relationship conict, individuals other pieces of information are unshared. When all information
may increase their attention to the task-related arguments of oth- available to the group is considered, group members should be able
ers, but only in the hope of detecting aws in those arguments. to derive the correct solution. Yet no group member can identify
Therefore, despite increased attention to others task-related view- that solution on the basis of his or her individual information
points, group members will not really process task-related argu- alone. The use of others information, as well as the disconrma-
ments systematically (for example, to nd a mutually benecial tion of group members own initial preferences, is thus required
solution). Instead, they will only be motivated to use information to derive the correct solution (Schulz-Hardt et al., 2006).
that supports their initial viewpoints, and/or information that Participants were asked to solve the hidden-prole task in small
depreciates the viewpoints of others (e.g., Brodbeck et al., 2007; groups, each of which contained one participant and two confeder-
Greitemeyer & Schulz-Hardt, 2003). In sum, when a task conict ates who were trained to behave in certain ways. The use of confeder-
co-occurs with a relationship conict, group members may not ates enabled us to experimentally induce a task conict. We
be motivated to thoroughly process the alternative perspectives operationalized task conict (see Jehn, 1994) as an overt disagree-
of other group members, due to a lower commitment to the group ment about the solution for a task. We induced task conict by having
and a more competitive social motivation. the confederates clearly state that they disagreed with the partici-
In addition to this motivational explanation, group members pants preferred solution, explicitly mention their own preferred solu-
bias towards their own viewpoints may also result from the anxi- tion to the task, and explain exactly why they preferred their solution.
ety that is triggered by relationship conict. Workplace conict Group members interacted with confederates for two reasons.
tends to elicit anxiety (e.g., Dijkstra, Van Dierendonck, & Evers, The rst reason was to keep factors such as the content, duration,
2005; Narayanan, Menon, & Spector, 1999), especially when the and intensity of the task conict constant across participants. The
conict becomes personal and emotional (e.g., Jehn, 1997). The second reason was to ensure that group members discussed all
presence of relationship conict, therefore, may impair cognitive the unshared information necessary to derive the correct solution,
180 F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

and that participants also knew about the information that con- person. A hidden prole was constructed by allocating three criti-
tradicted their initial opinion. That is, in hidden-prole situations, cal unshared items to each group member. Based on these items,
group members often stick to their initially preferred solutions be- each group member was oriented to a specic initial preference
cause they fail to discuss their unshared information and focus on (Mr. X, Mrs. Y, or Mr. Z). To derive the correct solution, participants
what information they have in common (Gigone & Hastie, 1993; were required to use the unshared information of other group
Stasser & Titus, 1985). By experimentally controlling the reactions members and to disconrm their own initial preferences. All par-
of the confederates to the real participants preferences, we could ticipants were directed to the same initial solution (Mr. X); other
ensure that individuals rigidity in holding onto their initial opinion group members ostensibly believed that Mrs. Y or Mr. Z was the
was not due to the groups failure to share information. Instead, culprit.
rigidity could only result from an individuals tendency (not) to
process or use certain pieces of information (see Greitemeyer & Procedures and independent variable
Schulz-Hardt, 2003 for a similar procedure).
We designed Study 1 to examine how the misinterpretation of When participants arrived in the lab, they were told that they
task conict as a relationship conict can inuence participants were going to work on a decision-making task with two other
information processing and decision making. In Study 2, we used people (who had not yet arrived). Participants and confederates
the same induction of task conict, but in combination with an were seated in separate cubicles and told that they would work
experimentally induced relationship conict (or no relationship on the task as a group via the computer system. The participants
conict). We designed Study 2 to assess how the actual presence were instructed to rst study the road accident case individually
(vs. absence) of a relationship conict during task conict altered and to decide who the guilty person was. They were provided
information processing and decision making. with the 19 shared items, along with three unshared items that
oriented them towards a specic suspect (Mr. X). The shared
Study 1 information did not make any of the suspects seem more guilty
than the other suspects. Participants knew which of their own
In the rst study we examined how information processing and items were shared and which were unshared, and they were
subsequent rigidity in decision making are affected by the extent aware that the other two group members would receive different
to which group members perceive relationship conict during a unshared items. This transparency was important because it al-
task conict. We expected that the extent to which group mem- lowed participants to deliberately choose not to use other mem-
bers perceived relationship conict during a task conict would bers unshared information during their decision making (Toma &
be positively related to their tendency to rigidly hold onto their Butera, 2009). Participants were given 1.5 min to nd a solution.1
initial opinions (Hypothesis 1), and to their biased information Next, they were invited to present their decision in front of a web-
processing (using their own information rather than information cam, and give a clear reason why they made that decision (Record-
they received from other group members) (Hypothesis 2). More- ing 1). Participants were told that their statement would be
over, we expected that biased information processing would medi- recorded; that the other members of the group would watch their
ate the effect of the level of perceived relationship conict on recording; that other group members would react to their record-
group members tendency to hold onto their initial opinions ing; and that they (the participants) would be able to watch the
(Hypothesis 3). Finally, we expected that the more group members reactions of the other group members and then come up with a -
perceived relationship conict during a task conict, they less nal decision. This set-up (as opposed to a real discussion) was used
likely they would be to use the information provided by other to control the task situation and to standardize it across partici-
group members because (i) they would be less motivated to pro- pants (see Greitemeyer & Schulz-Hardt, 2003 for a similar
cess information systematically (Hypothesis 4a) and (ii) they procedure).
would be more likely to appraise the task conict as a threat
(Hypothesis 4b). Induction of task conict

Participants and design After participants announced their initial decision, there was a
short waiting period, after which we induced the task conict by
A total of 82 participants (50 women, 32 men) took part in this means of the pre-recorded reactions of the other group members.
study in return for a monetary award (6 Euros) or the partial fulll- In these recordings, each of the other group members openly sta-
ment of a course requirement. There were no experimental condi- ted his or her disagreement with the participants initial solution.
tions; we induced the same task conict for all participants and We made sure that each group member had a different solution
then measured the level of perceived relationship conict during in mind (so that none of the possible answers was favored by the
that conict as the independent variable. majority of members), and that all previously unshared informa-
tion was shared during the conict. The confederates portrayed
Decision task the disagreement in a non-emotional way; they were instructed
not to make any gestures that could signal emotions (e.g., anger
Participants worked on a hidden prole task. This task was or frustration), to express their opinion in a neutral tone of voice,
adapted from Toma and Butera (2009) and concerned a road acci- and to keep a neutral face (e.g., no frowns).
dent investigation. Four persons were potential suspects. Based on Based on the unshared items that participants originally re-
a set of nine clues, three of these persons could be exonerated (Mr. ceived (see Appendix A), we expected participants to argue that
X, Mrs. Y, and Mr. Z) and the fourth (Mr. Xs son) incriminated. The Mr. X was the culprit. When that happened, the reaction of the rst
task involved 28 items of information in all. Nineteen of these were
1
shared (given to everyone in the group) and nine were unshared The reason we gave participants so little time was because we wanted them to (a)
(given to only one group member) (see Appendix A). The 19 shared spend roughly the same time reading the information, (ii) feel some time pressure;
and (c) avoid feeling bored. Considering the average reading time of an adult (about
items described the circumstances of the accident and some spe- 200 words per minute), and the number of words that the text contained (179 words),
cic characteristics of the suspects. On the basis of the nine un- the average participant should have been able to read all of the text at least once
shared items, participants could identify Mr. Xs son as the guilty within these 1.5 min.
F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189 181

(female) group member was: Hi all, I dont agree, I dont think Mis- solution, when a task conict co-occurred with a relationship
ter X did it. My information said that the guilty person is less than 30 conict.2
years old. Due to inexperience, the guilty person wasnt able to avoid
the collision. Also it said that the guilty person claimed that he or Information processing
she did not see others approaching the intersection. Therefore I The presentations of participants nal decisions (Recording 2)
thought it was Mrs. Y. . . were transcribed by two coders (trained by the rst author) who
A reaction from the second (male) group member followed. This were blind to the research hypotheses. These coders counted
person also disagreed with the participant, but chose Mr. Z as the the number of pieces of unshared information that were men-
most likely culprit. The second group member said: Mhmm.. I tioned by each participant. The reliability of their coding was sub-
dont agree either, I dont think its X.. But I had Mr. Z because my info stantial (Cohens Kappa = .71, p < .001); any disagreements were
said that the guilty person was a man and that a family member was resolved through discussion between the coders and the rst
indirectly responsible for the accident and it said that the guilty person author.
was driving at 110km/h. . . After the coding was completed, we determined which of the
To check whether all this debate was indeed perceived to be a nine unshared items participants used to support their nal deci-
task conict, participants were asked to rate their agreement (right sions. Next, we determined how many of these items the partici-
after hearing the reactions of other group members to their opin- pants had initially received themselves, and how many they had
ion) using three items adapted from Jehn, Greer, Levine, and Szu- later received from other group members. We then determined
lanski (2008). These items were The solutions of my teammates the proportion of unshared information from the self vs. others
are different from my own solution, The guilty person that my that was used to support a nal decision. This was accomplished
teammates have in mind is different from the guilty person that I by dividing the number of unshared items that were provided to
have in mind and Within our team we disagree about the solution participants by other group members by the total number of un-
of this dilemma, a = .68. All of the items were always presented in shared items that participants used to support their nal decision.
the same order, and there were no time-constraints on how long So, for example, if a participant used four unshared items to sup-
participants could take to answer them. port his or her nal decision, and three of these items came from
Next, we checked the extent to which participants perceived other group members, then the resulting score was 3=4 or .75.
relationship conict during the task conict. Participants were
asked to rate their agreement with ve statements regarding the Motivation to process information systematically
amount of relationship conict they perceived. The items were We measured participants motivation to process information
adapted from Jehn (1995) and were I felt somewhat irritated by systematically with the following four items adapted from De
the response of my teammates, The exchange of our preferences Dreu, Koole, and Oldersma (1999) When solving the dilemma, I
got a bit personal, The comments of my teammates were not hardly thought about all the information I had received(reverse
really helpful, I think I can get along well with my teammates, coded), I tried to use and combine all available information in
and I think our personalities do not work well together my solution, I have not paid much attention to the information
(a = .74). After this, we measured the appraisal of a task conict that my teammates gave me (reverse coded), and I tried to utilize
as a threat (see below for more details), and participants were all available information in my solution (a = .77).
asked to present their individual nal decisions by means of a web-
cam recording and to provide explanations for their decisions Conict-related threat appraisals
(Recording 2). Finally, participants were asked to ll in a short sur- According to Tomaka, Blascovich, Kelsey, and Leitten (1993),
vey, including our measure of their motivation to process informa- and Blascovich and Tomaka (1996), appraisal of a situation as a
tion systematically (see for more details below), and to provide a threat depends on a persons beliefs about (a) the demands of
nal rank-order of the four persons who seemed most likely to the situation, and (b) the persons resources for coping with these
be the culprit (Mr. X, Mrs. Y, Mr. Z, or the Son of Mr. X). After that, demands. Therefore, we used four items adapted from Tomaka
they were debriefed, paid and thanked for their participation. et al. (1993). Two of the items concerned the perceived demands
of the task conict (i.e., It is difcult to get past the differences be-
tween our solutions and It is stressful that our solutions are so
Dependent measures different) and the other two concerned the resources available
to cope with the task conict (I think that I am able to solve the
Rigidity in decision making differences between our solutions and Despite our different solu-
The primary dependent variable was the nal decision that was tions, I think I am able to nd the right solution to this dilemma).
made. Following Toma and Butera (2009), the nal decision was a In line with the BPSM, we reverse-coded the two items measuring
categorical measure expressing whether participants chose the perceived resources, and then combined them with the two items
decision reecting rigidity (Mr. X), the correct decision (Son of measuring perceived demands to form one scale (a = .79), such
Mr. X), or a decision that reected yielding (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z). Mr.
X was considered as the rigid decision, because participants who 2
We performed a pilot test to investigate whether the task conict manipulation
made this decision stuck with their initial solution, despite dis- facilitated superior decision making, compared to a no task conict (agreement)
agreements with the other group members and the unshared infor- manipulation. In the no task conict condition, we slightly altered the messages,
mation provided by those persons. Mrs. Y and Mr. Z were changing the messages from signaling disagreement to signaling agreement, and
considered to be yielding decisions because participants who instead of video-messages, in the pilot test we used text messages (as in Study 2). The
task conict manipulation indeed facilitated superior decision making. Whereas in
chose Mrs. Y or Mr. Z changed their initial decision (Mr. X) in def-
the task conict condition, 46.8% of the participants derived the correct solution, in
erence of one their group members preferred decisions. Given the the no task conict condition, 26.9% were able to derive the correct solution (N = 114,
three decision types, our dependent variable allowed us to exam- v2 = 4.74, p < .05). The texts used in task conict absent condition were: Hi all, I agree
ine whether an increased tendency to hold onto their initial opin- and also think it is Mister X. Yet, my information did say that the guilty person is less than
ion comes at the expense of individuals tendency to yield and/or 30 years old and that due to inexperience, the guilty person wasnt able to avoid the
collision. Also it said that the guilty person claimed that he or she did not see others
their tendency to derive the correct solution. We expected that approaching the intersection. . ., and I also think it is Mister X because my info said that
compared to holding onto an initial opinion, individuals would the guilty person was a man and that a family member was indirectly responsible for the
be less likely to yield, as well as less likely to derive the correct accident and it said that the guilty person was driving at 110km/h. . ..
182 F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

Table 1
Multinomial logistic regressions examining the decisions made in Study 1.

Predictor Model 1 Model 2


B SE OR Wald B SE OR Wald
Correct solution (Son of Mr. X) vs. incorrect rigidity solution (Mr. X)
Intercept 2.83** .97 8.52 .91 1.34 .46
Perceived relationship conict 1.27** .39 .28 10.90 .83* .41 .43 4.09
Information use .06*** .02 1.07 14.31
Incorrect yielding solution (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z) vs. incorrect rigidity solution (Mr. X)
Intercept 2.06* 1.03 4.01 .20 1.28 .03
Perceived relationship conict 1.09** .41 .34 7.19 .73 .41 .48 3.19
Information use .04** .01 1.04 7.44
Correct solution (Son of Mr. X) vs. incorrect yielding solution (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z)
Intercept .76 .97 .62 .71 1.30 .30
Perceived relationship conict .18 .42 .83 .19 .11 .41 .90 .07
Information use .02 .01 1.02 2.38
Chi-square 17.63*** 39.56***
df 2 4
2 log likelihood 77.08 112.69
Cox and Snell pseudo R2 .20 .40
Sample size 81 77

Note. OR = odds ratio.

p < .10.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

that higher values indicated greater conict-related threat Information processing


appraisals.
To test our second hypothesis, we regressed the unshared infor-
mation used by participants to support their nal decision (relative
Results to their use of their own unshared information) on the extent to
which people perceived relationship conict during the task con-
Manipulation checks and descriptive statistics ict. In support of the hypothesis, we found that the relative use
of the unshared information that participants received from the
Recall that to facilitate our manipulation of task conict, partic- other group members was negatively related to the extent to
ipants initially received unshared information that directed them which participants perceived relationship conict during the task
to one specic answer category (Mr. X). To check whether partici- conict, b = .38, t(76) = 3.524, p < .001, R2 = .14.3
pants indeed opted for Mr. X, we content-analyzed the video-
recordings to identify their initial solution. The results showed that
except for one participant, all of the 82 participants initially
thought that it was Mr. X who caused the accident (the one partic- Mediation analyses
ipant who did not chose Mr. X was excluded from further analy-
ses). The results showed that the manipulation of task conict To test our third hypothesis, which focused on mediation, we
was successful; the average level of reported task conict was high followed the three-step procedure suggested by Baron and Kenny
and signicantly higher than the midpoint (4) of the rating scale (1986). The rst step was to establish that our independent vari-
(M = 6.54, SD = .68, t(80) = 33.63, p < .001). able (the perceived level of relationship conict during the task
conict) affected our main dependent variable (the tendency to
hold onto an initial opinion). The second step was to establish that
Rigidity in decision making our independent variable affected our proposed mediating variable
(the tendency to use the information received other group mem-
To test our rst hypothesis, we estimated a multinomial logistic bers). The third and nal step was to establish that the effect of
regression predicting the answer categories, with the extent to our independent variable on the main dependent variable was sig-
which people perceived relationship conict during the task con- nicantly reduced when controlling for the mediating variable. The
ict as a predictor variable. Model 1, in Table 1 showed that, in line results presented earlier showed that the perceived level of rela-
with Hypothesis 1, the extent to which group members perceived tionship conict during the task conict indeed predicted individ-
the task conict as a relationship conict had a signicant inu- uals tendency to hold onto an initial opinion, as well as the
ence on their decision making (v2 = 17.63, df = 2, p < .001). Specif- tendency to use the information received from other group mem-
ically, participants were (1/.281=) 3.56 times more likely to stick bers. To test the third step, we estimated a multinomial logistic
to their incorrect initial solution (i.e., rigidity), instead of choosing regression predicting the answer categories, with two predictor
the correct solution, with every one-point increase in the extent to variables: (i) the extent to which relationship conict was per-
which they perceived relationship conict during the task conict. ceived during the task conict, and (ii) the proportion of other
Similarly, participants were three times more likely to hold onto
their initial opinion, instead of adopting one of the solutions of 3
Identical results were obtained when we conducted these analyses with the
the other group members (yielding) with every one-point increase absolute (instead of the relative) extent to which participants used the unique
in perceived relationship conict. information provided by other group members.
F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189 183

group members vs. own unique information that was used during The results also showed strong support for our second and third
decision making (information use). hypotheses. Participants were less likely to use information pro-
As can be seen in Table 1, Model 2, the effect of the perceived vided by other group members when they perceived a relatively
level of relationship conict during the task conict on decision high level of relationship conict during a task conict. This, in
making was mediated by information processing. That is, when turn, explained why they were less likely to adjust their initial
information processing was entered in the model, the effect of per- opinion. Study 1 further revealed that group members become less
ceived relationship conict was reduced, both for the correct solu- motivated to process information, and also more likely to appraise
tion vs. the incorrect rigidity solution and for the incorrect the task conict as a threat, when they perceived a relatively high
rigidity solution vs. the incorrect yielding solution. We also con- level of relationship conict. In line with our fourth hypothesis,
ducted bootstrapping analyses, as described by Preacher and Hayes and the motivated information processing in groups model (De
(2004), for estimating the direct and indirect effects of perceived Dreu et al., 2008), group members reduced motivation to process
relationship conict on participants tendency to stick to their ini- information led to lower use of the information from others. In line
tial opinion. Using bootstrapping we derived a condence interval with our fourth hypothesis, and research linking threat to conr-
for the indirect effect; this provides a more accurate estimate of the matory information search (e.g., Fischer et al., 2011), group mem-
indirect effect with small-to-moderate samples size than does the bers appraisal of the task conict as a threat, also mediated the
Sobel test (see Shrout & Bolger, 2002). The indirect effect is signif- effect on information processing, such that group members be-
icant at p < .05 if the 95% condence interval does not include the came less likely to use the information from other group members
value of zero. As seen in Table 2, the test conrmed that the medi- the more they appraised the task conict as a threat.
ation was signicant. Hence, the third hypothesis was supported. In Study 1, we measured (rather than manipulated) the spontane-
ous interpretation of task conict as relationship conict. That is,
participants did not receive explicit information about a relationship
Motivation to process information and threat appraisal conict. Hence, it is difcult to attribute the relationship conict to
Finally, we tested whether motivation (Hypothesis 4a) and the other factors than task conict, given that the groups were dened
appraisal of the task conict as a threat (Hypothesis 4b) could ex- in terms of the task, and participants didnt have any other informa-
plain why participants made less use of other group members tion about group members except from the task-relevant informa-
information when they perceived more relationship conict during tion they shared. This converges with many real-life situations
the task conict. Using regression analyses, we rst established where relationship conict often arises merely due to misinterpre-
that the extent to which group members perceived relationship tation of task conict as relationship conict (e.g., Fisher & Ury,
conict during the task conict was negatively related to their 1981; Janssen et al., 1999; Shaw et al., 2011; Simons & Peterson,
motivation to process information systematically, b = .42, 2000). Nevertheless, it is difcult to tell where the individual differ-
t(1, 80) = 4.15, p < .001, and positively related to the appraisal of ences in perceived relationship conict came from. We could not
the task conict as a threat, b = .36, t(1, 78) = 3.43, p < .001. Next, isolate the precise source of the variability in perceived relationship
and in line with both hypotheses 4a and 4b, we found that after conict. That is why we turned to an experimental design in Study 2.
entering motivation (b = .56, t(75) = 6.006, p < .001) and the apprai- The aims of our second study were twofold. First, we sought to
sal of the task conict as a threat (b = .33, t(75) = 3.559, replicate the ndings of Study 1, and to establish more causal sup-
p < .001), the main effect of perceived relationship conict on infor- port for the ndings by experimentally controlling the presence of
mation processing was indeed reduced (b = .047, t(75) = .469, relationship conict. A possible limitation of the rst study is that
p = .64). As seen in Table 2, subsequent bootstrapping analyses it did not provide causal support for the hypothesized relation-
showed these mediating effects were signicant. ships, only correlational evidence that the presence of a relation-
ship conict leads to more rigidity and biased information
processing during a task conict. Second, as we noted earlier, rela-
Discussion tionship conict does not always have to involve the misinterpre-
tation of task conict. It can also develop relatively independently
Study 1 revealed that the extent to which group members per- of task conict. Therefore, another aim of our second study was to
ceived relationship conict during a task conict was positively re- examine whether this second reason for co-occurring task and
lated to their rigidity in holding onto their initial preferences relationship conict could also account for the negative effects of
during decision making. This was in line with our rst hypothesis, relationship conict on information processing and decision mak-
and given that all available information was exchanged, showed ing. Many relationship conicts arise independently from a task
that the presence of a relationship conict augments individuals conict. For example, group members may have different norms,
tendency to hold onto a suboptimal preferences even when all values, political preferences and/or lifestyles (e.g., Jehn, 1997). In
available information is exchanged (see Brodbeck et al., 2007; these instances, the increased tendency of individuals to hold onto
Greitemeyer & Schulz-Hardt, 2003). their initially preferred decision alternative might be due to defen-

Table 2
Bias-corrected bootstrapped estimates of the mediations of Studies 1 and 2.

Bias-corrected bootstrapped 95% condence interval estimates


Study 1 Study 2
Mediation analysis 1a
TC and RC ? information use ? nal decision ( 1.446; .246)* ( .301; .046)*
a
Mediation analysis 2
TC and RC ? motivation to process information ? information use ( .115; .031)* ( .104; .002)*
TC and RC ? threat appraisal ? information use ( .063; .005)* ( .034; .024)

Note. Mediation analyses 1 examine the incorrect rigidity solution (Mr. X) against the correct solution (Son of Mr. X) and the incorrect yielding solution (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z)
together.
a
TC and RC stands for the co-occurrence of task conict (TC) and relationship conict (RC) and refers to the extent to which group members perceived relationship conict
during the task conict in Study 1, and the manipulation of the absence (vs. presence) of relationship conict in Study 2.
184 F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

sive and competitive cognitions and behaviors that are triggered by 60,00% Relationship Conflict Condition
the unrelated relationship conict, but spill over to the task conict No Relationship Conflict Condition
50,00%

Percentage of Participants
(e.g., Janssen et al., 1999). Therefore, we designed Study 2 to exam-
ine whether the damaging effects of relationship conict on infor-
40,00%
mation processing and decision making are also found when a
relationship conict arises independently form a task conict, 30,00%
rather than from misinterpretation of the task conict itself.
20,00%

Study 2 10,00%

In Study 2, we used the same experimental induction of task 0,00%


conict that was used in Study 1, but now in combination with Rigidity solution Correct solution Yielding solution
an experimental manipulation of the presence (vs. absence) of a (Mr. X) (son of Mr.X) (Mrs. Y or Mr.Z)

relationship conict. We again predicted that group members Fig. 2. Percentage of participants choosing the rigid (Mr. X), correct (Son of Mr. X),
would be less likely to adjust their initial opinion (Hypothesis 1), or yielding solution (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z) as a function of the presence (vs. absence) of
and make less use of the information given to them by other group relationship conict (Study 2).
members (Hypothesis 2), when they encountered a task conict in
the presence (compared to the absence) of a unrelated relationship complete text of these messages). In line with self-verication the-
conict. And we again predicted that information processing ory we expected that participants would feel this relative harsh
would mediate the effect of the presence (vs. absence) of relation- scrutiny of their artistic preferences as a negative assessment of
ship conict on group members tendency to hold onto their initial the self. We therefore expected this disagreement to elicit more
opinions (Hypothesis 3). Finally, we again predicted that biases in anxiety than just a general difference of opinion and to make group
information use would be due to a reduced motivation to process members more likely to take the disagreement personally, and
information systematically (Hypothesis 4a) and to the appraisal thus to perceive it as a relationship conict.
of task conict as a threat (Hypothesis 4b). Following the discussion of the paintings, participants were
presented with the same ve items used in Study 1 to measure
perceived relationship conict (a = .88). Thereafter, the same pro-
Participants, design, and procedure
cedure as in Study 1 commenced. The only difference was that
throughout the study, participants continued to communicate
109 undergraduate students participated in partial fulllment
through text-messages instead of video-recordings.
of a course requirement.4 The design consisted of one manipulated
factor with two levels (relationship conict present vs. relationship
conict absent). As in Study 1, we induced a task conict for all par- Results
ticipants. Participants were randomly assigned to the experimental
conditions. Manipulation checks
The decision-making task and the procedure to induce a task con-
ict were identical to the rst study. Instead of measuring relation- Task conict
ship conict, however, we manipulated the presence (vs. absence) of We content-analyzed the text messages to check whether par-
a relationship conict using a procedure adapted from Lcken and ticipants initially opted for Mr. X. The results showed that 105 of
Simon (2005). More specically, after participants were seated in the 109 participants initially thought that it was Mr. X who caused
separate cubicles, they were asked to rate the beauty (on a scale of the accident. As in Study 1, the data from four participants who did
zero to 100) of a set of paintings. This set contained paintings by four not opt for Mr. X were excluded from further analyses. Content
different artists, each represented by three paintings. The paintings analyses of the nal decisions showed that seven participants in
were presented one at a time on the participants computer screens. the relationship conict condition, and four participants in no-rela-
For each of the four artists, the computer automatically calculated tionship conict condition, did not provide a specic nal decision.
the participants average rating. The four resulting scores were then Instead, they simply stated (for example) all were guilty. These
presented to the participants on their computer screen, and the par- participants were thus excluded from the analyses. The nal sam-
ticipants preferred artist was clearly highlighted. Next, we invited ple contained 94 participants, divided equally across the two con-
participants to discuss their ratings with other group members as a ditions. Analyses of the perceived level of task conict (a = .76)
way for them to get to know each other. Participants communicated showed that our induction of task conict was again successful;
via instant messaging about their preferred artist, and about the rea- the average level of reported task conict was high, and signi-
sons why they liked that artist best. cantly higher than the midpoint on the scale (i.e., 4; M = 6.08,
At this point, we manipulated the presence vs. of absence of SD = 1.13, t(93) = 17.89, p < .001).
relationship conict. The participants in the non-relationship con-
ict condition were met with agreeable responses because the Relationship conict
other group members preferred the same artist that they did (see The manipulation of relationship conict was also successful.
Appendix B). In contrast, participants in the relationship conict Directly after the manipulation, participants in the relationship
condition were confronted with group members who preferred a conict condition (M = 3.74, SD = 1.31) reported higher levels of
different and who received negative personal messages from those relationship conict than did participants in the non-relationship
members about their own artistic preferences. For example, in the condition (M = 1.92, SD = .85), t(92) = 8.01, p < .001.5
relationship conict condition, when referring to the participants
preferred artist [artist 1], the group members argued I could prob- 5
Although a mean of 3.74 in the relationship conict present condition is still
ably produce stuff like those other artists and pretty much any po- relatively low, it is comparable and even higher than the levels of relationship conict
ser or try-hard would appreciate artist 1 (see Appendix B for the typically reported in other studies. For example, De Wit, Greer et al. (2012) showed in
their meta-analysis that across all studies, the mean reported level of relationship
conict was 2.72. Given this relatively low value, we may actually have conducted a
4
Unfortunately we did not assess participants gender in Study 2. relatively conservative test of our hypotheses.
F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189 185

Rigidity in decision making Table 3


Means and standard deviations Study 2.
To test our rst hypothesis, we estimated a multinomial logistic Measure Condition
regression predicting the answer categories with the presence (vs. Relationship No relationship
absence) of relationship conict as a predictor variable. Table 4 and conict conict
Fig. 2 show that the effect of that variable was signicant N = 47 N = 47
(v2 = 6.69, df = 2, p = .035). Supporting Hypothesis 1, the presence Motivation to process information 5.69a (1.17) 6.12a (0.89)
of relationship conict had a signicant impact on whether partic- Appraisal of the task conict as a 3.61 (1.15) 3.63 (1.22)
ipants held onto their initial opinion, instead of deriving the correct threat
Use of others unique information 0.98a (1.05) 1.57a(1.31)
solution (B = 1.19, p = .013, OR = .30). In fact, participants were
Use of own unique information 1.74 (0.82) 1.57 (0.85)
3.3 times more likely to hold onto their incorrect initial solution Relative use of others vs. own .28 a (.27) .45a (.29)
(rigidity), instead of choosing the correct solution, when relation- information
ship conict was present compared to when it was absent. Simi- a
Difference between conditions p < .05.
larly, in the presence of relationship conict, participants were
2.3 times more likely to hold onto their initial opinion instead of
adopting one of the solutions of the other group members (yield-
ing). However, this effect was not signicant (B = .84, p = .146, motivation to process information systematically was again signif-
OR = .43). icant, supporting Hypothesis 4a. In contrast to Study 1, however,
we did not nd support for Hypothesis 4b.
Information processing

To test our second hypothesis, we conducted a one-way ANOVA General discussion


on information processing with the presence (vs. absence) of rela-
tionship conict as factor. Compared to those in the relationship The results of our research showed that the presence of rela-
conict absent condition (M = .45), participants in the relationship tionship conict affects group members motivation and ability
conict present condition used relatively fewer information items to deal with diverging task-related viewpoints and causes rigidity
from others than from themselves (M = .28), F(1, 89) = 8.68, and biased information processing. All of this plays a crucial role
p = .004, g2 = .09 (see Table 3). These results supported our second in the link between task conict and decision making. Many
hypothesis.6 researchers and practitioners consider task conict a potential as-
set to improve a groups performance and creativity (e.g., Amason,
Mediation analyses 1996; Deutsch, 1973; Klein, 2008), yet the evidence for this has
been inconsistent (e.g., De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; Jehn & Bender-
The results described so far from this study showed that that sky, 2003). To shed more light on the somewhat controversial rela-
the presence (vs. absence) of relationship conict during a task tionship between task conict and group decision-making, several
conict predicted (i) whether participants derived the correct solu- reviews of the intragroup literature have suggested that it is crucial
tion instead of holding onto their initial suboptimal opinion, and to examine the factors that may moderate the link between task
(ii) the extent to which participants used the information provided conict and group decision-making (e.g., De Wit, Greer, et al.,
by other group members during their decision making. Hence, we 2012; Jehn & Bendersky, 2003). In line with this suggestion, our re-
continued with a test of our third hypothesis, which involved search investigated how relationship conict affects the link be-
mediation. Table 4, Model 2 shows that in line with the ndings tween task conict and decision making. We found that when a
of Study 1, our third hypothesis was again supported: when infor- task conict occurred in the presence (compared to the absence)
mation use was entered in the model, the effect of presence (vs. ab- of a relationship conict, group members showed more biased
sence) of relationship conict on decision making was reduced. As information-processing and were more likely to hold onto subop-
seen in the third column of Table 2, bootstrapping analyses con- timal solution alternatives. More specically, we found that the le-
rmed that the mediation effect was signicant. vel of perceived relationship conict during a task conict (Study
1), and the actual presence (vs. absence) of a relationship conict
Motivation to process information and threat appraisal (Study 2), are related to biased information processing and rigidity
One-way ANOVAs on participants motivation to process infor- in decision making.
mation, and their appraisal of the task conict as a threat, with the In both studies, we found that biased information processing
presence (vs. absence) of relationship conict as the independent mediated the effect of the co-occurrence of task and relationship
variable, revealed a signicant main effect for the motivation to conict on decision making. Because they were less likely to use
process information, F(1, 94) = 4.04, p = .047, g2 = .04, yet not for the information provided by others, those who perceived relatively
the appraisal of the task conict as a threat, F(1, 92) = .01, high levels of relationship conict (Study 1), and those who had
p = .95, g2 = .00, see also Table 3. Subsequent mediation analyses just encountered a relationship conict (Study 2) were less likely
tested whether group members motivation to process information to yield and/or derive the correct solution, instead of rigidly hold-
mediated the effect of the presence (vs. absence) of relationship ing onto an initial incorrect opinion. In Study 1, two different pro-
conict on information processing. After entering motivation as a cesses (motivation to process information systematically and the
mediator (b = .363, t(88) = 3.808, p < .001) and the appraisal of appraisal of the task conict as a threat) were found to explain
the task conict as a threat as a control variable (b = .328, the harmful effects of the co-occurrence of task and relationship
t(88) = 2.144, p = .035), the effect of relationship conict was in- conict on information processing. Study 2 replicated these effects
deed reduced (b = .216, t(88) = 2.285, p = .025). As shown in Ta- for motivation. Because our manipulation of relationship conict
ble 2, bootstrapping analyses showed that the mediating effect of did not affect group members appraisal of the task conict as a
threat, no mediation for threat appraisal was found in Study 2,
6
Identical results were obtained when we conducted these analyses with the
yet the effects of the threat appraisals on information processing
absolute (instead of the relative) extent to which participants used the unique and decision making were identical to those in Study 1. Together
information provided by other group members. these results imply that group members are less likely to shift from
186 F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

Table 4
Multinomial logistic regressions examining the decisions made in Study 2.

Predictor Model 1 Model 2


B SE OR Wald B SE OR Wald
Correct solution (Son of Mr. X) vs. incorrect rigidity solution (Mr. X)
Intercept .50 .34 2.15 1.56* .64 6.05
Presence of relationship conict 1.19* .48 .30 6.14 .63 .58 .53 1.21
Information use .05*** .01 1.05 17.97
Incorrect yielding solution (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z) vs. incorrect rigidity solution (Mr. X)
Intercept .34 .41 .66 1.92** .73 6.93
Perceived relationship conict .84 .58 .43 2.12 .47 .67 .62 .50
Information use .04** .01 1.04 9.57
Correct solution (Son of Mr. X) vs. incorrect yielding solution (Mrs. Y or Mr. Z)
Intercept .83* .38 4.84 .35 .77 .21
Presence of relationship conict .35 .59 .71 .35 .16 .63 .85 .06
Information use .01 .01 1.01 .65
Chi-square 6.69* 32.77***
df 2 4
2 log likelihood 15.70 74.06
Cox and Snell pseudo R2 .07 .31
Sample size 94 89

Note. OR = odds ratio.


*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

their initial standpoint to a more correct decision alternative or to when all information is shared (Greitemeyer & Schulz-Hardt,
adopt another ones standpoint when a task conict co-occurs with 2003. Our research extends these studies, showing that the co-
a relationship conict. These problems arise because the people in- occurrence of a task and relationship conict augments this initial
volved are less motivated to systematically process information preference effect because (for instance) it undermines group mem-
and/or appraise the task conict as a threat, and so they make less bers motivation to process information systematically and causes
use of information provided by others in their nal decision. group members to focus too much on their own information.
Finally, in the vast literature on intragroup conict, relatively
Implications little attention has been paid to stress and threat appraisals (see
Dijkstra et al., 2005 for an exception). Yet, both of our studies
Our research contributes to the intragroup conict literature in showed that threat appraisals are strongly related to information
several ways. First, it illustrates that research on the consequences processing and decision making in the context of an intragroup
of task conict should control for specic moderating characteris- conict. In line with studies on threat rigidity (e.g., Kamphuis,
tics such as the co-occurrence of a task and relationship conict. 2010; Kassam et al., 2009) our participants were more likely to
Hence, it re-emphasizes the need for conict researchers to move hold onto an initial solution, instead of deriving the correct solu-
beyond the usually proposed uniform positive or negative relation- tion when they felt relatively threatened by the conict. Moreover,
ship between task conict and performance. Second, it highlights our ndings supported recent work on threat and conrmatory
the importance of taking individual-level processes into account information search (Fischer et al., 2011), work that has shown con-
when studying the consequences of intragroup conict. Thus far, gruent threat (threat that is contextually related to the subsequent
most research on intragroup conict aggregated individual percep- decision) to produce increased levels of conrmatory information
tions of conict to the group level. Thereby it implicitly assumed search in a decision-making context.
that group members perceive the conict in a similar way, neglect-
ing potentially important differences among group members (e.g.,
Amason, 1996; Jehn, 1994 cf. Jehn & Chatman, 2000; Jehn, Rispens, Limitations and future research
& Thatcher, 2010). Our research shows that individuals not only
differ in their perception of one and the same conict. It also shows To induce a task conict in the present research, the discussion
that these different perceptions affect how group members deal among group members was experimentally controlled. This con-
with the conict and, thereby, how the conict affects the deci- trolled interaction had three important advantages. First, it en-
sions that are made. abled us to make sure that all participants were confronted with
Our research also contributes to the literature on hidden prole exactly the same task conict. In this way, we were able to keep
situations. The majority of the research on such situations has fo- meaningful factors constant across participants such as the emo-
cused on the dominance of shared information during group dis- tionality of the conict, the duration of the conict, and the
cussions and the failure of groups to exchange and discuss acquaintanceship of group members. Second, the controlled inter-
important information possessed by only one or only a few group action allowed us to make sure that all the unshared information
members. More recently, attention has been directed at the trouble necessary to derive the correct solution would be available to the
group members often seem to have with identifying the correct participants. This allowed us to exclude an alternative explanation
solution even when all information is shared and known (e.g., Gre- for the effects of relationship conict on decision making, namely
itemeyer & Schulz-Hardt, 2003). For instance, it has been shown whether the information was actually shared or not. Thirdly, be-
that when there is no process accountability, or low epistemic cause all the unshared information necessary to derive the correct
motivation, group members show poorer and less systematic infor- solution was available to the participants, we could directly assess
mation processing. Likewise, it has been shown that group mem- the extent to which individuals processed the information pro-
bers tend to hold onto their initially preferred solution even vided by other group members during decision making.
F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189 187

Yet, an important limitation of our research was that group results showed that relationship conict did not have a damaging ef-
members never interacted directly. Likewise, the conicts con- fect when task conict was absent. Moreover, in the two new condi-
sisted of only one round of discussion, which meant that group tions where there was no task conict participants were
members could not go back and forth on an issue. Hence, there signicantly more likely to hold onto their initial solution (rather
was not a full decision process involving extensive collective infor- than to yield or derive the correct solution) compared to the two
mation processing, and only a low level of differentiation (i.e., only conditions of Study 2, where task conict was present. A closer look
an exchange of information) could take place in the current set-up at the data also showed that this result was contingent on the pres-
(cf. integrative complexity theory; Suedfeld, Tetlock, & Streufert, ence of relationship conict. Participants were more likely to hold
1992). In real groups, members can interact more directly, and onto their initial solutions (instead of choosing the yielding or the
are more interdependent. The task conicts arising in such situa- correct decision) when task conict was absent (rather than pres-
tion are often more complex, with a greater variety of opinions, ent), but only when the task conict occurred in the absence of rela-
more people involved, and more lengthy discussions. Recent re- tionship conict. In sum, these results indicate that in the absence of
search, however, provides initial evidence for the generalizability task conict, people are not very likely to change an incorrect initial
of our results to settings outside the laboratory. Shaw et al. opinion. The likelihood that they will change their opinion increases
(2011), for instance, found that in real organizational teams, the when there is task conict, but only when the task conict is not
association between task conict and group member performance accompanied by relationship conict.
was more positive when levels of relationship conict were low Another limitation of our research was that during the task-re-
rather than high. Likewise, Janssen et al. (1999) found that group lated disagreement, participants initial solutions to the dilemma
members were more likely to push through their own ideas when were always incorrect. Therefore, rigidity was always dysfunc-
high levels of task conict occurred alongside high (vs. low) levels tional for decision-making quality. What we do not yet know,
of relationship conict and they and their group members did not and what future research should address, is what happens when
share a superordinate goal. Researchers should attempt to inte- participants initial solutions are actually correct. When an initial
grate these different insights, perhaps by examining real teams solution is correct, then rigidity might actually be benecial for
to see whether the damaging effect of relationship conict on the decision-making quality. Another limitation is that although we
link between task conict and group decision-making can indeed expected and found in Study 1 that perceived relationship conict
be explained by group members increased rigidity in holding onto affected individuals appraisal of the task conict as a threat, this
initially preferred task-related viewpoints. nding was not replicated in Study 2 This suggests that appraisals
Another issue worth discussing is that we did not manipulate of task conict as a threat are only augmented when task and rela-
the presence vs. absence of a task conict. Task conict was held tionship co-occur due to the misinterpretations of task conict. De-
constantly high, and we only varied the level of relationship con- spite this inconsistency, across the two studies we did nd that
ict. The advantage of manipulating task conict would have been threat appraisals had an important effect on information process-
that we could test whether relationship conict also has a damag- ing and decision making. Because threat appraisals are associated
ing effect when task conict is absent. Moreover, it would have al- with distinct patterns of cardiovascular reactivity (Blascovich &
lowed us to examine whether task conict would facilitate Mendes, 2000; Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996), future research should
superior decision making over no task conict. Although we examine more precisely when group members feel more threa-
acknowledge that this is a limitation of our methodology, there tened by task conict, and whether distinct vascular reactions
are two reasons why we eventually chose to manipulate only rela- might explain how conicts affect decision making (see also De
tionship conict, holding task conict constant. First, our main aim Wit, Scheepers, et al., 2012).
with this research was not to demonstrate a damaging effect of Future research should also examine more closely the causal
relationship conict per se, or to demonstrate that task conict pathway between information processing and decision-making.
would lead to superior decision making than no task conict. In- In line with common theorizing about group decision-making
stead, our aim was to examine whether people respond differently (see, for example, Brodbeck et al., 2007), we assumed a causal
to a task conict in the presence (vs. absence) of a relationship con- pathway from information processing to decision-making (i.e.,
ict. Second, examining a situation where task conict was absent individuals held onto their initial opinion because they processed
would also have made it more difcult to include one of our pro- information in a biased manner). On the basis of our research,
posed mediators (i.e., conict related threat appraisals), because however, the opposite causal pathway (from decision-making to
this mediator only applies to situations where there is task conict. information processing) cannot be fully excluded. Indeed, we do
To address this limitation of our design, we recently conducted not know whether the pieces of information that participants men-
a follow-up study in which we used the same design as in Study 2, tioned during decision making were the only items they processed.
except that task conict was absent (rather than present).7 The de- Participants actually may have processed all the information they
sign again included one manipulated factor with two levels (rela- received from the other group members but decided not to men-
tionship conict present vs. relationship conict absent). The tion this information because it did not support their nal decision.
To examine whether relationship conict really impairs informa-
7
tion processing during task conict, future research might examine
101 individuals (74 women and 27 men) participated in the follow-up study and
were randomly divided across a relationship conict present condition (N = 52) and a
information processing more directly, for example, by examining
relationship conict absent condition (N = 49). The texts we used to make sure task the effect of relationship conict on the ability of individuals to re-
conict was absent are reported in Footnote 2. The results showed that in the absence call information received from other group members during group
of task conict, the decisions that the participants made were unaffected by the discussion.
presence (vs. absence) of relationship conict, v2 = 1.41, df = 2, p < .494 and that in
Future research could also examine whether our ndings can be
both conditions, 67.3% of the participants held onto their initial viewpoint. This was
signicantly higher than in the two task conict present conditions of Study 2 in generalized to relationship conicts that involve other group mem-
which the percentage of participants holding onto their initial viewpoint was 42.6%, bers than the individuals themselves. On the one hand, one might
v2 = 13.42, df = 2, p < .001. Finally, a closer examination of the data showed that this argue that our ndings are restricted to relationship conicts in
result depended on the presence of relationship conict; the tendency of individuals which individuals are involved themselves. This is because a rela-
to hold onto their initial viewpoint (instead of choosing the yielding or the correct
decision) only dropped signicantly when the task conict occurred in the absence of
tionship conict may only induce a competitive mindset, and cause
relationship conict (29.8%, v2s > 13.54, ps < .001), but not when task conict a reduced motivation to systematically process information, when
occurred in the presence of relationship conict (55.3%, v2s < 1.47, ps > .21). group members are involved themselves. Yet our ndings could
188 F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189

also apply to relationship conicts that involve other group mem- car, Mrs. Y, 27 years old and having held a driving license for only
bers than individuals themselves. In such cases, relationship con- 1 year, is going shopping. Her cars lights are damaged. On the mo-
icts may cause individuals to become frustrated with a groups torcycle, Mr. Z, 28 years old, who has held a driving license for
members, which might reduce their willingness to consider what 5 years, is going to meet his sick father who asked him to come
those persons have to say. Hence, future research should examine rapidly. He is speeding on the N13 road.
whether individuals need to be involved in a relationship conict
to become rigid in holding onto their task-related viewpoints and
more biased in their information processing. A.2. Unshared Information that was provided to the participant
Another idea for future research is to more directly compare the (suggesting that the culprit is Mr. X)
impact of the two different forms of relationship conict described
in this article on group decision making (i.e., relationship conicts The guilty person is driving a car. During police inspection, the
that are triggered by a task conict, and relationship conicts that guilty car owner was discovered to have a 1.5 level of alcohol. The
concern a topic unrelated to the topic of the task conict). One pos- guilty person admits that he was inattentive at the time of the
sibility is to run a study in which rst a task conict is induced, and collision.
subsequently a relationship conict is induced that either directly
follows from the task conict, or involves a topic unrelated to the
task at hand. In both these conditions individuals are likely to show
A.3. Unshared information that is provided to the participant by bogus
more rigidity in holding onto initial task-related viewpoints, com-
group members
pared to a situation free of relationship conicts. Yet, such a study
could examine whether relationship conicts that arise from a task
Information provided by bogus group member 1. (suggesting that
conict have a stronger negative impact on decision making and
the guilty person is Mrs. Y). The guilty person is less than 30 years
information processing than relationship conict that arise inde-
old. Due to inexperience, the guilty person wasnt able to avoid the
pendently from a task conict. Moreover, such a study could be
collision. The guilty person claims that he or she did not see others
used to examine possible interventions to curb the negative effects
approaching the intersection.
of both forms of relationship conict.
Information provided by bogus group member 2. (suggesting that
Bradley, Postlethwaite, Klotz, Hamdani, and Brown (2012), for
the guilty person is Mr. Z). The guilty person is a man. His father is
example, found that task conicts have a more positive impact
indirectly responsible for the accident. The guilty person was driv-
on group performance when group members experience high lev-
ing at 130 km/h.
els of psychological safety (i.e., the shared belief held by members
of a team that the team is safe for interpersonal risk taking;
Edmondson, 1999). Presumably, this is because team members
working in a psychologically safe environment feel a sense of Appendix B. Relationship conict manipulation responses
openness and avoid taking task disagreements personally (Brad-
ley et al., 2012, p. 152). To prevent group members from misinter- B.1. Non-relationship conict condition8
preting task conict as relationship conict, interventions should
thus be aimed at increasing feelings of psychological safety. For Group member 1: I absolutely adored the work of artist 1. The
example, teams could support training aimed at coaching mem- textures and tones were so effortlessly advanced and elegant. Im
bers to approach and manage different viewpoints in an open glad to nd that someone else sees what I see. I feel like we share
and considerate manner. Moreover, leaders could be trained to fos- a relevant and meaningful connection. Art is the great leveler isnt
ter an environment that is safe for interpersonal risk taking (e.g., by it? Glad to know there are others intellectual enough to appreciate
stimulating leader inclusiveness; see Nembhard & Edmondson, true art!
2006). These interventions might not only make group members Group member 2: Some of that work was pretty nice but Id
feel more at ease when they want to express a diverging view- have to say its pretty close between 1 and the third artist. I think
points, but also help them to approach others viewpoints with a 1 is the best though because it really just makes me think. I believe
less competitive mindset. That would, in turn, makes them less that artist 1 appeals because their work requires a higher level of
likely to become defensive when someone disagrees with them. artistic literacy. It feels so great that were all in agreeance!

Acknowledgments
B.2. Relationship conict condition8
We would like to thank Claudia Zoontjes and Brian Van for their
Group member 1: The second artist was my favorite because
assistance with data collection. We thank Lindred Greer and Maar-
their work is way better than those other guys, including that of
ten Zaal for their valuable comments and suggestions on earlier
the rst artist. Technically, aesthetically, everything. .. I know this
drafts of this article.
is subjective but seriously, I could probably produce stuff like those
other artists. Its ridiculous how much worse those others were, I
Appendix A. Hidden prole task (adapted from Toma and think our tastes clash and thats not really good? Maybe reconsider
Butera (2009)) your interpretation?
Group member 2: Im not sure about that rst artist either.
A.1. Shared information Their work just seemed simplistic and way too entry-level. They
were obviously trying to appeal to anyone and everyone, pretty
The collision takes place at the St. Georges intersection, on much any poser or try-hard would appreciate artist 1, but to
Monday at 7 p.m. The road is narrow and poorly lit. Two cars be able to see and interpret the understatement and effortless
and one motorcycle are involved. In the rst car, Mr. Xwho is beauty of the third thats something I can say I do.
53 years old and has held a driving license for 30 yearsand his
17-year-old son return home. The father had just drunk several 8
Note that for all participants, their preferred artist was always labeled and
glasses of spirits during a dinner with his friends. In the second referred to as Artist 1, or the rst artist.
F.R.C. de Wit et al. / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 122 (2013) 177189 189

References Jehn, K. A., & Bendersky, C. (2003). Intragroup conict in organizations: A


contingency perspective on the conictoutcome relationship. In R. M.
Kramer & B. M Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 25,
Amason, A. C. (1996). Distinguishing the effects of functional and dysfunctional
pp. 187242). New York: Elsevier JAI.
conict on strategic decision-making: Resolving a paradox for top management
Jehn, K. A., & Chatman, J. A. (2000). The inuence of proportional and perceptual
teams. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 123148.
conict composition on team performance. The International Journal of Conict
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderatormediator variable distinction in
Management, 11, 5673.
social psychological research. Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
Jehn, K. A., Greer, L. L., Levine, S., & Szulanski, G. (2008). The effects of conict types,
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.
dimensions, and emergent states on group outcomes. Group Decision and
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to
Negotiation, 17, 465495.
violence and aggression: The dark side of self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103,
Jehn, K. A., Rispens, S., & Thatcher, S. (2010). The effects of conict asymmetry on
533.
workgroup and individual outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 53,
Behfar, K., & Thompson, L. (2007). Conict within and between organizational
596616.
groups: Functional dysfunctional and quasi-functional perspectives. In L.
Kamphuis, W. (2010). Teams under threat: Uncovering and overcoming rigidity effects.
Thompson & K. Behfar (Eds.), Conict in teams (pp. 335). Evanston, IL:
Unpublished dissertation, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands.
Northwestern University Press.
Kassam, K. S., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (2009). Decisions under distress: Stress
Blascovich, J., & Mendes, W. B. (2000). Challenge and threat appraisals: The role of
proles inuence anchoring and adjustment. Psychological Science, 20,
affective cues. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social
13941399.
cognition (pp. 5982). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Klein, J. (2008). Obamas team of rivals. Time Magazine. <http://www.time.com/
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation.
time/politics/article/0,8599,1815849,00.html>.
In M. P. Zanna (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29,
Lau, R. S., & Cobb, A. T. (2010). Understanding the connections between relationship
pp. 151). New York: Academic Press.
conict and performance: The intervening roles of trust and exchange. Journal of
Bradley, B. H., Postlethwaite, B. E., Klotz, A. C., Hamdani, M. R., & Brown, K. G. (2012).
Organizational Behavior, 31, 898917.
Reaping the benets of task conict in teams: The critical role of team
Lcken, M., & Simon, B. (2005). Cognitive and affective experiences of minority and
psychological safety climate. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 151158.
majority members: The role of group size, status, and power. Journal of
Brodbeck, F. C., Kerschreiter, R., Mojzisch, A., & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2007). Group
Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 396413.
decision making under conditions of distributed knowledge: The information
Narayanan, L., Menon, S., & Spector, P. E. (1999). Stress in the workplace: A
asymmetries model. Academy of Management Review, 32, 459479.
comparison of gender and occupations. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20,
De Dreu, C. K. W. (2008). The virtue and vice of workplace conict: Food for
6373.
(pessimistic) thought. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29, 518.
Nemeth, C. (1995). Dissent as driving cognition, attitudes and judgments. Social
De Dreu, C. K. W., Koole, S., & Oldersma, F. L. (1999). On the seizing and freezing of
Cognition, 13, 273291.
negotiator inferences: Need for cognitive closure moderates the use of
Nembhard, I. M., & Edmondson, A. C. (2006). Making it safe: The effects of leader
heuristics in negotiation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 348362.
inclusiveness and professional status on psychological safety and improvement
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A., & van Knippenberg, D. (2008). Motivated
efforts in health care teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 941966.
information processing in group judgment and decision making. Personality and
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
Social Psychology Review, 12, 2249.
effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Van Knippenberg, D. (2005). The possessive self as a barrier to
Computers, 36, 717731.
conict resolution: Effects of mere ownership, process accountability, and self-
Rubin, J. Z., Pruitt, D. G., & Kim, S. H. (1994). Social conict: Escalation, stalemate, and
concept clarity on competitive cognitions and behavior. Journal of Personality
settlement. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
and Social Psychology, 89, 345357.
Scholten, L., Van Knippenberg, D., Nijstad, B. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2007).
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Weingart, L. R. (2003). Task versus relationship conict, team Motivated information processing and group decision-making: Effects of
performance, and team member satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied process accountability and information dissemination. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 88, 741749. Social Psychology, 43, 539552.
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Weingart, L. R. (2003). A contingency theory of task conict and Schulz-Hardt, S., Brodbeck, F. C., Mojzisch, A., Kerschreiter, R., & Frey, D. (2006).
performance in groups and organizational teams. In M. West, D. Tjosvold, & K. Group decision-making in hidden prole situations: Dissent as a facilitator for
G. Smith (Eds.), International handbook of organizational teamwork and decision quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 10801093.
cooperative working (pp. 151166). Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley. Shaw, J. D., Zhu, J., Duffy, M. K., Scott, K. L., Shih, H. A., & Susanto, E. (2011). A
De Dreu, C. K. W., Weingart, L. R., & Kwon, S. (2000). Inuence of social motives on contingency model of conict and team effectiveness. Journal of Applied
integrative negotiation: A meta-analytic review and test of two theories. Journal Psychology, 96, 391400.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 889905. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-experimental
Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conict: Constructive and destructive processes. studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7,
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. 422445.
De Wit, F. R. C., Greer, L. L., & Jehn, K. A. (2012). The paradox of intragroup conict: A Simons, T. L., & Peterson, R. S. (2000). Task conict and relationship conict in top
meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 360390. management teams: The pivotal role of intragroup trust. Journal of Applied
De Wit, F. R. C., Scheepers, D. T., & Jehn, K. A. (2012). Cardiovascular reactivity and Psychology, 85, 102111.
resistance to opposing viewpoints during intragroup conict. Psychophysiology, Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision-
49, 16911699. making: Biased information sampling during discussion. Journal of Personality
Dijkstra, M. T. M., Van Dierendonck, D., & Evers, A. (2005). Responding to conict at and Social Psychology, 48, 14671478.
work and individual well-being: The mediating role of ight behavior and Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E., & Dutton, J. E. (1981). Threat-rigidity effects in
feelings of helplessness. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, organizational behavior: A multilevel analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly,
14, 119135. 26, 501524.
Edmondson, A. C. (1999). Psychological safety and learning behavior in work teams. Suedfeld, P., Tetlock, P. E., & Streufert, S. (1992). Conceptual/integrative complexity.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 350383. In C. P. Smith, J. W. Atkinson, D. C. McClelland, & J. Veroff (Eds.), Motivation and
Fischer, P., Kastenmller, A., Greitemeyer, T., Fischer, J., Frey, D., & Crelley, D. (2011). personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 393400). New York:
Threat and selective exposure: The moderating role of threat and decision Cambridge University Press.
context on conrmatory information search after decisions. Journal of Swann, W. B., Jr., Polzer, J. T., Seyle, D. C., & Ko, S. J. (2004). Finding value in diversity:
Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 5162. Verication of personal and social self-views in diverse groups. Academy of
Fisher, R., & Ury, W. (1981). Getting to yes: Negotiating agreement without giving in. Management Review, 29, 927.
Boston: Houghton Mifin. Tjosvold, D. (1998). Cooperative and competitive goal approach to conict:
Gigone, D., & Hastie, R. (1993). The common knowledge effect: Information sharing Accomplishments and challenges. Applied Psychology: An International Review,
and group judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 959974. 46, 285342.
Greitemeyer, T., & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2003). Preference-consistent evaluation of Toma, C., & Butera, F. (2009). Hidden proles and concealed information: Strategic
information in the hidden prole paradigm: Beyond group-level explanations information sharing and use in group decision-making. Personality and Social
for the dominance of shared information in group decisions. Journal of Psychology Bulletin, 35, 793806.
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 322339. Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., & Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective,
Janssen, O., Van De Vliert, E., & Veenstra, C. (1999). How task and person conict physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal
shape the role of positive interdependence in teams. Journal of Management, 25, of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 616624.
117141. Torrance, E. P. (1957). Group decision making and disagreement. Social Forces, 35,
Jehn, K. A. (1994). Enhancing effectiveness: An investigation of advantages and 314318.
disadvantages of value-based intragroup conict. International Journal of Conict Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2002). Social value orientation and impression
Management, 5, 223238. formation: A test of two competing hypotheses about information search in
Jehn, K. A. (1995). A multimethod examination of the benets and detriments of negotiation. International Journal of Conict Management, 13, 5977.
intragroup conict. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 262282. Yang, J., & Mossholder, K. W. (2004). Decoupling task and relationship conict: The
Jehn, K. A. (1997). Qualitative analysis of conict types and dimensions in role of intragroup emotional processing. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25,
organizational groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 530557. 589605.

También podría gustarte