Está en la página 1de 39

University of Connecticut

DigitalCommons@UConn
Honors Scholar Theses Honors Scholar Program

Spring 5-10-2009

Labeling in the Classroom: Teacher Expectations


and their Effects on Students' Academic Potential
Jacqueline Ercole
University of Connecticut - Storrs, jacher11@aol.com

Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.uconn.edu/srhonors_theses


Part of the Psychology Commons

Recommended Citation
Ercole, Jacqueline, "Labeling in the Classroom: Teacher Expectations and their Effects on Students' Academic Potential" (2009).
Honors Scholar Theses. 98.
http://digitalcommons.uconn.edu/srhonors_theses/98
Labeling in the Classroom, 1

Running Head: TEACHER LABELING AND STUDENTS ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE

Labeling in the Classroom:

Teacher Expectations and their Effects on Students Academic Performance

Jackie Ercole

University of Connecticut
Labeling in the Classroom, 2

Abstract

The transition to high school can be challenging for some adolescents, resulting in drops

of academic functioning (Barber & Olsen, 2004; Smith, 2006). While changes in academic

demands and the disparity between adolescent needs and the environmental characteristics of

high school have both been cited as possible contributors to this decrease in academic and

personal functioning (Barber & Olsen, 2004), it is possible that teachers may play an even larger

role in undermining these students functioning, specifically through labeling. Although labeling,

and how it can lead to self-fulfilling prophesies, is a concept that has been thoroughly researched

and applied to the field of criminology and deviant behavior, it is the goal of this current study to

investigate if labeling also occurs in the classroom setting and how such labels ultimately effect

the academic potential of high school students.


Labeling in the Classroom, 3

Labeling in the Classroom:

Teacher Expectations and their Effects on Students Academic Performance

The Transition to High School

Initially, many scholars hypothesized that the transition to high school was difficult for

all students. Yet recent research has suggested that these difficulties are likely characteristic of

only some students (Wargo-Aikins, Bierman, & Parker, 2005). For those students who have had

a difficult time transitioning, common patterns have begun to emerge to explain why some

students are more dissatisfied than others and demonstrate more negative outcomes.

Current research has indicated that the decrease in teacher support that is typically found

once students enter high school can make the transition more difficult (Barber & Olsen, 2004). In

their research, Barber and Olsen (2004) state that as children progress to higher levels of

education, their autonomy should increase, and thus they should have a less personal relationship

with their teachers. Yet Barber and Olsen (2004) indicate that this decrease in dependency on

teachers is not beneficial for students, because it is developmentally regressive, contradicting the

adolescent need for positive interpersonal connections, not only making the transition harder for

students, but making students like school less. The importance of positive interpersonal

connections between teachers and students, specifically in regards to a students academic

potential, is also reflected in the Cornell process model of motivation, which states that students

perceived social context in the school setting (i.e. teachers) directly influences the self system

(i.e. engagement in school) consequently impacting academic outcomes (Caraway, Tucker,

Reinke & Hall, 2003).


Labeling in the Classroom, 4

This division in interpersonal connections at the high school level is ultimately attributed

to the change in classroom size. Once students reach the high school level, the student-teacher

ratio inevitably grows, making it difficult for students to collaborate with their teachers in

regards to the material they will be taught (Davis, 2003). In turn, this inability to collaborate with

their teachers in high school is very problematic for students, because in order to be motivated to

learn, students need teachers who put their students needs and interests first, which, given the

increased size of the high school classroom, is a difficult task to accomplish.

This change in classroom environment that is seen at the high school level also tends to

place a greater emphasis on student evaluation and on the recognition of a students academic

potential, ultimately creating competition in the classroom between students. Such an emphasis

is clearly evident once students are segregated into specific classes as a result of their academic

achievement level (i.e. special-ed, honors, AP, etc.) These classes, although created to enhance a

students academic potential, could actually be denying students the full benefit of school

membership, especially for those students in the lower levels. These students begin to lose

confidence in their ability to perform well academically, ultimately causing them to like school

less (Ames, 1992).

While lack of interpersonal relations with teachers, loss of autonomy in the classroom,

and the increased competitiveness that is placed on students academic performance at the high

school level certainly leads to dissatisfaction for some students once in high school, effecting

their ability to transition, a topic that has not been as thoroughly researched, or applied to the

transition difficulties some students face at the high school level, are teachers expectations of

students.
Labeling in the Classroom, 5

Teacher Expectations

According to Schultz (1983), the expectations teachers have of their students inevitably

effects the way that teachers interact with them, which ultimately leads to changes in the

students behavior and attitude. In a classic study performed by Robert Rosenthal, elementary

school teachers were given IQ scores for all of their students, scores that, unbeknownst to the

teachers, did not reflect IQ and, in fact, measured nothing. Yet just as researchers predicted,

teachers formed a positive expectation for those students who scored high on the exam vs. those

who scored low (Harris, 1991). In response to these expectations, the teachers inevitably altered

their environment in four ways (Harris, 1991): First, the teaching climate was drastically

different depending on if a smart child asked questions, or offered answers, vs. if a dumb

child performed the same behaviors. The former was met with warm and supportive feedback

while the latter was not. Second, the amount of input a teacher gave to a smart student was

much higher, and entailed more material being taught, vs. if the student was dumb. Third, the

opportunity to respond to a question was only lengthened for students identified as smart. Lastly,

teachers made much more of an effort to provide positive and encouraging feedback to the

smart children while little attention/feedback was given to the dumb students, even if they

provided the correct answer.

This discrepancy between how the high IQ vs. low IQ children were treated by their

teacher illustrates how problematic extrinsic rewards can be in the classroom setting, not only

because not all students are rewarded for their behavior, but because these rewards, or lack there

of, soon become internalized by these students as a method of maintaining behavior and

ultimately determining their self worth (Ames, 1992). Thus because students self perceptions,

such as self efficacy, goal orientation, and/or autonomy, are robust indicators of motivation and
Labeling in the Classroom, 6

performance in school (Furrer & Skinner, 2003) the students who are continually rewarded by

their teacher begin to see themselves as good students, and become motivated to learn and do

well, while those who do not receive positive feedback inevitably abandon their motivation to do

well, causing their academic potential to suffer, becoming the very individuals their teachers

expected them to be.

Yet if students are continually progressing to higher grades and different schools, these

initial teacher evaluations should have little impact on these students once they have new

teachers. Unfortunately, because many teachers rely on previous evaluations from other teachers

as a way to evaluate incoming students (Harris, 1991), these labels inevitably follow the student

throughout their academic career, making it difficult, if not impossible, for those labeled as poor

students to ever reach their true, academic potential.

The idea that these teacher expectations, these labels teachers inevitably assign their

students, have long term effects on students adjustment in school is an area that has not been

thoroughly researched in the adolescent literature. Yet the long term implications of labels on

deviant behavior has been thoroughly addressed in the criminological setting, and thus this study

will examine how Labeling theory can inevitably be applied in the classroom to explain deviant

academic vs. deviant criminal behavior.

Labeling Theory Applied to the Classroom

Although the concept of Labeling theory, specifically the idea that criminal labels carry a

stigmatizing effect on those individuals who have been identified as criminals, originated as

early as 1911 by criminologist scholars, it wasnt until 1951 that this theory was formalized by

Edwin Lemert (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007). Lemert stated that there were two types of deviance:

Primary deviance, which is when an individual gets caught committing a criminal act, and
Labeling in the Classroom, 7

secondary deviance: the reaction society has to the individual now identified as being a criminal

(Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007). It is this latter form of deviance that enabled Labeling theory to gain

such immense popularity in the 1960s, forcing criminologists to reconsider how large a part

society plays in not only formulizing the laws of society, but in determining the future of its

criminal population.

Thus while primary and secondary deviance has traditionally been used to explain

criminal behavior, in this study, it can easily be applied to the school setting, particularly in

regards to explaining how students identify themselves as bad vs. good. This comparison is

particularly evident in Rosenthals experiment, where primary deviance would be the individuals

who scored poorly on the exam, and secondary deviance would be the way the teacher now

treated these children as a result of their low scores. Thus just like an individual who is labeled a

criminal is forever seen as an outcast in the eyes of others, reducing them from a whole person to

that of a tainted, discounted one (Link & Phelan, 2001), so too is the poor student.

As Labeling theory clearly illustrates, both the social and academic deviant, once labeled

as such, are forced to carry these labels with them, performing the very behavior society expects

of them, inevitably leading to confirmation bias. William Chambliss identified this bias when he

examined societys reaction to two groups of high school boys: the Saints and the Roughnecks

(Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007). Although both sets of boys had an equal amount of delinquency

between them, because of their less desirable status (i.e. lower social class) the Roughnecks were

assumed by police, and regular citizens alike, as being more likely to participate in deviant

behavior (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007). Thus as this example illustrates, secondary deviance leads

to the formulation of stereotypes, which is troubling, considering how persistent stereotypes are.
Labeling in the Classroom, 8

Understanding how persistent stereotypes are in the criminological literature inevitably

helps explain why prior teacher evaluations can be so problematic, particularly for students who

have been evaluated negatively. Because these teachers only know these students as poor

students, they consequently do not put in the effort to challenge these students academically, and

thus because their poor reputation is constantly following them, these students never get the

opportunity to prove that they can be good students and, instead, perform the only way they

know how to: poorly. This phenomenon has been defined as a self-fulfilling prophesy, and just

like criminal labels have the unanticipated consequence of pushing labeled individuals into

future criminal careers (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007), negative academic labels may be pushing

students into greater academic failure. Thus it is the goal of this study to illustrate that negative

academic labels, like negative social labels, are inevitably carried by students throughout their

academic career, specifically from middle school into high school, ultimately affecting their

ability to transition.

Alienation, as a result of being negatively labeled, is another consequence that has been

thoroughly researched in the criminological literature. As a result of individuals being labeled as

criminal, there is a fear of getting too close to these individuals, too close to these stigmatized

persons, much like the discomfort we feel when having to sit next to a physically disabled

individual (Hilton & Von Hippel, 1996). In fact, many will try to avoid this predicament at all

cost, resulting in the isolation, as well as alienation, of the stigmatized person. This feeling of not

belonging, as a result of a negative label, is not just limited to criminals, but has also been

documented in adolescent research, particularly with students who do not perform well

academically.
Labeling in the Classroom, 9

Because many teachers do not interact with their poor students, these students ultimately

lack support from their teachers, support which, during a stressful transition (i.e. middle school

to high school), is imperative for a student to have in order to increase their overall adjustment

and functioning in school (Rosenfeld, Richman, & Bowen, 2000; Barber & Olsen, 2004). Thus

as a result of this alienation, these students are at an increased risk of engaging in health

compromising behavior, such as smoking and drinking, as a means to cope with the stress of not

feeling accepted (Samdal, Wold, Klepp, & Kansas, 2000). Research has also shown that

difficulty in school may be one of the best predictors of delinquency in American society (Lotz

& Lee, 1999) Thus it is the goal of this current study to illustrate that individuals who have been

labeled as bad students are more likely to feel disengaged from the schooling process, and thus

as a result of this alienation, have high engagement in unhealthy behaviors.

While labels can be applied to anyone, regardless of race, gender, and education level, the

research conducted in the criminological setting clearly illustrates that those on the bottom of the

socio-cultural hierarchy (i.e. minorities, the poor, etc.) are those that are the most prone to

carrying these stigmatizing labels. Racial profiling is a perfect example of this: In police records,

there are more incidences of police citing, searching, arresting, and using force against minority

drivers vs. white drivers (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007). Although criminologists opposed to

Labeling theory have argued that cops are more likely to arrest someone due to the seriousness of

the offense vs. their racial makeup, proponents of Labeling theory have shown that even when

seriousness of offense is controlled, the relationship between race/social class remains for police

intake of individuals (Wellford, 1975). Thus official crime statistics may well be inaccurate in

their portrayal of who commits the most crimes due to this systematic bias that is enforced by

police officers with minority groups (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007). This evidence is troubling
Labeling in the Classroom, 10

considering the implications it unduly has for minority groups: By utilizing discriminatory

practices (i.e. racial profiling) those with higher social status and power are able to persuade

minorities that they are inferior, ultimately making them less likely to challenge these

discriminatory practices, keeping them in existence (Link & Phelan, 2001).

This discriminatory practice is unfortunately also evident in the academic setting,

specifically for students enrolled in urban and rural schools. Because these schools tend to be in

sections with little income, they often do not have the resources to offer an extensive number of

AP classes like suburban schools do, putting minority students at an even greater disadvantage

academically by placing them in the lower level courses (Solorzano & Ornelas, 2002). As a

result of this, minority students are more likely to feel disengaged from the schooling process,

because they have no means, or support, to challenge themselves academically (Barber & Olsen,

2004) inevitably causing these students to either cool out, a process that involves just going

through the motions of school, vs. trying to succeed academically (Frtizberg, 2001), or, in the

most severe cases, drop out (Caraway et al., 2003; Little & Garber, 2004). In fact, the drop out

rate between white and minority students is so disproportionate, with many more minority

students dropping out vs. white students, that it has led some researchers to conclude that our

society is guilty of institutional racism, where only the culture of the white race is taught and

tested in school, not the culture of minority students (Fritzberg, 2001).

Thus one of the goals of this study is to investigate if students in lower SES schools are at

a greater disadvantage than students in higher SES schools, specifically because both their

minority standing, and unequal access to educational resources, puts them at a greater risk of

being labeled as poor students, leading them to become disengaged, and thus not perform well

academically.
Labeling in the Classroom, 11

An area that has received little attention, in both the adolescent and criminological

setting, is that of positive labels, thus it cannot be assumed that these labels, while positive, are

always advantageous. As mentioned before, in high school, the classes are divided in a

hierarchical fashion, with AP and honor classes being on top. In order to get a better

understanding of how students in these higher level classes view themselves, a recent study was

conducted at Northwestern State University investigating 14 high achieving adolescents from

various high schools around the country (Moulton, Moulton, Housewright & Bailey, 1998).

According to this study, while many students reported positive perceptions towards their label as

gifted students (i.e. special experiences, internal gratification, unique, etc.) they also listed

negative aspects associated with this same label, specifically the increased pressure and high

expectations from parents and teachers. In relation to this, the authors also found that many

counselors, teachers, and administrators virtually ignored the developmental needs of the gifted

and only focused on their cognitive abilities (Moulton et al., 1998).

It was also found that even high achieving students are not immune from feelings of

academic inadequacy. According to Little and Garber (2004), people who exhibit high levels of

investment in certain areas (i.e. school) are particularly susceptible to becoming depressed

following stressful events related to their respective sensitivities (i.e. bad test grade). This finding

is especially compelling when taking into account that the transition from middle school to high

school results in a decreased GPA for most students (Barber & Olson, 2004; Smith, 2006). This

decline in academic achievement is especially troublesome for adolescent girls, specifically in

regards to their self-motivation and confidence in their academic abilities (Little & Garber,

2004). This achievement loss, as well as the loss of both self-esteem and self perception,
Labeling in the Classroom, 12

exhibited by the female students in this study, has been shown to be highly correlated with

increased drop out rates in high school (Alspaugh, 1998).

Thus even high achieving students can feel disengaged from the learning process,

feelings that could potentially have long lasting effects on future academic endeavors,

discouraging these students from reaching their potential. With this in mind, another goal in this

study is to investigate if these high performing students, because of their positive labels, still

manage to do well in high school, even if they had a difficult time transitioning.

Purpose of this Study

The goals of this study were to measure how the initial stress of transitioning from

middle school to high school interacts with teacher expectations, and whether labels carry over

from one school to the other, further affecting the childs academic ability, even when immersed

in a new environment. Risky behavior was also used as a means to gauge students successful or

unsuccessful integration into high school. Academic potential was also measured by comparing

schools from different environments (i.e. rural vs. suburban) in order to determine if school

location really does impact the rate of success, or failure, for students. By comparing these

different schools, it was also possible to conclude whether or not SES is predictive of academic

success and/or drop out rates.

The hypotheses of the current study were as follows: Hypothesis 1: Negative labels

would continue to impact the academic achievements of students from middle school to high

school. Hypothesis 2: Positive labels would impact high achieving students from middle school

to high school even if these students had a difficult transition into high school. Hypothesis

3:Bad students will be more likely to involve themselves in risky behaviors (i.e. drug use,

violence, sex, etc.). Hypothesis 4: Students from low SES schools will be more likely to be
Labeling in the Classroom, 13

labeled negatively as students as a result of both their minority status and decreased educational

opportunities, resulting in lower academic achievement then students in higher SES schools.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 279 adolescents (164 females) who were in the eighth grade and

ranged from age 13 to 15 years (M = 13.95; SD = .36) at the beginning of the study. The ethnic

composition of the sample included 79% European American, 1% African American, 1% Asian

American, 15% Latino American, and 4% from mixed ethnic background. Socioeconomic status

data was not available for individual children and therefore per capita income for each town and

percentage of students receiving free or reduced lunch were used as a proxy for SES. As such,

SES was considered as a group variable rather than an individual variable. According to the 2000

Census data, the 5 towns where the students were enrolled in public school ranged in per capita

from $35,087 to $77,794 (M = $58, 465, SD = $16, 036). According to school records, the

number of children who were eligible for free/reduced lunch ranged from 2% to 57%.

At Time 1, in the spring of 8th grade, all students were recruited for participation. Consent

forms were returned by 62% of families (n = 388) and of these, 72% of parents consented their

childs participation (n = 281, 53% of total population). Students who were absent on one of the

days of testing (n = 1) or refused to participate (n = 1) were excluded from the analyses, yielding

a final sample of 279 participants at Time 1. A total of 248 (89%) of these participants were

available for testing 6 months later in the Fall of 9th grade (Time 2). Attrition was due to

participants moving away from the area (n = 5), attending high schools that were not part of the

study (n =19), retention in 8th grade (n = 1), incomplete data (n = 2), and refusal to participate

(n= 3). Six months later, during the Spring of 9th grade, data was collected again (Time 3). A
Labeling in the Classroom, 14

total of 241 (86%) participants were available for testing. Attrition between Time 2 and Time 3

was due to participants movie away from the area (n = 3), school absence (n = 1), and placement

out of district (n = 3).

Measures

GPA. GPAs were calculated by averaging students grades, which were obtained from

their report cards, across their classes during Time 1, 2, and 3. Scores were based on a

percentage from 0-100%.

Absences. Absences were calculated as the number of school reported days a student had

missed class during the year.

Referrals. School referrals were calculated as the number of school reported times

students were sent to the office for disciplinary reasons during the year.

YRBS (Youth Risk Behavior Survey). For the purpose of this study, a shortened version of

the YRBS was used as a measure of risky behavior. Specifically, lifetime engagement in

smoking cigarettes, alcohol consumption, and marijuana use, were examined. For each behavior,

participants were asked to specify if they have ever participated in the risky behavior, how old

they were when they first engaged in that behavior, and the frequency in which they participated

in the risky behavior within the past month (i.e. 30 days). The YRBS has been found to be a

reliable measure, with 71.7% of the items rated as having substantial or higher reliability (kappa

= 61%-100%) (Stevens & Griffin, 2001). Data from a test-retest reliability study indicated that

students report risk behaviors reliably over time on this measure (Brenner, Collins, Kann,

Warren, & Williams, 1995; Stevens & Griffin, 2001).

Academic engagement. As a measure to identify teachers labeling of students, teachers

ratings of participants academic engagement was assessed using the Academic Helplessness
Labeling in the Classroom, 15

Scale (Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, & Seligman, 1992, Rudolph, Kurlakowsky, Conley, 2001) at

Time 1 and Time 3. For each of the 12 items, teachers rated (1 = not true to 5 = very true)

students tendency toward helpless behavior in the context of schoolwork (i.e. Takes little

independent initiative; you must help him/her get started and keep going on an assignment.

When s/he begins a difficult problem, his/her attempts are half hearted.). Scores were

calculated as the mean of the 12 items. Higher scores represent more helpless academic behavior.

In this study, these ratings of helpless behavior are believed to be proxies of teacher perceptions

of poor students. Teachers also rated students academic effort (i.e. Works hard academically.)

versus their academic performance (i.e. Performs well academically.) at Time 1 and Time 3,

with teacher ratings of poor effort and performance, again, being indicators of negative teacher

perceptions.

Teacher ratings of students behavioral and emotional engagement in class were also

believed to be proxies of teachers perceptions of poor students. Behavioral and emotional

engagement were assessed using the 24 item Academic Engagement questionnaire. Each item

was rated on a four-point Likert scale ranging from Not at all true (1) to Very true (4).

Sample items used to assess behavioral engagement included In class, the student works as hard

as he/she can and When Im in class, the student just acts like they are working. Sample items

used to assess emotional engagement included When we work on something in class, this

student gets bored and Class is not all that fun for the student The mean of all responses was

then calculated, with low scores indicating decreased behavioral and emotional engagement.
Labeling in the Classroom, 16

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 displays the means and standard deviations of the labeling and outcome measures

at both Time 1 and Time 3. Correlations between the labeling and outcome variables at Time 1

and Time 3 are presented in Tables 2 and 3.

There was notable stability across outcomes from Time 1 and Time 3. Except for

smoking, all other substance use at Time 1 was significantly correlated with substance use at

Time 3 at the p <.01 level, indicating that if students were engaged in unhealthy behavior in

eighth grade, they were likely to continue such behavior in ninth grade. There was also a

significant correlation between grades at Time 1 and Time 3, indicating that academic

performance in eighth grade was predictive of academic performance in ninth grade. Looking at

Table 2, it is evident that teacher ratings of helplessness, behavioral engagement, and emotional

engagement during Time 3 were significantly predictive of students engagement in drug,

alcohol, and cigarette use, as well as lower academic performance.

Significant stability was also found from Time 1 to Time 3 for all three labeling

variables. Ratings of helplessness (.47), behavioral engagement (.30), and emotional engagement

(.67) were all found to be positively correlated from Time 1 to Time 3 indicating that students

labeled as more helpless, less behaviorally engaged, and less emotionally engaged at Time 1

were also likely to be labeled as more helpless, less behaviorally engaged, and less emotionally

engaged at Time 3. At Time 1, ratings of helplessness and behavioral engagement were found to

be significantly correlated to each other (-.44) indicating that the more helpless a teacher saw a

student, the more likely they were to also see that student as being less behaviorally engaged.

This relationship was again observed at Time 3 (-.45). Although behavioral engagement and
Labeling in the Classroom, 17

emotional engagement were not significantly correlated with each other at Time 1, at Time 3, a

significant relationship emerged between the two labeling variables (.44) indicating that teachers

who rated their students low on behavioral engagement were also likely to rate them low on

emotional engagement.

SES and Academic Performance

In order to determine if those students labeled negatively by their teachers in lower

SES schools were at an even greater disadvantage academically than the negatively rated

students at higher SES schools, a T-test was performed. Compared to negatively rated students at

higher SES schools, negatively rated students at lower SES schools were seen as more helpless

t(341) = -3.39, p < .001, M = 2.28, SD = 1.00 vs. M = 1.8, SD = .89, less behaviorally engaged

t(332) = 4.64, p< .001, M = 2.61, SD = .33 vs. M = 2.8, SD = .26, and less emotionally engaged

t(332) = .96, p = .34, M = 2.4, SD = .05 vs. M = 2.45, SD = .27, supporting the initial hypothesis.

Hierarchical Regressions

Hierarchical regression analyses were performed to examine the impact of teacher

labeling on change in adolescent academic performance and engagement in health risk behavior.

For each analysis, pre-transition levels of the outcome of interest were entered into Step 1 and

teachers post-transition labels (i.e. post-transition ratings of behavioral and emotional

engagement, as well as teachers ratings of helplessness) were entered into Step 2. Results of

these analyses can be found in Tables 4 and 5.

The first analysis examined the influence of teachers labeling on change in academic

performance. In Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that pre-transition teacher ratings of

performance were significantly predictive of post-transition ratings of academic performance

(R = .21, p<.001, = .46). In Step 2, teacher labeling added significantly to the prediction of
Labeling in the Classroom, 18

academic performance, with teachers ratings of behavioral engagement at Time 3 (R = .40,

p<.001, = .28) and teachers ratings of helplessness at Time 3 (R = .40, p<.001, = -.52)

significantly adding to the prediction.

The influence of teachers labeling on students academic behavior, specifically their

grades, absences, and detentions, were also measured using hierarchical regression analyses. The

results of these can be found in Table 5.

The effects of teachers labels on students grades was the first examined. In Step 1, the

analysis demonstrated that students grades at Time 1 were significantly predictive of students

grades at Time 3 (R = .60, p <.001, = .78). In Step 2, teacher labeling did add significantly to

the prediction of students grades at Time 3, specifically teachers ratings of students behavioral

engagement at Time 3 (R = .06, p =.03, = .12) and teachers ratings of students helplessness

at Time 3 (R = .06, p< .001, = -.21) contributed to this prediction.

Absences were then studied as another outcome of teachers labels of students academic

effort. In Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that absences at Time 1 were significantly predictive

of students absences at Time 3 (R = .30, p <.001, = .54). In Step 2, teacher labeling did add

significantly to the prediction of students absences at Time 3, specifically teachers ratings of

students emotional engagement at Time 3 (R = .07, p =.03, = -.13) contributed to this

prediction.

Referrals were then studied as the last outcome of teachers labels of students academic

effort. In Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that referrals at Time 1 were significantly predictive

of students referrals at Time 3 (R = .51, p <.001, = .71). In Step 2, teacher labeling did not

add significantly to the prediction of students referrals at Time 3.


Labeling in the Classroom, 19

The next set of analyses examined the influence of teacher labeling on adolescent health

risk behavior. The first regression analysis examined the influence of teachers labeling on

harmful behavior; specifically students experimentation with cigarettes (i.e. did the student ever

try cigarettes?). In Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that experimentation with cigarettes during

Time 1 was not predictive of experimentation with smoking at Time 3 (R = .00, p = .917, = -

.01). Yet in Step 2, teacher labeling did significantly add to the prediction of students

experimentation with smoking, specifically teachers ratings of behavioral engagement at Time 3

(R = .07, p =.004, = -.21).

The influence of teachers labeling on lifetime use of cigarettes was then assessed using

regression analysis. In Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that students lifetime engagement in

smoking cigarettes at Time 1 was significantly predictive of students lifetime engagement in

smoking cigarettes at Time 3 (R = .26, p< .001, = .51). Yet in Step 2, teacher labeling

variables did not significantly add to the prediction of students lifetime engagement in smoking.

Consumption of alcohol was then measured as a possible outcome of teachers labels. In

Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that lifetime consumption of alcohol at Time 1 was

significantly predictive of students alcohol use at Time 3 (R = .43, p.001, =.66). In Step 2,

teacher labeling significantly added to the prediction of students engagement in alcohol

consumption, specifically teachers ratings of behavioral engagement at Time 3 (R = .02, p

=.004, = -.14) contributed to this prediction.

Students alcohol consumption in the past 30 days was also measured as a possible

outcome of teachers labels. In Step 1, the analysis demonstrated that whether students consumed

alcohol within the past 30 days at Time 1 was significantly predictive of students consumption

of alcohol in the past 30 days at Time 3 (R = .12, p, .001, =.35). In Step 2, teacher labeling
Labeling in the Classroom, 20

did add significantly to the prediction of students engagement in alcohol consumption over the

past 30 days, specifically teachers ratings of students emotional engagement at Time 3 (R =

.03, p =.05, = -.18) contributed to this prediction.

Students experimentation with marijuana (i.e. had the student ever tried marijuana?) was

then examined. In Step 1, students experimentation with marijuana at Time 1 was significantly

predictive of students experimentation with marijuana at Time 3 (R = .27, p< .001, =.52). In

Step 2, teacher labeling did add significantly to the prediction of students engagement in

marijuana use, specifically teachers ratings of students helplessness at Time 3 (R = .04, p<

.001, = -.17) contributed to this prediction.

Whether students had used marijuana within the past 30 days was the last possible

harmful behavior outcome that was studied as a result of teacher labeling. In Step 1, the analysis

demonstrated that whether a student had used marijuana within the past 30 days at Time 1 was a

significant predictor of students using marijuana at Time 3 (R = .19, p> .001, = .44). In Step

2, teacher labeling did add significantly to the prediction of students engagement in marijuana

use in the past 30 days, specifically teachers ratings of students helplessness at Time 3 (R =

.05, p< .001, = .19) contributed to this prediction.

Positive Labels

In order to determine if students labeled positively at Time 1 were still motivated to do

well academically at Time 3, in spite of having a difficult time transitioning, teacher evaluations

of students behavioral and emotional engagement, their helplessness, as well as their actual

grades during Time 2 were measured. The mean of these four variables was then calculated and

those who had the lowest scores (i.e. were the least behaviorally/emotionally engaged, most

helpless, received lowest grades, etc.) during Time 2 were further analyzed. Out of these students
Labeling in the Classroom, 21

(82), those who had scored above the mean on all four measures during Time 1 (i.e. were rated

as behaviorally/emotionally engaged, not helpless, doing well academically, etc.) were compared

to those who scored above the mean during Time 3 to see if there was any overlap. It was

reasoned that if the overlap of students from Time 1 to Time 3 was high, that is, if the same

students did well academically during Time 1 and then again during Time 3, despite their

difficulty during Time 2, then the hypothesis would be supported.

For emotional engagement, out of the 12 participants who scored above the mean at Time

1 (M = 2.37, SD = .23), 8 of these same participants, or 67%, also scored above the mean at Time

3 (M = 2.44, SD = .28), indicating that even though these students experienced a difficult

transition, their positive labels motivated them to still be emotionally engaged in the classroom

setting.

Evaluating behavioral engagement, out of the two participants who scored above the

mean at Time 1 (M = 2.72, SD = .30), both failed to score above the mean at Time 3 (M = 2.78,

SD = .28), indicating that for high achieving students who were negatively impacted by the

transition, being behaviorally engaged in the classroom setting was much more difficult.

In regards to helplessness, out of the 35 participants who scored below the mean at Time

1 (M = 1.60, SD = .80; which indicated lower levels of helplessness), 13 of these participants, or

37%, scored below the mean at Time 3 (M = 1.87, SD = .92), indicating that for more than half

of the high achieving students at Time 1, feelings of helplessness were still persistent even after

the initial transition period had passed.

For teacher ratings of students academic performance, out of the 32 participants who

scored above the mean at Time 1 (M = 3.69, SD = 1.26), 13 of these participants, or 41%, scored

above the mean at Time 3 (M = 3.60, SD = 1.167), indicating that for close to half of the high
Labeling in the Classroom, 22

achieving students at Time 1, having a difficult transition was not found to be correlated with

impaired performance at the high school level.

In relation to performance, students grades at Time 1 and Time 3 were also compared.

Out of the 25 students who scored above the mean at Time 1 (M = 85.74, SD = 7.98), 20 of these

students, 80%, also scored above the mean (M = 80.38, SD = 10.93) at Time 3, indicating that

while the transition for these high achieving students may have been difficult, it did not prevent

the majority of them from earning high grades like they did in middle school.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to identify if teacher labeling of students not only occurs in

the classroom setting, but if these labels carry over from one academic setting to another,

specifically from middle school to high school. Teacher evaluations of students behavioral and

emotional engagement, along with ratings of helplessness, were identified as the labeling

variables while academic performance (e.g. grades, absences, and referrals) and engagement in

health risk behaviors were identified as the outcome measures.

The findings indicate that labeling in the classroom setting is a real occurrence which has

the potential to have long lasting, and even negative, consequences for students as they transition

to higher levels of education. Teachers labels of students behavioral engagement and school

related helplessness were the best predictors of students academic performance and engagement

in health risk behaviors. The stability of these two outcomes across Time 1 and Time 3 was

another significant indicator of how labels can continue to affect and impact students as they

transition from one grade to another. Notably, teachers labels of students emotional

engagement had little influence on student outcomes. Although ratings of emotional engagement

were not seen as being significantly correlated with academic performance and engagement in
Labeling in the Classroom, 23

health risk behaviors, this labeling variable was predictive in regards to whether or not students,

who demonstrated positive eighth grade adjustment, continued to do well in high school, despite

a difficult transition. The effects of labeling were also found to be exacerbated by whether or not

the student attended a low vs. high SES school. Specifically, poor performing students at low

SES schools were rated as more helpless and less behaviorally and emotionally engaged than

poor performing students at high SES schools.

The stability of students negative adaptation from Time 1 to Time 3, specifically in

regards to decreased academic performance and increased participation in health risk behaviors,

can be interpreted as evidence of labeling theory. Researchers have illustrated that while high

school can be a stressful transition for students, it also gives them the opportunity to start over, to

meet new friends, get involved in more activities, even challenge themselves academically

(Smith, 2006). Yet if poorly adjusted students from middle school have internalized this label, if

they now see themselves as being incapable of being a good student, they will inevitably

continue to do poorly in high school despite the opportunity to shed that image and start anew.

This would explain why individuals who did poorly in eighth grade continued to do poorly in

ninth grade, as well as continued to engage in risky health behaviors.

Although there were four significant correlations found between Time 1 teacher labeling

variables (i.e. behavioral engagement and helplessness), and the outcome measures of academic

performance and health risk behavior, at Time 3, all three labeling variables were found to be

significantly correlated with every single outcome measure. What is particularly significant

about this finding is that it illustrates that negative labels at the high school level tend to have

even greater negative implications for students than they did at the middle school level. This

certainly validates the previous point about how the stability of outcomes from middle school to
Labeling in the Classroom, 24

high school can be viewed as evidence of labeling theory, specifically in regards to how

persistent labels can be. If an individual sees themselves as a poor student in middle school, not

only is this label likely to impact them as they transition to high school, but as the results

indicate, it may affect them to a much greater extent, further increasing their likelihood of

engaging in health risk behaviors. As previous findings have indicated, the more these poorly

adapted students become disengaged with the schooling process, the more isolated and helpless

they become. In turn, they may involve themselves in risk seeking behavior, ultimately resulting

in an increased drop out rate (Caraway et al., 2003; Little & Garber, 2004). Such negative

consequences are further exacerbated as a result of transitioning to a new school (i.e. middle

school to high school). Transitioning to another school environment tends to be a highly

emotional and stressful time for some students (Barber & Olson, 2004; Smith, 2006), perhaps

suggesting why students decreased emotional engagement was so much more predictive of their

engagement in health risk behaviors in ninth grade than it was when they were in eighth grade.

In addition to labeling variables being predictive of negative outcomes, they were also

found to be stable across Time 1 and Time 3. As the results indicate, a positive correlation was

found for all three labeling variables at Time 1 and Time 3, illustrating that the pervasiveness of

labels, from one school environment to another, was observed within this study. Clearly this

finding supports the original hypothesis and ultimate purpose of this current research, which was

to illustrate that negative labels have the potential to follow a student as they progress to higher

levels of education as a result of their academic information being passed from one teacher to the

next. It was reasoned that these evaluations would influence the current teachers perception of

the poor performing student, resulting in confirmation bias, or seeking out information that

supports the label of that student, enabling that label to persist as a result of a self fulfilling
Labeling in the Classroom, 25

prophesy, where students continue to act out the behaviors expected of them (Lilly, Cully, &

Ball, 2007). This would explain why students who were engaged in negative health risk

behaviors and had low academic performance in eighth grade continued to abuse substances and

perform poorly in school in ninth grade, illustrating the long term consequences labeling can

have if such labels become internalized and used as a measure of academic capability.

While the data supported the hypothesis that negative labeling of helplessness by teachers

of their students does have a negative impact on these students academic potential and

involvement in risk seeking behavior, it is not clear as to whether it is the label per say that led to

these significant results. What makes interpreting this data difficult under the lens of labeling

theory is that in the questionnaires that were given to students, they were never asked to state

whether or not they saw themselves as good vs. poor students, nor was there ever any direct

observation of teacher-student interaction to determine if these different types of students were,

in fact, treated differently, as was documented in the Rosenthal (1963) experiment. Without this

crucial information regarding students own self perception of themselves as a result of

positive/negative interactions with their teachers, it cannot be concluded, with certainty, that

negative outcomes of low grades and involvement in health risk behaviors is a direct result of

negative labels. Moreover, it is not clear that teachers necessarily had access to previous

teachers labels regarding these students and, as such, that their impressions and future labels

of these students were shaped by this information. Instead, these negative outcomes may just be

evidence of typical coping strategies enacted by adolescents when they are having a difficult

school transition vs. an act of secondary deviance, or evidence of a self fulfilling prophesy.

Further, teachers may be making valid inferences regarding students engagement and

approaches to school and if indeed they are disengaged and have few active approaches for
Labeling in the Classroom, 26

coping with school challenges, poor academic performance and engagement in risky behavior

may be the result.

While research has clearly shown that there is a racial bias in terms of labeling

individuals socially deviant, specifically that minority individuals tend to carry this label more

often than White individuals (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007), it was the hopes of this current

research to identify if such bias also made its way into the classroom setting. According to the

results, in terms of teacher ratings, individuals at the lower SES schools were perceived as being

the least engaged and the most helpless compared to students at the higher SES schools.

Unfortunately, because demographic information of the student body was not collected for each

of the schools, identifying if more ethnic minorities attended the lower SES schools cannot be

determined, and thus it cannot be confirmed that ethnic minorities are at a greater disadvantage

when it comes to being negatively labeled by their teachers. Yet this finding is nevertheless

significant because it still illustrates a system of bias occurring in the school system, specifically

a class bias that can potentially put poorer students at a greater disadvantage academically than

their more affluent peers. Similar to how the roughnecks in William Chambliss experiment

were seen as being more likely to engage in criminal behavior simply because of their decreased

economic standing (Lilly, Cully, & Ball, 2007), students at low SES schools may be perceived as

less behaviorally and emotionally engaged, and more helpless, by their teachers for this same

reason. Yet because GPAs from the lower and higher SES schools were not calculated

separately, and thus were not compared to one another, it cannot be determined if students at the

lower SES schools, who were labeled negatively, performed worse academically than their

counterparts at the higher SES schools as a result of these more severe, negative labels.
Labeling in the Classroom, 27

Although we had hypothesized that engaged students would perform well in school as a

result of this positive label, results indicated that high achieving students were able to maintain

their high performance despite the fact that very few of these students were rated high on

behavioral engagement by their teachers. Notably, few of the students who were labeled as

behaviorally engaged during Time 1 remained engaged during Time 3. Although these findings

were not consistent with the initial hypothesis, perhaps these findings emerged because positive

labels were not needed for these high achieving students to continue to do well post transition.

These high achieving students were rated as being highly emotionally engaged by their teachers

during Time 1 and Time 3, and thus perhaps showing enthusiasm or interest in class was enough

to illicit a positive response from a teacher, prompting that teacher to encourage that student to

continue to do well despite the challenges they may be facing as a result of transitioning. As the

Rosenthal (1963) study demonstrated, when teachers know their students have the potential to be

great students, they are willing to work harder and help that student reach their potential, even if

that student does not exhibit clear behavioral instances of interest, such as volunteering to solve a

problem in class or handing in their homework. As such, while these students may not be

behaviorally engaged in the classroom setting, it does not mean they do not understand the

material. They may just need extra encouragement, or they may be intimidated by their new class

environment, specifically the hierarchical setup, explaining why they continue to feel helpless.

As mentioned earlier, high school is the first time students are segregated into specific classes

based on their academic performance. The added work and increased independence of these new

classrooms can be overwhelming, and the fear of not being seen as a good student by their new

teacher (Little & Garber, 2004) may inhibit their engagement in class, preventing the student

from adequately participating. Thus because teachers are more likely to invest time and energy
Labeling in the Classroom, 28

with students who exhibit the potential to do well academically, these high achieving students

continue to do well in school despite the negative labels of low behavioral engagement and high

helplessness.

Limitations

The most significant limitation of this study was not being able to conclude with certainty

that the labeling variables teachers used to evaluate their students (i.e. behavioral engagement,

emotional engagement, and helplessness) as high vs. low performing were indeed internalized by

these same students, leading to either positive or negative outcome variables (i.e. academic

performance and engagement in health risk behaviors). Thus although there were significant and

stable relationships found between these labeling variables and outcomes, without self report

data from the students themselves identifying whether or not they believed teachers labeled

them, and thus whether they were treated differently because of that label, there is much more

research that needs to be conducted in order to obtain more definitive and conclusive results

regarding the extent and consequences of labeling in the classroom.

Another limitation of this study which makes it difficult to determine whether negative

outcomes in high school were solely a result of negative teacher labels is the difficulty in

separating the effects of teacher expectations on students from the effects of having a stressful

transition once entering high school. Researchers have concluded that for many students entering

high school, there tends to be a decline in their GPA as a result of more challenging classes and

thus a more stressful, academic environment (Barber & Olson, 2004; Smith, 2006). Thus with

this in mind, students declines in performance may be the result of transition stress and change

rather than negative labeling by teachers. While negative labeling and experiencing a stressful

transition are certainly interdependent on one another, the ultimate goal of this study was to
Labeling in the Classroom, 29

determine if teacher expectations alone contribute to negative outcomes for students, which is

why the post transition period of these students freshman year (i.e. second semester) was

specifically analyzed. Yet it is not clear if giving these students a mere semester to adjust to high

school was, in fact, enough time to separate these two variables from one another, and thus it is

difficult to predict with certainty as to whether it was the negative labels vs. the transition itself

which led to negative outcomes for students, specifically in regards to academic performance.

Thus more research needs to be conducted regarding students academic performance post

freshman year in high school in order to conclude, with certainty, that students poor

performance in high school is in fact due to teacher expectations vs. a stressful transition to a

new, academic environment.

Simply depending on GPA to identify which students are well vs. poor performing is

problematic since it cannot be concluded with certainty that students who earn low marks are

necessarily seen as poor students by their teachers. These students may very well be receiving

low marks because they are in more challenging classes, yet because they are challenging

themselves as students by taking these more advanced courses, and thus have the potential to do

well, they may still be labeled positively by their teachers despite their lower academic

performance. As such, without knowing what type of classes students are enrolled in, it is

difficult to conclude if teachers labels of these students truly correlate with their academic

performance. It would have been more ideal if the students had been separated via their academic

levels (i.e. basic, Honors, AP, etc.), specifically in terms of their racial make up as opposed to

their SES standing. Without this information, it is difficult to conclude if, similar to social

deviance, minority students are more likely to be labeled as academically deviant and thus more

likely be placed in lower level classes simply because of their skin tone, severely limiting their
Labeling in the Classroom, 30

academic potential. The absence of these two demographic features severely decreases the ability

of this study to identify the effects of labeling in the classroom setting, ultimately reducing the

strength of the reported findings.

In terms of the stability of labels from eighth to ninth grade, previous research had

concluded that prior evaluations from eighth grade could effect, and thus bias, the evaluations

ninth grade teachers gave about their students (Harris, 1991). In this manner, one of the goals of

this study was to illustrate the relationship between eighth and ninth grade labels. Yet because

this study did not ask the ninth grade teachers whether or not prior evaluations from eighth grade

teachers biased their interactions and their evaluations of their incoming students, and because

evaluations were only administered during the second half of they year, it is difficult to conclude

if the stability of these labels was the result of teachers depending on prior evaluations. Then

again, labeling, like stereotyping, tends to be an unconscious action (Link & Phelan, 2001), and

so even if teachers had access to, and were influenced by, past teacher evaluations, they may be

quick to deny that such evaluations effected them, specifically if these evaluations were negative

in nature.

Conclusion

While it may be difficult to conclude from this specific study that teacher expectations

alone have profound effects on students in regards to their academic potential and involvement in

negative health risk behavior, labeling in the classroom setting is not simply a phenomenon: it is

an important reality that should continue to be addressed and researched by future studies.

As stated before, what makes labeling in the classroom so problematic is that like other

types of stereotyping, it is an unconscious process and thus done automatically. After all, the

goal of the educational system within this country is to provide every student with equal access
Labeling in the Classroom, 31

and equal opportunity to a quality education, and thus many individuals who become teachers do

so because they want to enact these goals within their own classrooms, with their own students.

Yet these good intentions are quickly undermined when these teachers begin to treat their

students differently based on their academic achievement, and because they believe so strongly

in creating an environment free of discrimination and favoritism, it becomes difficult for them to

acknowledge the discrepancy between their cognitions and their actions. Thus similar to

unintentional, covert racism, unintentional labeling is probably the most dangerous kind of

labeling, because it is practiced by well intentioned individuals who do not see themselves as

being biased, which makes eliminating it extremely difficult (Mio, Barker, Tumambing, 2009).

The more awareness that is brought to teachers attention about how detrimental this

unintentional action can be for their students, the better prepared teachers will be in recognizing

this differential treatment they enact with their students, preventing poor students from

internalizing this label and allowing it to dictate their potential as both a student, and as an

individual.
Labeling in the Classroom, 32

References

Aikins, J., Bierman, K., & Parker, J. Navigating the transition to junior high school: The

influence of pre-transition friendship and self-system characteristics. Social development,

14. 42-26.

Alspaugh, J. (1998). Achievement loss associated with the transition to middle school

and high school. The Journal of Educational Research, 92. 22-25.

Ames, Carol. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 84. 261-268

Barber, B. & Olsen, J. (2004). Assessing the transitions to middle and high school.

Journal of Adolescent Research, 19. 4-23.

Brenner, N., Collins, J., Kann, L., Warren, C., & Williams, B. (1995). Reliability of the

youth risk behavior survey questionnaire. American Journal of Epidemiology, 141. 575-

580.

Caraway, K., Tucker, C., Reinke, W., & Hall, C. (2003). Self-efficacy, goal orientation,

and fear of failure as predictors of school engagement in high school students.

Psychology in the School, 40. 417-419.

Davis, Heather. (2003). Conceptualizing the role and influences of student-teacher

relationships on childrens social and cognitive development. Educational

Psychologist, 38. 221-223.

Fritzberg, G. (2001). Less than equal: A former urban schoolteacher examines the causes

of educational disadvantagement. The Urban Review, 33. 109-119.

Furrer, C. & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in childrens academic

engagement and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95. 148-159.


Labeling in the Classroom, 33

Harris, M. (1991). Controversy and cumulation: Meta-analysis and research on

interpersonal expectancy effects. PSPB, 17. 317-319.

Hilton, J. & Von Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review Psychology, 47. 245-

252.

Lilly, J., Cully, F., & Ball, R. (2007). Context & consequences. Sage Publications.

Link, B. & Phelan, J. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Reviews Sociology, 27. 368-375.

Little, S. & Garber, J. (2004). Interpersonal achievement orientations and specific

stressors predict depressive and aggressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescent Research,

19. 64-73.

Lotz, R. & Lee, L. Sociability, school, experience, and delinquency. Youth & Society, 31.

199-207.

Mio, J., Barker, L. & Tumambing, J. (2009). Multicultural Psychology: Understanding our

diverse communities. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Moulton, M., Moulton, P., Housewright, M., & Bailey, K. (1998). Gifted and talented:

Exploring the positive and negative aspects of labeling. Roeper Review, 21. 153-154.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S, Girgus, J.S. & Selgiman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and

consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 101. 405-422.

Rosenfeld, L., Richman, J., & Bowen, G. (2000). Social support networks and school

outcomes: The centrality of the teacher. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 17.

205-220.

Rudolph, K., Lambert, S., Clark, A. & Kurlakowsky, K. Negotiating the transition to

middle school: The role of the self-regulatory processes. Child Development, 72. 935.
Labeling in the Classroom, 34

Samdal, O., Wold, B., Klepp, K., & Kannas, L. (2000). Students perception of school

and their smoking and alcohol use: A cross-national study. Addiction Research, 8. 142-

146.

Schultz, R. (1983). Sociopsychological climates and teacher-bias expectancy: A possible

mediating mechanism. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76. 167-168.

Solorzano, D. & Ornelas, A. (2002). A critical race analysis of advanced placement

classes: A case of educational inequality. Journal of Latinos and Education, 4. 220.

Smith, J. (2006). Examining the long-term impact of achievement loss during the

transition to high school. The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 17. 211.

Stevens, P., & Griffin, J. (2001). Youth high risk behaviors: Survey and results. Journal

of Addictions and Offender Counseling, 22. 33-34.

Wellford, C. (1975). Labelling theory and criminology: An assessment. Social Problems,

22. 337-339.
Labeling in the Classroom, 35

Table 1

Means and Standard Deviations for all Time1 and Time 3 Variables

Variables Time 1 Time 3

Mean Standard Mean Standard


Deviation Deviation
Behavioral Engagement 2.79 .30 3.04 .51
Emotional Engagement 2.37 .23 2.99 .53

Helplessness 1.61 .80 1.87 .92


Smoking .27 .83 .26 .44
Lifetime Smoking .45 .50 .24 .97
Alcohol Use .75 1.29 1.21 1.59
Alcohol Consumption .17 .60 .43 .99
(30 days)
Marijuana Use .09 .58 .35 1.06
Marijuana Use (30 days) .05 .35 .14 .58
Grades 85.74 7.98 80.39 10.93
Absences 5.59 5.15 6.09 6.20
Referrals 2.01 7.37 .81 2.30
Labeling in the Classroom, 36

Table 2

Correlations among Time 1 and Time 3 Labeling Variables and Outcomes

Time 1 Variables Time 3 Variables


Outcomes Behavioral Emotional Helplessness Behavioral Emotional Helplessness
Engagement Engagement Engagement Engagement
Smoking .02 -.10 -.02 -.23** -.27** .21**
Lifetime -.16** .05 .17 .16**
Smoking
Alcohol -.12* -.02 .13* -.25** -.19** .11**
Use
Alcohol -.02 .06 .1 -.22** -.15** .16**
Use (30
days)
Marijuana .05 -.01 .04 -.23** -.18** .17**
Use
Marijuana .05 .04 .06 -.18** -.16** .19**
Use (30
days)
Grades .45 .05 -.73** .38** .30** -.60**
Absences -.13* .14* .24** -.26** -.16* .25**
Referrals -.05 -.02 .42** -.37** -.09 .34**
*p <.05, **p <.01
Labeling in the Classroom, 37

Table 3

Correlations: Stability of Outcomes between Time 1 and Time 3

Time 3
Outcomes Smoking Lifetime Alcohol Alcohol Marijuana Marijuana Grades
Smoking Use Use (30 Use Use (30
days) days)
Time 1
Smoking .002
Lifetime
Smoking .51**
Alcohol Use .65**
Alcohol Use
(30 days) .35**
Marijuana .49**
Use
Marijuana
Use (30 .41**
days)
Grades .77**
*p <.05, **p <.01
Labeling in the Classroom, 38

Table 4

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses Examining Labeling Variables on Outcome Drug

Measures at Time 1 and Time 3

Smoking Lifetime Alcohol Use Alcohol Use Marijuana Use Marijuana


Smoking (30 Days) Use
(30 days)

R R R R R R
Step 1
Time 1
Drug Use -.01 .00 .51* .26** .67** .43** .35** .12** .52** .27** .44** .19**
Step 2
Time 3
Behavioral
Engagement -21 .07 -.03 .01 .06 .02** -.02 .03* -.06 .04** -.05 .05**

Emotional
Engagement .00 .07 .03 .01 -.14* .02** -.12* .03* .02 .04** -.03 .05**

Helplessness .10 .07 .07 .01 .09 .02** .11 .03* .17** .04** .19** .05**
* p <.05, **p <.01

Table 5

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses Examining Labeling Variables on Outcome

Academic Measures at Time 1 and Time 3

Performance Grades Absences Referrals

R R R R
Step 1
Time 1
Academic
Measures .46** .22** .78** .60** .55** .30** .71** .51**
Step 2
Time 3
Behavioral .28** .40** .12* .06** -.11 .07** -.09 .05**
Engagement
Emotional .02 .40** .03 .06** -.13* .07** -.11 .05**
Engagement
Helplessness -.52** .40** -.21** .06** .12* .07** .11 .05**
*p <.05, **p <.01

También podría gustarte