Está en la página 1de 14

ROBERT A.

LEVI NE
Ethnographic Studies of Childhood:
A Historical Overview
ABSTRACT In this article, I briey survey the ethnographic research literature on childhood in the 20th century, beginning with the
social and intellectual contexts for discussions of childhood at the turn of the 20th century. The observations of BronislawMalinowski and
Margaret Mead in the 1920s were followed by later ethnographers, also describing childhood, some of whom criticized developmental
theories; still others were inuenced initially by Freudian and other psychoanalytic theories and later by the suggestions of Edward
Sapir for research on the childs acquisition of culture. The Six Cultures Study led by John Whiting at midcentury was followed by
diverse trends of the period after 1960including eld studies of infancy, the social and cultural ecology of childrens activities, and
language socialization. Ethnographic evidence on hunting and gathering and agricultural peoples was interpreted in evolutionary as
well as cultural and psychological terms. The relationship between ethnography and developmental psychology remained problematic.
[Keywords: history, ethnography, child development, cultural acquisition, interpersonal relationships]
I
NTHIS ARTICLE, I present anoverviewof ethnographic
research on childhood during the 20th century, includ-
ing works in biological, linguistic, social, and psychologi-
cal anthropology. The literature is extensive and interdis-
ciplinary, appearing in journals of psychology, psychiatry,
and linguistics, as well as anthropology, and in monographs
and volumes of collected papers. It grew during the second
half of the century, covering all regions of the world, and
in the 1990s there were more ethnographic studies of child-
hood published in book form than in any previous decade.
The aim of this article is to show the extent and di-
versity of published ethnographic accounts of childhood
over the past 80 years,
1
providing a bibliography and a ba-
sic historical background that might prove useful for future
analyses.
2
In the interests of brevity, I have limited this ac-
count to ethnographies of infancy and childhood in domes-
tic and community settings. That has meant omitting not
only theoretical treatises, psychometric studies, and school
studies but also other bodies of anthropological research
on childhood: studies of immigrant children (e.g., Suarez-
Orozco and Suarez-Orozco 2001); studies of chronically ill,
dying, or disabled children (e.g., Bluebond-Langner 1978,
1996); research on child abuse and other social problems of
children in diverse cultures (e.g., Korbin 1981; Montgomery
2001; Montgomery et al. 2003; Scheper-Hughes 1987); the
cultural politics literature on childhood (e.g., Scheper-
Hughes 1992; Scheper-Hughes and Sargent 1998; Stephens
AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST, Vol. 109, Issue 2, pp. 247260, ISSN 0002-7294 online ISSN 1548-1433. C
2007 by the American Anthropological Association.
All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the University of California Presss Rights
and Permissions website, http://www.ucpressjournals.com/reprintInfo.asp. DOI: 10.1525/AA.2007.109.2.247.
1995); and the archaeology of childhood (e.g., Baxter 2005).
These bodies of research deserve, and in some cases have
had, overviews of their own.
An ethnographic study of childhood as used herein is
a descriptive account, based on eld observations and in-
terviews, of the lives, activities, and experiences of children
in a particular place and time, and of the contextssocial,
cultural, institutional, economicthat make sense of their
behavior there and then. There would be no need for such
descriptive accounts if childhood in ideal and practice were
uniformacross humanpopulations andhistorical periods or
if the developmental pathways for children were randomly
distributed among humans. The ethnography of childhood,
then, is based on the premiseconstantly reexamined in
empirical researchthat the conditions and shape of child-
hood tend to vary in central tendency from one population
to another, are sensitive to population-specic contexts,
and are not comprehensible without detailed knowledge
of the socially and culturally organized contexts that give
them meaning.
HISTORICAL BACKGROUND
The recording and publishing of descriptions of the lives,
activities, and experience of children did not originate
with anthropology, but in the course of the 20th century
anthropologists gave such accounts a distinctive stamp
248 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
as research to enhance knowledge of human variation for
scientic and policy purposes. From the 1920s onward,
anthropologists published ethnographic reports on child-
hood among human populations across the world, devised
and rened observational approaches, and developed
and borrowed theoretical frameworks for analyzing data
collected in eldwork.
Descriptions of customary practices regarding children
and their activities appeared in the texts of many cultures,
non-Western as well as Western, before the 19th century,
including travelers reports on foreign practices that seemed
strange (and therefore noteworthy) and clergymens advice
to parents in a particular context. The 19th century saw
a heightened public interest in children in Western coun-
tries, manifested in literary and journalistic representations
of children as innocent victims of economic exploitation,
the child-saving movement that sought to abolish child
labor and provide care for orphans, the laws that made
school attendance compulsory, and efforts to create an
empirical science that would replace religious doctrine and
philosophical speculation in public discourse about chil-
dren. This public interest had been institutionalizedin
school systems, orphanages, and academic and medical
specialtiesby the end of the century, when anthropology
and other social science elds were taking their modern
forms.
In the United States and western Europe soon after
1900, there were some fundamental alterations in the con-
ditions of childrens lives: (1) Infant and child mortality be-
gan to decline, partly as the result of public health measures
(purication of drinking water, pasteurization of milk, etc.)
and sanitary practices in the home and other settings; and
(2) mass secondary school enrollment extended schooling
throughout childhood and into adolescence.
The biomedical changes formed the basis of what might
be called the medicalization of childcare, as illustrated
by the hygiene movement in the United States, which was
actively promoted by government and medical authorities
(Ewbank and Preston 1990; Preston and Haines 1991). The
vast increases in school attendance during the late 19th and
early 20th centuries called public attention to the processes
of learning and developmental change during childhood
and stimulated the emergence of child psychology and edu-
cational psychology, led by pioneers such as G. Stanley Hall,
Edward L. Thorndike, James Mark Baldwin, and (in France)
Alfred Binet. Inthe United States, questions of learning were
made urgent by the problems of educating large numbers
of immigrant children.
Thus, from about 1880 to 1920, childhood was increas-
ingly formulated in what might be called pediatric and ped-
agogical terms, and there was public discussion about how
science might improve the care and learning of children in
the contexts of newly expanded medical and educational
institutions. There were also new and radical ideas about
the family, women, and child rearing that arose during
this period in the Anglo-American world, although their
greatest impact would be later. Women were to be freed
fromthe home to study at postsecondary institutions, vote,
and participate in public life; fertility was to be voluntarily
controlled through contraception supported by sex educa-
tion, so that families could be kept small; and children were
to be raised scientically rather than according to the dic-
tates of custom.
In this milieu, Sigmund Freuds writings became avail-
able (in what he regarded as inadequate translations) and
inuential. Freud was hardly an extreme environmentalist,
but he was widely interpreted as claiming that young chil-
dren were not only sentient and passionate beings but also
highly vulnerable to parental and other early inuences,
for worse as well as for better. By 1920, a Freudian con-
text for the interest in childrens experience, often based on
an exaggeration or distortion of Freuds ideas, was becom-
ing widespread in public discourse on childhood. Freuds
works drew unprecedented attention to the subjective ex-
perience of children and added a public preoccupation with
the newand anxious questions: What is the best way to raise
a child? What is the normal child? Through what stages of
child development do infants become adults?
Christian missionaries and colonial administrators of
the late 19th and early 20th centuries wrote books describ-
ing the customs of the peoples with whom they worked in
Oceania, Asia, Africa, and the Americas and included sec-
tions (often with photographs) on customs of child rear-
ing that struck them as noteworthy, including adoption,
naming and initiation ceremonies, practices of breast feed-
ing and sibling care, and childrens houses. Although these
observers were not as deliberate, comprehensive, or dis-
ciplined as the anthropologists who followed them, and
although they were more likely to make ethnocentric judg-
ments, such writers often had a good command of the lo-
cal language, having spent long periods among the peoples
they wrote about, with many opportunities to observe chil-
dren. Some of the later missionary ethnographers like Henri
Junod (1927), who wrote about the Thonga of Mozambique,
were trained inanthropology and published valuable if frag-
mentary accounts of childrens lives in cultural context.
Ethnographies of childhood by missionaries and colonial
administrators continued into the 1940s (e.g., Childs 1949;
Elwin 1947; Raum 1940).
This early ethnographic literature provided the initial
evidence for wide cultural variations in childhood environ-
ments used by anthropologists in generalizing about hu-
man childhood (e.g., Mead 1931; van Gennep 1960). Exam-
ination of this literature made it clear that there was diver-
gence among peoples of the world in their concepts of the
best way to raise children and of what constitutes normal
child development. The stage was set for future confronta-
tions between ethnographic evidence and the concepts of
normal child development emerging from theory and re-
search in Western countries.
By 1928, professional anthropologists were publishing
ethnographic accounts of childhood based on their own
eldwork in non-Western communities. Margaret Mead
(1928a, 1928b) and Bronislaw Malinowski (1929) were
LeVine Ethnography of Childhood 249
among the rst. In her brief article Samoan Children at
Work and Play in Natural History, Mead presents a number
of ways in which childhood in Samoa as she observed it dif-
fers fromthat of children in the United States: Samoan chil-
drenchange their ownnames, move fromone relative to an-
other, live in families of as many as 20, and are spoiled and
pampered until the age of ve years old, whenthey become
the caretakers or trainers of younger children. They have to
work at gender-specic tasks of increasing difculty as they
get older during childhood. (No ages are given.) They have
no toys, but they participate in adult life almost from the
start and play games in which they imitate adult activities.
Yet they are punished if they stand out conspicuously above
other children or attempt grown-up tasks above their sta-
tus. These are themes that would be elaborated in detail in
the ethnographies that followed over the 20th century.
Like Mead, Malinowski, in The Sexual Life of Savages in
North Western Melanesia (1929), was addressing the general
public rather than other professional anthropologists, and
his whole book was intended to describe Trobriand ideas
and practices concerning sexual behavior that contrast with
those of the West. But he also sought to show (as he had
argued in his earlier book, Sex and Repression in a Savage
Society [1927]) that Trobrianders, being matrilineal in so-
cial organization, had family dynamics and sexual devel-
opment contrasting sharply with those Freud claimed to
be generically human in his concept of the Oedipus com-
plex. Malinowskis crystalline descriptions and vivid anec-
dotes were carefully selected to make this point: Trobriand
father and mother are of equal status; fathers participate
in infant care and take great pride in their infants; children
are free and independent; and, although parents sometimes
beat their children, Malinowski also observed quite as of-
ten childrenbeating their parents (1929:45). He reports the
childrens sex games he observed (with no interference by
older persons [Malinowski 1929:50]) and those he heard
about but was unable to observe. While making his case for
a revision of Freuds ideas, he was demonstrating the ethno-
graphers command of language, context, culture, and be-
havior that would set a standard for future studies.
THE PLASTICITY PRINCIPLE AND HUMAN VARIABILITY
The precursor to ethnographic research on childhood in
U.S. anthropology was Franz Boass anthropometric work
on child growth among European immigrants to the United
States. Boas had been trained in anthropometric methods
in 1883 in the Berlin laboratory of Rudolf Virchow, the pi-
oneer of medical pathology (and physical anthropology),
for whom plasticity was a major biological principle, oper-
ating at every level from the cell to the organism (Stocking
1968:139, 1974:22). Virchow argued, and Boas later taught,
that the plasticity characteristic of biological processes often
made it difcult to distinguish between normal and patho-
logical variants, mandating caution in claiming as patho-
logical a variant formthat might actually represent an adap-
tive response to a particular environment. In other words,
difference may betoken adaptive variation rather than de-
ciency or defect (Boas 1902; see also reprint in Stocking
1974:3641).
Boas brought this perspective to his studies of
child growth among immigrants, rst in Worcester,
Massachusetts, when he was teaching in G. Stanley Halls
Psychology Department at Clark University from 1889 to
1892, later in New York from 1908 to 1910. After the large
New York study, Boas (1912) formulated a developmen-
tal perspective suggesting not only that human growth is
inuenced by environmental factors but also that, given
the gradual maturation of the human nervous system, the
childs mental makeup must also be affected by the social
and geographical environment (1912:217218).
Boas left further specication of this perspective to his
students Edward Sapir, RuthBenedict, and Margaret Mead
the founders of the culture and personality movement
although it is unclear whether he liked what they did with
it (Stocking 1992). His silence during the next 30 years on
the very cultural psychology of child development to which
he had opened the door was no doubt related to his well-
known antagonismto theoretical speculation. But as he was
well acquainted with the psychologies of his time, the si-
lence seems to indicate an especially prescient skepticism
about psychological theories and methods, many of which
were eventually thrown aside by child psychologists them-
selves in the course of the 20th century.
This instability in psychology points to the paradox
later anthropologists of childhood would face: Anthropolo-
gists are at least partly dependent on developmental knowl-
edge fromother disciplines in their assumptions about how
children experience their environmentsincluding the en-
vironmental features to which they are sensitive and their
age-related concepts for understandingand they have of-
ten turned to psychology and psychiatry for guidance in
making these assumptions plausible. For much of the 20th
century, however, this guidance was unreliable, as one de-
velopmental theory followed another into the trash heap
of history.
The Freudian theory of psychosexual stages led some
ethnographers to focus their data collection on feeding,
toilet training, and emerging sexuality in young children,
whereas behavioristic psychology recommended recording
the rewards and punishments in the childs environment.
Whenthese theories lost credibility, sodidthe ethnographic
accounts based on them as adequate records of childhood
experience.
The turnover in developmental theories in psychology
continued after 1960, as the inuence of Jean Piaget and
other cognitive theorists waxed and then waned, and the
social-cognitive formulations of Lev Vygotsky and his stu-
dents and the attachment theory of John Bowlby and Mary
Ainsworth introduced a new but not universally accepted
focus on social interactions.
Thus the chronic instability of dominant theories
concerning the childs psychological development during
the 20th century posed a quandary for ethnographers of
250 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
childhood. If they organized their eldwork around the
best knowledge available at a particular moment, they
risked having their account of childhood become obsolete
and inadequate by the time they published their ndings.
Yet they had to make some assumptions about what was
worth observing and recording in the environments and
activities of children in diverse settings, and it was the de-
velopmental psychology of the day that offered attractive if
potentially faulty assumptions. The ways in which anthro-
pologists of childhood dealt with this dilemma is taken up
in the following sections.
CULTURAL CRITIQUE AND ETHNOGRAPHIC
EXPLORATION
A characteristic response of anthropologists to the claims
of developmental psychology and psychiatry was to use
ethnographic evidence to prove them culture bound, sug-
gesting that a theory needed to be modied to encompass
cultural variation or abandoned as a false generalization
about the human species. Malinowski and Mead led the
way. Malinowski (1927) proposed that Freuds concept of
the Oedipus complex in young children had to be modi-
ed for matrilineal societies like that of the Trobriand Is-
landers, and Mead (1928b) argued that the Samoans she
studied did not experience the adolescent turmoil posited
by Hall (1904) as a human universal. Thus, before 1930, an-
thropologists had initiated a critical ethnography of child-
hood that could play a decisive role in rejecting universal
propositions falsiable by a single negative instance (Kauf-
mann 1944) in the study of child development.
Mead (1932) provided another example of this gady
role for ethnographic research on childhood when she pub-
lished an article criticizing Piagets theory of childhood ani-
mism on the basis of interviews she conducted with Manus
children in Melanesia. Psychologists nevertheless contin-
ued to generate developmental propositions claiming uni-
versal validity during the rest of the 20th century, even af-
ter John Whitings (1954) persuasive case for cross-cultural
analysis in the Handbook of Social Psychology, so the gad-
y role has remained relevant, and anthropologists have
continued to exercise their veto with evidence from non-
Western cultures.
Piagets universalist account of childhood cognitive de-
velopment, which became inuential among U.S. psychol-
ogists after 1963, also came under empirical attack with
cross-cultural evidence at that time (e.g., Greeneld 1966;
Shweder and LeVine 1975). Other proposed universals of
child development that were subject to cultural critique in-
cluded Lawrence Kohlbergs (1984) cognitively based model
of moral development (Shweder et al. 1990), Carol Gilli-
gans (1982) conception of gender differences in develop-
ment (Miller 1994), and the John Bowlby-Mary Ainsworth
attachment theory (Harwood et al. 1995; LeVine and Nor-
man 2001). However powerful these cultural critiques may
seem to anthropologists and to some leading psycholo-
gists such as Jerome Bruner (1990), developmental psychol-
ogy has consistently immunized itself not only to evidence
from ethnography and cross-cultural replications that casts
doubt on its theories du jour but even to the gross limita-
tions of its empirical base for generalizing about childhood:
As of 2000, 88 percent of the primary schoolaged children
in the world lived in the less-developed regions (UN Pop-
ulation Division 2005:10) where anthropologists typically
work, but most psychological studies of children are con-
ducted in the United States and a few other Western coun-
tries. The resulting knowledge decit has not been recog-
nized by the child development research eld, even in the
agenda-setting report by a National Academy of Sciences
panel on The Science of Early Childhood Development
(Shonkoff and Phillips 2000).
Thus, relations between anthropology and develop-
mental psychology have been sporadic and often distant.
Yet there have beenanthropologists throughout the century
whoaddressedtheir researchtoissues inchilddevelopment,
and psychologists who have conducted culturally informed
contextual researchonchildrenindiverse cultures. The eld
studies of psychologists such as Wayne Dennis (1940), Pa-
tricia M. Greeneld (1966, 2004), Heidi Keller (in press),
Barbara Rogoff (2003), Charles Super (Super and Harkness
1985, 1986), and Edward Z. Tronick (Tronick et al. 1992;
Tronick et al. 1987) are examples of this latter tendency.
ETHNOGRAPHY OF CHILDHOOD AS PART
OF ANTHROPOLOGY
Anthropologists, regardless of their developmental assump-
tions, sought to describe child rearing and the activities of
children throughout the world. Even Mead (1930) did not
intend to limit the anthropology of childhood to disproving
the propositions of psychologists. She characterized world
cultural variation in child rearing as a laboratory in which
thousand-year experiments were being conducted by dif-
ferent peoples. The results were retrievable through ethno-
graphic eldwork in distant places like Melanesia, to be
brought back to the Western world for the resolution of
issues like whether permissive rearing was advisable for
U.S. middle-class children. Although they might have dis-
agreed with the broad sweep of this claim, anthropologists
were inspired by Meads metaphor of a laboratory and its
mission of learning lessons about childhood from other
cultures. Thus, in the 1930s and later, ethnographers sought
to add to our knowledge of childhood by publishing their
eld observations.
In Britain, the students of MalinowskiRaymond Firth
(1936), Audrey I. Richards (1932, 1956), Ian Hogbin (1931),
C. H. Wedgwood (1938), Phyllis Kaberry (1939), E. E. Evans-
Pritchard (1940, 1953), and Margaret Read (1960, 1968)
and those directly inuenced by him like Meyer Fortes
(1938, 1949)published accounts of childhood in vary-
ing amounts of detail. In his ethnographic classic, We, the
Tikopia: Kinship in Primitive Polynesia (1936), Firth described
the relational context of childhood and the initiation ritu-
als for boys. In Chisungu (1956), Richards devoted an entire
LeVine Ethnography of Childhood 251
monograph to the description of girls initiation among the
Bemba of Zambia. Evans-Pritchard, in The Nuer (1940) and
Kinship and Marriage among the Nuer (1953), offered gen-
eralizations about Nuer childhood and fatherchild rela-
tions based on his eld observations. Read, a student of
Malinowski who pursued a career in colonial and interna-
tional education, published a book-length study, Children of
Their Fathers (1960, 1968), of childhood among the Ngoni of
Nyasaland/Malawi. Fortes, who had received a Ph.D. in ed-
ucational psychology before turning to anthropology, pro-
duced an unsurpassed observational account of learning
and social relationships among Tallensi children in 1938,
and then published more about the course of childhood in
The Web of Kinship among the Tallensi (1949).
These works by a generation of anthropologists inu-
enced directly by Malinowski leave no doubt that by the
1930s childhood was an established topic of ethnographic
description, often in the context of kinship or ritual, some-
times in relation to education or socialization, only oc-
casionally with psychological interpretations. Childhood
was part of their anthropology, not a topic borrowed from
developmental psychology or other disciplines (although
Richards and Fortes knew the child development literature
of their time). Future generations of British and U.S. ethno-
graphers would continue to describe the care, relationships,
and learning of children, and their social participation,
in published reports from Africa (e.g., Gibbs 1965; Goody
1982; Grindal 1972; Leis 1972; Peshkin 1972; Smith 1954),
the Americas (e.g., Bolin 2006; De Laguna 1965; Hughes
1974; Lantis 1960), Asia (e.g., Lebra 1976, 1990, 1994; Peak
1991; Schwalb and Schwalb 1996; Stafford 1995), Ocea-
nia (e.g., Fajans 1997; Howard 1970; Mead and MacGregor
1951; Ritchie 1957; Toren 1990, 1993; Williams 1969), and
Europe (e.g., Mead and Wolfenstein 1954; Norman 1991).
THE INFLUENCES OF FREUD, THE NEO-FREUDIANS,
AND EDWARD SAPIR
Some anthropologists in the rst half of the 20th cen-
tury conducted ethnographic eldwork focused on the
Freudianstages of psychosexual development inearly child-
hood. This began with Geza Roheims attemptsuggested
by Freud and funded by his disciple Marie Bonaparteto
prove Freud right in claiming the Oedipus complex to be
universal and, more urgently, to prove Malinowski (1927)
wrong in his claim, based on ethnographic evidence from
the Trobriands, that the Oedipus complex takes a distinc-
tive form in matrilineal societies. In 1928, Roheim began a
three-year trip that involved eldwork of varying durations
among four non-Western peoples, two of them matrilineal.
His results were published in 1932. Roheims conclusions
were predictably supportive of Freud against Malinowski,
but some of his eld methods, including observations of
child behavior with dolls to elicit reactions, were innovative
then and interesting even now. (His more detailed observa-
tions in Australia were not published until 20 years after his
death as Children of the Desert [1974].)
Erik Eriksons (1950) work among the Yurok Indians
of California was also based on Freuds psychosexual stages
of child development, although he superimposed on them
his famous psychosocial stages. But there was a wider
and subtler Freudian inuence on many ethnographers,
then and later, who assumed, for example, that breast feed-
ing, weaning, toilet training, and sexual behavior were the
most important events of early experience to investigate
and describe, whether or not they interpreted them in
terms of Freuds concepts of oral, anal, and phallic-genital
stages or xations. Melford E. Spiros (1958) comprehen-
sive and richly detailed account of childhood on an Is-
raeli kibbutz, for example, devotes a chapter to the social-
ization of the oral, anal, and sexual behavior systems
following the concepts and terminology of Whiting and
Irving Child (1953)and another chapter to the Oedipus
complex and sexual identity. Ironically, Whiting (1994:24
25) himself sawthe ndings of their 1953 book, Child Train-
ing and Personality: A Cross-Cultural Study, as casting doubt
on the validity of Freuds psychosexual stages, and in his
own later research and writing they were rarely mentioned.
Years later, Jean Briggs (1972) criticized Eriksons stages of
early childhood from the perspective of her study of Inuit
children.
For many U.S. anthropologists interested in childhood
during the 1930s and 1940sparticipants in the culture
and personality movement initiated by Edward Sapir, Ruth
Benedict, and Margaret Meadit was the neo-Freudian
psychoanalysis (more accurately termed interpersonal) of
Harry Stack Sullivan, Karen Horney, and Erich Fromm that
supplied its conception of childhood experience. In this
conception, there were no libidinal or psychosexual stages,
and the focus was on the quality of the childs interper-
sonal experience with parents and siblings during the early
years. Unlike the orthodox Freudians, the neo-Freudians
were open to the study of cultural variation, and their the-
ory could be related to social relationships as ethnographers
observed them. The pioneering monograph in this vein was
Balinese Childhood: A Photographic Analysis by Gregory Bate-
son and Margaret Mead (1942).
Abram Kardinera psychoanalyst who, after graduate
study withBoas, was trained as a physicianand thenwas an-
alyzed by Freud in 1921formulated another line of theory
and research similar to the neo-Freudians, replacing Freuds
biological drives unfolding in the course of early child de-
velopment with a culturally variable theory of ego develop-
ment. Kardiner organized an interdisciplinary seminar on
culture and personality in New York that ran from 1933
onward, with numerous anthropologists participating. He
also published (with the collaboration of Ralph Linton) two
inuential volumes, The Individual and His Society (1939)
and The Psychological Frontiers of Society (1945), based on
the seminar proceedings. The anthropologist Cora DuBois
(1944) worked with Kardiner in the seminar and then car-
ried out eldwork among the Alorese in Indonesia that in-
cluded ethnographic observations of childhood, based in
part on Kardiners approach.
252 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
In retrospect, these psychiatrically inspired ethnogra-
phies (i.e., Bateson and Mead 1942 and DuBois 1944)
have a judgmental castBalinese mothers torment their
toddlers, Alorese mothers neglect theirsand imply patho-
logical consequences of the culture-specic parental prac-
tices they describe. This was consistent with neo-Freudian
approaches, in which the mental illness of children was
blamed on parents whose practices failed to give their chil-
dren emotional security. The parents point of view on
these practices is not givenprominence inthe ethnographic
accounts. Furthermore, despite the claim of Bateson and
Mead that Balinese culture provides therapeutic ritual dra-
mas for the childhood conicts of adults, these ethnogra-
phies are not portraits of alternative normal or healthy path-
ways for child development. At the time, however, they rep-
resented a new focus on interpersonal relationships in early
childhood, and Bateson and Meads photographs enabled
readers to share a microscopic view of childhood social in-
teraction in sequence.
Jules Henry and Zunia Henrys (1944) study of sibling
rivalry among Pilaga Indian children of the Argentine Gran
Chaco, attempting to replicate a doll play experiment de-
vised by the psychoanalyst David Levy (1937), was also fo-
cused on interpersonal relations in psychiatric perspective.
In this case, however, the ethnographic observers tended to
see the Pilaga parents and children with whomthey worked
as freer of pathogenic sibling rivalryand of remorse and
self-punishment after their sibling conictsthan their
counterparts in the United States. Henry and Henry con-
cluded that sibling rivalry is as inevitable among the Pilaga
as anywhere else, yet the Pilaga childrens lack of remorse
is culturally determined.
Finally, there is the inuence of Edward Sapir (1884
1939), who never did an ethnographic study of childhood
but who proposed research on the childs acquisition of cul-
ture to his students and others who carried out such stud-
ies. Sapir had a long-standing interest in the psychology
of culture and gave a course of lectures on that topic at
the University of Chicago from 1926 to 1931 and at Yale
until 1937. That course, reconstructed as a book from stu-
dents lecture notes and published in 1993 by Judith Irvine,
contained the following passages:
[Perhaps we can say something more about] the personal
world of meanings, [if we consider] the eld of child de-
velopment. As soon as we set ourselves at the vantage-
point of the culture-acquiring child, [with] the personal-
ity denitions and potentials that must never for a mo-
ment be lost sight of, and which are destined from the
very beginning to interpret, evaluate, and modify every
culture pattern, sub-pattern, or assemblage of patterns
that it will ever by inuenced by, everything changes.
Culture is then not something given but something to be
gradually and gropingly discovered.
[If we are to understand the transmission of culture, or
indeed the whole problem of culture from this develop-
mental point of view,] the time must come when the cos-
mos of the child of three will be known and dened, not
merely referred to. The organized intuitive organization
of a three-year-old is far more valid and real than the
most ambitious psychological theory ever constructed.
[Yet our three-year-olds are not all the same.] Our chil-
dren are fully developed personalities very early. [We do
not quite know how this comes about, but it depends
considerably on] the interactions between the child and
his early environment up to the age of three.
Inthe childs cosmos, patterns of behavior are understood
emotionally, [in terms of a particular constellation of re-
lationships].
Study the child minutely and carefully, from birth un-
til, say, the age of ten, with a view to seeing the order
in which culture patterns and parts of patterns appear in
his psychic world; study the relevance of these patterns
for the development of his personality; and at the end of
the suggested period, see how much of the total ofcial
culture of the group can be said to have a signicant exis-
tence for him. Moreover, what degree of systematization,
conscious or unconscious, in the complicating patterns
and symbolisms of culture will have been reached by this
child? [Sapir 1993:197198; square brackets in original]
These excerpts show that Sapir, prior to 1937, was con-
structing andadvocating a viewof childhoodthat, although
inuenced by the interpersonal theory of psychiatry of his
friend Harry Stack Sullivan, was focused on meanings, pat-
terns, and organizationinthe childs experience rather than
questions of pathology, emphasized the subjective experi-
ence rather than the behavioral conformity of the child,
and assumed the child to be an active and denitive de-
cision maker concerning the meanings of culture patterns.
This is a subtle conceptionof cultural acquisitionthat seems
as good a basis for research today as it was 70 years ago.
Did anyone pursue these leads so cogently expressed
by Sapir in his lectures? Not really, at least for a long while.
Many of his graduate students at Yale were linguists with
no apparent interest in childhood, but two who took his
course were John W. M. Whiting and Beatrice Blyth Whit-
ing, who devoted their careers to the anthropological study
of childhood. John Whiting conducted eldwork on the
transmission of culture to children among the Kwoma of
New Guinea in 193637 and wrote a monograph enti-
tled Becoming a Kwoma (1941) that contains much ethno-
graphic data Sapir would have appreciated. In analyzing
the data, however, Whiting followed the lead of John Dol-
lard who, although brought to Yale by Sapir as a kindred
spirit, was soon attracted to the (behavioristic) learning
theory of the Yale psychologist Clark L. Hull. So Whitings
account of the acquisition of culture by Kwoma children,
although representing the descriptive and contextual rich-
ness of a Malinowskian ethnography (Malinowski was at
Yale in 193839 and advised Whiting on the writing of
the book), was interpreted (in the last two chapters) not
in terms of Sapirs concepts but according to the stimulus-
response theory of learning derived from laboratory exper-
iments with animals.
Although not his student, Clyde Kluckhohn was also
inuenced by Sapir and in 1939 published an article on the
acquisition of culture (the culturalization of the child)
in which he made recommendations for data on this pro-
cess to be collected by eld workers. Like Whiting, however,
LeVine Ethnography of Childhood 253
Kluckhohn was concerned with developing an empirical
method for studying how children acquire culture, and his
emphasis is on a quantitative approach, illustrated by the
sample of 48 children in his Navajo research, which formed
the basis for the monograph Children of the People (Leighton
and Kluckhohn 1948). There is nothing of Sapirs focus on
symbolic meanings and the childs subjective perspective in
the article.
It would be the 1980s before linguistic anthropologists
translated Sapirs vision of the cultural study of childhood
intoethnographic research. Yet several ethnographers work-
ing after 1950 focused on the subjective experience of the
child in terms close to those Sapir was advocating: Hildred
Geertz (1959) in Java, William Caudill (1972) in Japan, and
Robert I. Levy (1969, 1973:430469) in Tahiti, for example.
Thus, from 1928 to 1950, professional anthropologists
led by Mead and Malinowski created an anthropology of
childhood that was grounded in ethnographic eldwork
in diverse cultures and critical of developmental formula-
tions in psychology. They experimented with interpretive
frameworks drawn from psychiatry, psychoanalysis, and
academic psychology. They constructed concepts to study
the childs acquisition of culture. The collection of ethno-
graphic data on childhoodin relation to kinship, religion,
the family, the life cycle, and other standard ethnographic
topics, as well as psychological developmentbecame part
of the ethnographers conventional toolkit, as eld studies
were conducted in many parts of the world.
3
THE SIX CULTURES STUDY OF SOCIALIZATION
AND OTHER WHITING PROJECTS
The Six Cultures Study (SCS), launched in 1954 by John
Whiting (by then at Harvard) and two psychologists, Irvin
L. Child at Yale and William Lambert at Cornell, was an
ambitious project in the anthropology of childhood and
deserves a separate article dedicated to its history. From the
perspective of the present overview, it was a unique effort
to harness six ethnographic studies in different parts of the
world to collect comparable data, following a single detailed
eld manual (Whiting et al. 1966), on childhood in social
and cultural context.
The six ethnographic studies of the SCS were conducted
by trained anthropologists between1954 and 1957 ina Mix-
tecan community in Mexico (Romney and Romney 1966);
an Ilongot community in the Phillipines (Nydegger and
Nydegger 1966); a Rajput community in India (Minturn
and Hitchcock 1966); an Okinawan community in Japan
(Maretzki and Maretzki 1966); a Gusii community of Kenya
(LeVine and LeVine 1966); and the small town of West Ac-
ton in eastern Massachusetts (Fischer and Fischer 1966).
Apart fromthe last, all were sedentary agriculturalists; none
were hunter-gatherers or pastoral nomads, and none were
urban. The studies were published in 1963 as a single vol-
ume edited by Beatrice Whiting and then as separate mono-
graphs in 1966.
The six monographs combined the topics covered in a
conventional general ethnography of the timeto estab-
lish the contexts of life for families, parents, and children
with a special focus on the course of childhood that went
beyond description of norms to report the frequency of
specic practices in a sample of 24 families. Thus, the SCS
described the variability of child rearing within a commu-
nity as well as its culturally distinctive ideals and practices.
Because the ethnographers had carried out naturalistic ob-
servations of children between three and 11 years of age,
their accounts were enriched by anecdotes of child behav-
ior from their many hours of observation. Each book con-
stituted a record or reconstruction of the routine practices,
relationships, settings, and activities that made up the en-
vironments of childrenand their interactions with those
environmentsfrombirth to adolescence in a particular lo-
cal community at a particular moment in time. The SCS
ethnographies established a new standard for the detailed
examinationof childhood throughethnographic eldwork.
The original plan of the SCS, however, was not sim-
ply to contribute toward . . . a more useful ethnography in
the study of socialization and enculturation (Whiting et
al. 1966:3) but also to test hypotheses relating child rear-
ing to its causes and consequencesthat is, the socioeco-
nomic and cultural factors that constrain and direct child
rearing practices, on the one hand, and the psychocultural
patterns resulting from childhood experience, on the other
hand. This hypothesis-testing programproved problematic,
largely because the processes posited by the SCS frame-
work of psychoanalytic behaviorism were not adequately
assessed through the data collected. The child interview
and projective test designed to assess the psychodynamic
outcomes of child rearing were difcult to apply in some
of the communities, and the Field Guide provided no al-
ternatives. Furthermore, the SCS used a mother interview
to assess child rearing at the individual level (Minturn and
Lambert 1964), but such retrospective interviewapproaches
to childhood environments were shown to be unreliable
even in the United States (Yarrow et al. 1968). When the
Whitings (1975) published the nal report, they narrowed
its analysis to relationships between the socioeconomic en-
vironments of families and the behavior patterns of chil-
dren assessed through naturalistic observation, from which
valid conclusions could be drawn. So, in the end, the SCS
had little to say about the causal inuence of child rearing
on personality development and cultural expression.
Apart from the monographs, the most enduring (and
largely unanticipated) contribution of the SCS may have
been the introduction of systematic naturalistic observa-
tions of childrenthat is, repeated and aggregated obser-
vations of children in their routine behavior settings as
a method for recording the interactions of children with
their environments in diverse cultures. Beatrice Whiting
salvaged the observational data for analysis and developed
this method further for the comparative study of childhood
in 14 diverse communities she conducted with Carolyn P.
Edwards (Whiting and Edwards 1988) years later.
While the SCS eldwork was being conducted, John
Whiting became interested in male initiation ceremonies
and, with his students, launched a programof cross-cultural
254 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
research on male identity. Its basic hypothesis linked the
conditions under which boys were raised in infancy and
early childhood to rituals ranging from circumcision to the
couvade that socialized them into aggressively masculine
adult roles or permitted them to act out female roles. The
ideas involved were developed in cross-cultural analyses of
published ethnographic data (Burton and Whiting 1961;
Whiting 1964; Whiting et al. 1958) and led to the eldwork
by Robert L. Munroe and Ruth H. Munroe among the Black
Caribs of Belize (Munroe 1980; Munroe et al. 1973, 1981)
and to Beatrice Whitings analysis (1965) of protest mas-
culinity among the six cultures of the SCS. Judith Brown
(1963) extended the comparative analysis to female initia-
tion rituals.
INFANT STUDIES, EARLY SOCIAL INTERACTION, AND
GUIDED PARTICIPATION
The last four decades of the 20th century saw anthrop-
ologistsbiological and linguistic as well as social and
psychologicaland some developmental psychologists
take new directions in ethnographic studies of childhood.
There were no projects approaching the scale of the SCS,
but this was an expansive period in which a greater num-
ber of academic scholars did more kinds of research in a
wider variety of places. (The growth of U.S. universities and
of funding for research in the 1960s facilitated this expan-
sion; so did the extension of jet aircraft service to all parts
of the world.)
Infant research was one of the new directions for
ethnography during this period. Even though it had been
taken for granted by anthropologists of the culture and
personality movement in the 1930s that the childs acquisi-
tion of culture began in infancy (e.g., Linton 1936), hardly
any postWWII eldwork was focused on that period.
By the 1960s, evidence from psychological experiments
was showing that shortly after birth human infants were
capable of perception, cognition, memory, imitation, and
social engagement (Osofsky 1987). This evidence was
often interpreted as revealing hard-wired universals of
development, but it also indicated a much greater and
earlier sensitivity to environmental inuence than had
been previously imagined. And the ethologists and behav-
ioral biologists studying imprinting and other aspects of
parentoffspring relations in nonhuman species provided
models for analyzing how both innate and environmental
factors inuence behavioral development (Blurton-Jones
1972).
Ethnographic observations on infancy and infant care
during this period were carried out by numerous social and
biological anthropologists, mostly insub-SaharanAfrica but
also in Japan and among the Navajo of New Mexico. The
rst studies were those of William Caudill (Caudill 1972;
Caudill and Frost 1974; Caudill and Weinstein1969) among
middle-class families in Japan and the United States and
Melvin J. Konner (1972, 1976, 1977) among the !Kung
San foragers of the Kalahari Desert in Botswana, both of
whom conducted their eldwork in the 1960s. Caudill
and Konner were followed by many others. All focused
their efforts on parental concepts and practices of infant
care, the infants changing social environment, and the
infants communicative and other social interactions, but
they differed in their approaches, methods, and theoretical
frameworks.
4
It is noteworthy that the contributors to this litera-
ture include biological anthropologists (Konner, Chisholm,
and Hewlett), who were often but not always study-
ing hunter-gatherers, and psychologists (Super, Tronick,
Morelli, Greeneld, and Keller), as well as social and cultural
anthropologists. Super and Sara Harkness (1986) formulated
the concept of the developmental niche to integrate di-
verse analytic perspectives on the observable conditions of
infancy. There are rich ethnographic descriptions in some
of these publications, whereas in others ethnographic evi-
dence is used primarily as context to interpret quantitative
observational data from ecological, cultural, developmen-
tal, and evolutionary perspectives.
A related newdirection for ethnographies of childhood
after 1960 concerns the childs social relationships and so-
cial participation. These topics had been covered in earlier
reports from the eld, but the new research involved closer
examination of specic patterns and broader concepts to
make sense of them. Adoption, for example, was studied in
easternOceania (Carroll 1970), and the fostering of children
in West Africa (Goody 1982). Thomas Weisner and Ronald
Gallimore (1977) summarized the evidence on sibling care
giving, suggesting that it was widespread among human
societies (although not in contemporary Western ones) and
had some advantages for infants and their older siblings
who cared for them. Weisner (1984, 1987, 1989) later pub-
lished more on this subject from his eldwork among the
Abaluyia of Kenya and set it in the context of an ecocultural
approach to childhood, drawing on the social developmen-
talism of the Soviet psychologists Lev Vygotsky (1978) and
A. N. Leontev (1981) and requiring ethnography for its im-
plementation. In a parallel move, Barbara Rogoff (Rogoff et
al. 1993), a developmental psychologist who had done eld-
work in a Guatemalan Mayan village over a 30-year period,
coined the term guided participation to refer more accurately
than earlier terms like rearing, training, and socialization to
the way in which children take part from their early years
in the social activities of their environments, with guidance
from those around them. An extensive example of ethno-
graphic description in this mode is provided by Suzanne
Gaskinss (1996, 1999, 2000, and 2003) articles on Yucatec
Mayan children.
Childrens play also received ethnographic attention
during the last quarter of the 20th century, in the stud-
ies by David Lancy (1996; see also Lancy and Tindall 1977),
HelenSchwartzman(1978), and Lawrence Goldman(1998),
as well as in the observations of childrens activities pro-
vided by the comparative study of Whiting and Edwards
(1988), the ethnography of Gaskins (2003), and in many
ethnographies not focused on play.
LeVine Ethnography of Childhood 255
THE ACQUISITION OF LANGUAGE AND CULTURE
Sapirs question of how three-year-old children acquire cul-
ture began to nd answers many years later through the
work of linguistic anthropologists inspired by the sociolin-
guistics movement (Ervin-Tripp and Slobin 1967; Gumperz
and Hymes 1972; Hymes 1974), who began to study the
childs acquisition of communicative competence. Between
the late 1960s and the early 1990s, child language was es-
tablished as a signicant focus for the ethnography of child-
hood.
Early eld studies of child language and language so-
cialization in non-Western societies included those of Keith
Kernan (1969) in Samoa, Benjamin Blount (1971, 1972,
1975) among the Luo of Kenya, and Sara Harkness (Hark-
ness and Super 1977) among the Kipsigis of Kenya. Af-
ter Shirley Brice Heaths (1982, 1983) pioneering work on
childrens communicative development in North Carolina,
Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin launched an extensive
series of publications on language acquisition and lan-
guage socialization in different cultures (Ochs 1988, 1990;
Ochs and Schieffelin 1984; Schieffelin 1990; Schieffelin and
Ochs 1986, 1987). Their edited volume Language Socializa-
tion across Cultures (1987) featured the studies of numer-
ous other linguists and linguistic anthropologists, and their
monographs on, respectively, Samoans (Ochs 1988) and the
Kaluli of New Guinea (Schieffelin 1990) showed the depth
and value of sociolinguistic eldwork on language social-
ization. They used electronic recording to shed light on cul-
tural meanings as children construe them and learn them
through communicative participation. Their ndings indi-
cate not only that children in the second, third, and fourth
years of life acquire fundamental aspects of cultural com-
munication as they learn a rst language (e.g., the conven-
tions of conversing, expressing, and suppressing emotions,
as well as giving social signals) but also that this early learn-
ing provides symbolic elements for the growing sense of
self.
During the 1990s, Peggy Miller and colleagues (Miller
et al. 1996; Miller et al. 1990; Miller et al. 2001; Miller
et al. 1997), particularly Heidi Fung (1999), carried the
sociolinguistic approach to the development of the self
into the three-year-old childs acquisition of cultural nar-
ratives. In analyzing the videotapes of personal story-
telling concerning the transgressions of young children in
the United States and Taiwan, they provided vivid ethno-
graphic evidence of culture-specic maternal communica-
tions and the coconstruction of narrative between mother
and child that contribute to the childs sense of shame,
self-esteem, and other culturally organized aspects of self.
Although it is not possible in this brief article to summa-
rize or illustrate further the ndings from sociolinguistic
research on childhood, it has clearly emerged as an im-
portant naturalistic strategy for understanding the ways
in which communicative interaction during childhood
helps shape both cultural acquisition and psychological
development.
CONCLUSIONS
In the 20th century, an anthropology of childhood was
created, grew, and diversied, with the ethnographic eld
study of children in context as its core empirical approach.
This historical overview has noted some major trends and
problems without conducting an exhaustive, analytic, or
critical review of the literature. Anthropological pioneers
like Malinowski, Mead, and Sapir identied ways in which
childhood was worthy of ethnographic attention, and the
literature grew at an increasing pace from the 1920s to the
present.
Ethnographers conducted eldwork among hunter-
gatherers and agriculturalists and in some urban settings on
virtually every continent. The entire corpus of data is still
fragmentary, but there have been ethnographic proles of
childhood environments in many different societies, and
the past 15 years have seen the publication of an unprece-
dented number of monographs and other books (e.g., Bolin
2006; Briggs 1998; Goldman 1998; Gottlieb 2004; Hewlett
1991, 1992; Hewlett and Lamb 2005; Kramer 2006; Kurtz
1992; Morton 1996; Norman 1991; Riesman 1992; Seymour
1999).
5
In addition, there have been specialized eld studies
of infancy, adoption and fostering, childrens relationships,
play, and language, as mentioned above.
The ethnographic exploration of childhood has taken
many different pathways over the past 75 years. Some early
studies were guided by Freudian or neo-Freudian premises,
but from the beginning ethnographers used the data they
had collected in the eld to criticize developmental formu-
lations in psychology, and the cultural critique of develop-
mental theory became an established genre. Developmen-
tal psychologys changing theoretical orientations, and its
reluctance to take seriously evidence from eld research,
posed obstacles to sustained interdisciplinary engagement
and made the building of cumulative knowledge difcult.
Ethnographers, including biological and linguistic anthro-
pologists, nevertheless constructed approaches to eldwork
on childhood and interpreted their ndings in theoretical
perspectives that were variously ecological, cultural, evolu-
tionary, and sociolinguistic, as well as developmental.
Ethnographers have documented the diversity across
human populations in parenting, both in the norms and
practices that form childhood environments and in the
baseline developmental pathways of children from birth to
adolescence. They have described the ways children learn
through participation in differing social and symbolic en-
vironments and acquire cultural meanings during the early
years. The research conducted to date, however, has just be-
gun to realize the potential of anthropologys contribution
to the understanding of childhood in the human species.
Ethnographic studies of childhood could be improved
in their depth and sophistication and even in their tech-
nology. The time seems ripe for a new eld manual rec-
ommending methods to ethnographers. Mead (Bateson
and Mead 1942; Mead and MacGregor 1951) advocated
the use of visual records to deepen the ethnographic
256 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
understanding of childhood, and although powerful and
portable video recording technology is nowavailable, it has
only rarely been used outside of linguistic studies. Joseph
Tobin and colleagues (1989) showed in Preschool in Three
Cultures that videos of child-care practices in one culture
shown to parents and teachers of another can elicit not
only emotional expressions of shock and disapproval but
also a spontaneous explication of indigenous assumptions
concerning children that had eluded conventional ethno-
graphic interviewing.
Furthermore, despite the global reach of ethnographic
research on childhood, there is hardly any area of the world
in which coverage could be called adequate and many oth-
ers to which ethnography of childhood needs to be ex-
tended. But ethnographic documentation by itself, how-
ever excellent, cannot create an anthropology of child-
hood of more than marginal signicance to anthropolo-
gists (Hirschfeld 2002), or for that matter to other social sci-
entists, without further theory building and cross-cultural
comparison (Quinn 2005; Shweder et al. in press).
ROBERT A. LEVINE Harvard Graduate School of Education,
Cambridge, MA 02138
NOTES
Acknowledgments. This article was written when the author was a
fellow of the John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Foundation.
1. The claim that the topic of childhood has been neglected by
anthropologists (Benthall 1992; Hardman 1973; Hirschfeld 2002;
Schwartzman 2001), without reference to many of the works re-
viewed here, leaves the impression of a small or declining literature
that is inconsistent with the results of this overview.
2. Some of my own interpretations of this literature and more ex-
tensive historical accounts of the anthropology of childhood will
appear in future publications (LeVine in press a, in press b; LeVine
and New in press).
3. The numerous case studies in cultural anthropology, edited by
George Spindler and Louise Spindler and published by Holt, Rine-
hart, and Winston fromthe 1960s to the 1980s, illustrate this point
with their chapters on Growing Up or The Life Cycle, which
contain ethnographic data on childhood from a particular society.
4. This literature cannot be adequately reviewed in this article,
but some of the major ethnographic projects can be listed: James
Chisholm(1983) among the Navajo; Sara Harkness and Charles Su-
per (1983, 1996) among the Kipsigis of Kenya; Ruth H. Munroe and
Robert L. Munroe (1972) among the Logoli of Kenya; Mary Martini
and John Kirkpatrick (1981) among the Marquesans; Greeneld et
al. (1989) among Mayans of Chiapas, Mexico; Robert A. LeVine and
colleagues (1988, 1994) among the Gusii of Kenya; Philip Kilbride
and Janet Kilbride (1990) among the Ganda of Uganda; Edward
Z. Tronick and Gilda Morelli (Tronick et al. 1992; Tronick et al.
1987) among the Efe pygmies of the Congo; Paul Riesman (1992)
among the Fulani of Burkina Faso; Barry Hewlett (1991; Hewlett et
al. 2000; Hewlett et al. 1998) among the Aka pygmies of Cameroun
and the Ngandu of the Central African Republic; Alma Gottlieb
(2004) among the Beng of the Ivory Coast; and Heidi Keller (in
press) among the Nso of Cameroun and three other cultures.
5. A special issue of Ethos (31[2], June 2003) devoted to The Cul-
tural Construction of Childhood is also worth noting here.
REFERENCES CITED
Bateson, Gregory, and Margaret Mead
1942 Balinese Childhood: A Photographic Analysis. New York:
New York Academy of Sciences.
Baxter, Jane Eva
2005 The Archaeology of Childhood: Children, Gender and Ma-
terial Culture. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Benthall, Jonathan
1992 A Late Developer? The Ethnography of Children. Anthro-
pology Today 8:2335.
Blount, Benjamin G.
1971 Socialization and Pre-Linguistic Development among the
Luo of Kenya. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 27:41
50.
1972 Aspects of Luo Socialization. Language in Society 1:236
248.
1975 Studies in Child Language: An Anthropological View.
American Anthropologist 77:580600.
Bluebond-Langner, Myra
1978 The Private Worlds of Dying Children. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
1996 Inthe Shadowof Illness: Parents andSiblings of the Chron-
ically Ill Child. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Blurton-Jones, N.
1972 Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Boas, Franz
1902 Rudolf Virchows Anthropological Work. Science 16(403):
441480.
1912 Instability of Human Types. In Papers on Interracial Prob-
lems Communicated to the First Universal Races Congress Held
at the University of London, July 2629, 1911. Gustav Spiller,
ed. Pp. 99103. Boston: Ginn and Co.
Bolin, Inge
2006 Growing Up ina Culture of Respect: Child Rearing inHigh-
land Peru. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Briggs, Jean
1972 The Issues of Autonomy and Aggression in the Three-year-
old: The Utku Eskimo Case. Seminars in Psychiatry 4:317329.
1998 Inuit Morality Play: The Emotional Education of a Three-
Year-Old. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Brown, Judith K.
1963 A Cross-Cultural Study of Female Initiation Rites. Ameri-
can Anthropologist 65(4):837853.
Bruner, Jerome S.
1990 Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Burton, Roger V., and John W. M. Whiting
1961 The Absent Father and Cross-Sex Identity. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly 7:8595.
Carroll, Vern, ed.
1970 Adoption in Eastern Oceania. Honolulu: University of
Hawaii Press.
Caudill, William
1972 Tiny Dramas: Vocal Communication between Mother and
Infant in Japanese and American Families. In Mental Health
Research in Asia and the Pacic, vol. 2. William Lebra, ed. Pp.
2548. Honolulu: East-West Center Press.
Caudill, William, and Helen Weinstein
1969 Maternal Care and Infant Behavior in Japan and America.
Psychiatry 32:1243.
Caudill, William, and Lois Frost
1974 A Comparison of Maternal Care and Infant Behavior in
Japanese-American, American and Japanese Families. In Men-
tal Health Research in Asia and the Pacic, Vol. 3. William
Lebra, ed. Pp. 415. Honolulu: East-West Center Press.
Childs, Gladwyn Murray
1949 Umbundu Kinship and Character. London: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Chisholm, James
1983 Navajo Infancy. New York: Aldine.
De Laguna, Frederica
1965 Childhood among the Yakutat Tlingit. In Context and
Meaning in Cultural Anthropology. Melford E. Spiro, ed. Pp.
323. New York: Free Press.
Dennis, Wayne
1940 The Hopi Child. New York: Appleton-Century.
DuBois, Cora
1944 The People of Alor. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press.
LeVine Ethnography of Childhood 257
Elwin, Verrier
1947 The Muria and Their Ghotul. Madhya Pradesh: Vanya
Prakashan.
Erikson, Erik
1950 Childhood and Society. New York: W. W. Norton.
Ervin-Tripp, Susan, and Dan Slobin
1967 A Field Manual for Cross-Cultural Study of the Acquisi-
tion of Communicative Competence. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Evans-Pritchard, E. E.
1940 The Nuer. London: Oxford University Press.
1953 Kinship and Marriage among the Nuer. London: Oxford
University Press.
Ewbank, Douglas C., and Samuel H. Preston
1990 Personal Health Behavior and the Decline in Infant and
Child Mortality: The United States, 19001930. In What
We Know about Health Transition: The Cultural, Social
and Behavioural Determinants of Health, vol. 1. John C.
Caldwell, Salley Findley, Pat Caldwell, Gigi Santow, Wendy
Cosford, Jennifer Braid, and Daphne Broers-Freeman, eds.
Pp. 116149. Canberra, Australia: The Australian National
University.
Fajans, Jane
1997 They Make Themselves: Work and Play among the Baining
of Papua New Guinea. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Firth, Raymond
1936 We, the Tikopia. New York: American Book.
Fischer, John L., and Ann Fischer
1966 The New Englanders of Orchard Town, U.S.A. New York:
John Wiley and Sons.
Fortes, Meyer
1938 Social and Psychological Aspects of Education in Taleland.
London: International African Institute Memorandum XVII.
1949 The Web of Kinship among the Tallensi. London: Oxford
University Press.
Fung, Heidi
1999 Becoming a Moral Child: The Socialization of Shame
among Young Chinese Children. Ethos 27(2):180209.
Gaskins, Suzanne
1996 How Mayan Parental Theories Come into Play. In Parents
Cultural Belief Systems. Sara Harkness and Charles Super, eds.
Pp. 345363. New York: Guilford.
1999 Childrens Daily Lives in a Mayan Village. In Childrens
Engagement in the Social World. Artin Goncu, ed. Pp. 2581.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
2000 Childrens Daily Activities in a Mayan Village: A Culturally
Grounded Description. Cross-Cultural Research 34:375389.
2003 FromCornto Cash: Change and Continuity withinMayan
Families. Ethos 31(2):248273.
Geertz, Hildred
1959 The Vocabulary of Emotion: A Study of Javanese Socializa-
tion Processes. Psychiatry 22:225237.
Gibbs, James L., ed.
1965 Peoples of Africa. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Gilligan, Carol F.
1982 In a Different Voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Goldman, Lawrence R.
1998 Childs Play: Myth, Mimesis and Make-Believe. Oxford:
Berg.
Goody, Esther N.
1982 Parenthood and Social Reproduction: Fostering and Occu-
pational Roles in West Africa. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Gottlieb, Alma
2004 Afterlife Is Where We Come From: The Culture of Infancy
in West Africa. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Greeneld, Patricia M.
1966 On Culture and Conservation. In Studies in Cognitive
Growth. Jerome S. Bruner, S. M. Olver, and Patricia M. Green-
elds, eds. Pp. 225256, 327334. New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
2004 Weaving Generations Together: Evolving Creativity in the
Maya of Chiapas. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press.
Greeneld, Patricia M., T. B. Brazelton, and C. P. Childs
1989 From Birth to Maturity in Zinacantan: Ontogenesis in
Cultural Context. In Ethnographic Encounters in Southern
Mesoamerica: Essays in Honor of Evon Zartman Vogt. Victo-
ria Bricker and Gray Gossen, eds. Pp. 177216. Albany: State
University of New York at Albany.
Grindal, Bruce T.
1972 Growing Up in Two Worlds: Education and Transition
among the Sisala of Northern Ghana.
Gumperz, John, and Dell Hymes, eds.
1972 Directions in Sociolinguistics: The Ethnography of Com-
munication. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Hall, G. Stanley
1904 Adolescence: Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiol-
ogy, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Educa-
tion. 2 vols. New York: Appleton.
Hardman, Charlotte
1973 Can There Be an Anthropology of Childhood? Journal of
the Anthropological Society of Oxford 4:8599.
Harkness, Sarah, and Charles Super
1977 Why African Children Are So Hard to Test. In Issues in
Cross-Cultural Research, 285. L. L. Adler, ed. Pp. 326331. New
York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
1983 The Cultural Construction of Child Development: A
Framework for the Socialization of Affect. Ethos 11(4):221
231.
Harkness, Sara, and Charles Super, eds.
1996 Parents Cultural Belief Systems. New York: Guilford Press.
Harwood, Robin, Joan G. Miller, and Nydia Lucca-Irizarry
1995 Culture and Attachment: Perceptions of the Child in Con-
text. New York: Guilford Press.
Heath, Shirley Brice
1982 What No Bedtime Story Means: Narrative Skills at Home
and School. Language in Society 11:4976.
1983 Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communi-
ties and Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Henry, Jules, and Zunia Henry
1944 Doll Play of Pilaga Indian Children. New York: American
Orthopsychiatric Association.
Hewlett, Barry S.
1991 Intimate Fathers: The Nature and Context of Aka Pygmy
Paternal Infant Care. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Hewlett, Barry S., ed.
1992 FatherChild Relations: Cultural and Biosocial Contexts.
New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Hewlett, Barry S., and Michael Lamb, eds.
2004 Hunter-Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmen-
tal, and Cultural Perspectives. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de
Gruyter.
Hewlett, Barry S., Michael E. Lamb, Birgit Leyendecker, and Axel
Scholmerich
2000 Internal Working Models, Trust, and Sharing among For-
agers. Current Anthropology 41:287297.
Hewlett, Barry S., Michael E. Lamb, Donald Shannon, Birgit Lyen-
decker, and Axel Scholmerich
1998 Culture and Early Infancy among Central African
Foragers and Farmers. Developmental Psychology 34:653
661.
Hirschfeld, Lawrence A.
2002 Why Dont Anthropologists Like Children? American An-
thropologist 104(2):611627.
Hogbin, H. Ian
1931 Education at Ontong Java, Solomon Islands. American An-
thropologist 33(4):601614.
Howard, Alan
1970 Learning To Be Rotuman: Enculturation in the South Pa-
cic. New York: Teachers College Press.
Hughes, Charles C.
1974 Eskimo Boyhood: An Autobiography in Psychosocial Per-
spective. Lexington: University of Kentucky Press.
Hymes, Dell
1974 Foundations in Sociolinguistics: An Ethnographic Ap-
proach. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Junod, Henri
1927[1912] Life in a South African Tribe. 2nd edition. London:
Macmillan.
Kaberry, Phyllis
1939 The Aboriginal Woman. London: G. Routledge and Sons.
258 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
Kardiner, Abram
1939 The Individual and His Society. New York: Columbia Uni-
versity Press.
1945 Psychological Frontiers of Society. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Kaufmann, Felix
1944 Methodology of the Social Sciences. London: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Keller, Heidi
In press Cultures of Infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kernan, Keith T.
1969 The Acquisition of Language by Samoan Children. Berke-
ley: University of California Language-Behavior Research Lab-
oratory.
Kilbride, Phillip L., and Janet C. Kilbride
1990 Changing Family Life in East Africa: Women and Children
at Risk. University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press.
Kluckhohn, Clyde
1939 Theoretical Bases for an Empirical Method of Studying the
Acquisition of Culture by Individuals. Man 39:98103.
Kohlberg, Lawrence A.
1984 The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and
Validity of Moral Stages. San Francisco: Harper and Row.
Konner, Melvin J.
1972 Aspects of the Developmental Ethology of a Foraging Peo-
ple. In Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. N. Blurton-
Jones, ed. Pp. 285304. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
1976 Maternal Care, Infant Behavior, and Development among
the !Kung. In Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers: Studies of the !Kung
San and Their Neighbors. Richard B. Lee and Irven DeVore, eds.
Pp. 218245. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
1977 Infancy among the Kalahari Desert San. In Culture and
Infancy: Variations in the Human Experience. P. Herbert Lei-
derman, Steven Tulkin, and Ann Rosenfeld, eds. Pp. 287328.
New York: Academic Press.
Korbin, Jill, ed.
1981 Child Abuse in Cross-Cultural Perspective. Berkeley: Uni-
versity of California Press.
Kramer, Karen L.
2006 Maya Children: Helpers at the Farm. Cambridge, MA: Har-
vard University Press.
Kurtz, Stanley
1992 All the Mothers Are One: Hindu India and the Cultural
Reshaping of Psychoanalysis. New York: Columbia University
Press.
Lancy, David F.
1996 Playing on the Mother-Ground: Cultural Routines for
Childrens Development. New York: Guilford.
Lancy, David F., and B. Allan Tindall, eds.
1977 The Anthropological Study of Play. West Point, NY: Leisure
Press.
Lantis, Margaret
1960 Eskimo Childhood and Interpersonal Relationships: Nuni-
vak Biographies and Genealogies. Seattle: University of Wash-
ington Press.
Lebra, Takie Sugiyama
1976 Japanese Patterns of Behavior. Honolulu: University of
Hawaii Press.
1990 Socialization of Aristocratic Children by Commoners. Cul-
tural Anthropology 5:78100.
1994 Mother and Child in Japanese Socialization: A JapanU.S.
Comparison. In Cross-Cultural Roots of Minority Child Devel-
opment. Patricia M. Greeneld and R. R. Cocking, eds. Pp. 259
274. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Leighton, Dorothea, and Clyde Kluckhohn
1948 Children of the People: The Navaho Individual and His
Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Leis, Philip E.
1972 Enculturation and Socialization in an Ijaw Village. New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Leontev, A. N.
1981 The Problem of Activity in Psychology. In The Concept of
Activity in Soviet Psychology. James V. Wertsch, ed. Pp. 3771.
Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
LeVine, Robert A.
In press a Anthropological Foundations of Cultural Psychology.
In Handbook of Cultural Psychology. Shinobu Kitayama and
Dov Cohen, eds. New York: Guilford.
In press b Plasticity and Variation: Cultural Inuences on Par-
enting and Child Development within and between Human
Populations. Pacic Palisades, CA: Foundation for Psychocul-
tural Research.
LeVine, Robert A., and Barbara LeVine
1966 Nyansongo: AGusii Community inKenya. NewYork: John
Wiley and Sons.
LeVine, Robert A., and Karin Norman
2001 The Infants Acquisitionof Culture: InfantMother Attach-
ment Re-Examined from an Anthropological Point of View. In
The Psychology of Cultural Experience. Carmella Moore and
Holly Mathews, eds. Pp. 83104. New York: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
LeVine, Robert A., and Rebecca S. New, eds.
In press Anthropology and Child Development: A Cross-
Cultural Reader. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
LeVine, Robert A., Patrice M. Miller, and Mary Maxwell West, eds.
1988 Parenting in Diverse Cultures. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
LeVine, Robert A., Suzanne Dixon, Sarah LeVine, Amy Richman,
Constance H. Keefer, P. Herbert Leiderman, and T. Berry Brazel-
ton
1994 Child Care and Culture: Lessons from Africa. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Levy, David M.
1937 Sibling Rivalry. Research Monograph No. 2. American Or-
thopsychiatric Association.
Levy, Robert I.
1969 Child Management Structure in Tahitian Families. Journal
of the Polynesian Society 78:3543.
1973 Tahitians. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Linton, Ralph
1936 The Study of Man. New York: D. Appleton-Century Co.
Malinowski, Bronislaw
1927 Sex and Repression in Savage Society. London: Paul,
Trench, Trubner.
1929 The Sexual Life of Savages in North Western Melanesia.
London: Routledge.
Maretzki, Thomas, and Hatsumi Maretzki
1966 Taira: An Okinawan Village. New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
Martini, Mary, and John Kirkpatrick
1981 Early Interactions in the Marquesas Islands. In Culture and
Early Interactions. Tiffany Field, Anita Sostek, Peter Vietze, and
P. Herbert Leiderman, eds. Pp. 189213. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mead, Margaret
1928a Coming of Age in Samoa. New York: William Morrow.
1928b Samoan Children at Work and Play. Natural History
28:626636.
1930 Growing Up in New Guinea. New York: William Morrow.
1931 The Primitive Child. In A Handbook of Child Psychology.
C. Murchison, ed. Pp. 669686. Worcester, MA: Clark Univer-
sity Press.
1932 An Investigation of the Thought of Primitive Children,
with Special Reference to Animism. Journal of the Royal An-
thropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 62:173
190.
Mead, Margaret, and Frances Cooke MacGregor
1951 Growth and Culture: A Photographic Study of Balinese
Childhood. New York: G. P. Putnams Sons.
Mead, Margaret, and Martha Wolfenstein, eds.
1954 Childhood in Contemporary Cultures. Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Miller, Joan G.
1994 Cultural Diversity in the Morality of Caring. Cross-
Cultural Research 28:339.
Miller, Peggy J., Angela R. Wiley, Heidi Fung, and Chung-Hui Liang
1997 Personal Storytelling as a Medium of Socialization in
Chinese and American Families. Child Development 68:557
568.
Miller, Peggy J., Heidi Fung, and Julie Mintz
1996 Self-Construction through Narrative Practices: A Chinese
LeVine Ethnography of Childhood 259
and American Comparison of Early Socialization. Ethos
24(2):237280.
Miller, Peggy J., R. Potts, Heidi Fung, Lisa Hoogstra, and Julie Mintz
1990 Narrative Practices and the Social Construction of Self in
Childhood. American Ethnologist 17(2):292311.
Miller, Peggy J., T. Sandel, Chung-Hui Liang, and Heidi Fung
2001 Narrating Transgressions in Longwood: The Discourses,
Meanings and Paradoxes of an American Socializing Practice.
Ethos 29(2):159186.
Minturn, Leigh, and John Hitchcock
1966 The Rajputs of Khalapur, India. New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
Minturn, Leigh, and William W. Lambert
1964 Mothers of Six Cultures. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Montgomery, Heather
2001 Modern Babylon? Prostituting Children in Thailand. New
York: Bergahn Books.
Montgomery, Heather, Rachel Burr, and Martin Woodhead, eds.
2003 Changing Childhoods: Global and Local. New York: John
Wiley and Sons.
Morton, Helen
1996 Becoming Tongan: An Ethnography of Childhood. Hon-
olulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Munroe, Robert L.
1980 Male Transvestism and the Couvade: A Psychocultural
Analysis. Ethos 8(1):4959.
Munroe, Robert L., R. H. Munroe, and J. W. M. Whiting
1973 The Couvade: A Psychological Analysis. Ethos 1(1):30
74.
1981 Male Sex-Role Resolutions. In Handbook of Cross-Cultural
Human Development. R. H. Munroe, R. L. Munroe, and B. B.
Whiting, eds. Pp. 611632. New York: Garland.
Munroe, Ruth H., and R. L. Munroe
1972 Household Density and Infant Care in an East African So-
ciety. Journal of Social Psychology 83:313.
Norman, Karin
1991 A Sound Family Makes a Sound State: Ideology and Up-
bringing ina GermanVillage. Stockholm: Department of Social
Anthropology, University of Stockholm.
Nydegger, William, and Corinne Nydegger
1966 Tarong: An Ilocos Barrio in the Phillipines. NewYork: John
Wiley and Sons.
Ochs, Elinor
1988 Culture and Language Development: Language Acquisi-
tionandLanguage Socializationina SamoanVillage. NewYork:
Cambridge University Press.
1990 Indexicality and Socialization. In Cultural Psychology:
Essays on Comparative Human Development. James Stigler,
Richard A. Shweder, and Gilbert Herdt, eds. Pp. 287308. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ochs, Elinor, and Bambi Schieffelin
1984 Language Acquisition and Socialization: Three Stories. In
Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. Richard
A. Shweder and Robert A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 276320. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Osofsky, Joy, ed.
1987 Handbook of Infant Development. 2nd ed. New York:
Wiley.
Peak, Lois
1991 Learning To Go to School in Japan. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Peshkin, Alan
1972 Kanuri Schoolchildren. NewYork: Holt, Rinehart and Win-
ston.
Preston, Samuel H., and Michael R. Haines
1991 The Fatal Years: Child Mortality in Late Nineteenth-
Century America. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Quinn, Naomi
2005 Universals of Child Rearing. Anthropological Theory
5:475514.
Raum, O. F.
1940 Chaga Childhood. London: Oxford University Press.
Read, Margaret
1960 Children of Their Fathers: Growing Up among the Ngoni
of Nyasaland. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
1968 Children of Their Fathers: Growing Up among the Ngoni
of Malawi. Case Studies in Education and Culture. New York:
Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Richards, Audry I.
1932 Hunger and Work in a Savage Tribe. London: George Rout-
ledge and Sons.
1956 Chisungu: A Girls Initiation Ceremony among the Bemba.
London: Faber and Faber.
Riesman, Paul
1992 First Find Your Child a Good Mother. New Brunswick, NJ:
Rutgers University Press.
Ritchie, Jane
1957 Childhood in Rakau: The First Five Years of Life. Welling-
ton: Victoria University.
Roheim, Geza
1932 The Psychoanalysis of Primitive Cultural Types. Interna-
tional Journal of Psychoanalysis 13:1233.
1974 Children of the Desert: The Western Tribes of Central Aus-
tralia. New York: Harper and Row.
Rogoff, Barbara
2003 The Cultural Nature of Human Development. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Rogoff, Barbara, Jayanthi Mistry, Artin Goncu, and Christine
Mosier
1993 Guided Participation in Cultural Activity by Toddlers and
Caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development 58(236).
Romney, A. Kimball, and Romaine Romney
1966 The Mixtecans of Juxtlahuaca, Mexico. New York: John
Wiley and Sons.
Sapir, Edward
1993 The Psychology of Culture: A Course of Lectures. Judith
Irvine, ed. The Hague: Mouton.
Schieffelin, Bambi
1990 The Give andTake of Everyday Life: Language Socialization
of Kaluli Children. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schieffelin, Bambi, and Elinor Ochs
1986 Language Socialization. In Annual Review of Anthropol-
ogy 15:163191.
Schieffelin, Bambi, and Elinor Ochs, eds.
1987 Language Socialization across Cultures. New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Schwalb, David W., and Barbara J. Schwalb
1996 Japanese Childrearing: Two Generations of Scholarship.
New York: Guilford Press.
Schwartzman, Helen
1978 Transformations: The Anthropology of Childrens Play.
New York: Plenum Press.
2001 Children and Anthropology: A Century of Studies. In
Children and Anthropology: Perspectives for the 21st Century.
Helen Schwartzman, ed. Pp. 1537. Westport, CT: Bergin and
Garvey.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy
1992 Death without Weeping: The Violence of Everyday Life in
Brazil. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, ed.
1987 Child Survival: Anthropological Perspectives on the
Treatment and Maltreatment of Children. Dordrecht, the
Netherlands: D. Reidel.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, and Carolyn F. Sargent, eds.
1998 Small Wars: The Cultural Politics of Childhood. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Seymour, Susan
1999 Women, Family and Child Care in India. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Shonkoff, Jack, and Deborah Phillips
2000 From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of
Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National
Academies Press.
Shweder, Richard A., Jacqueline Goodnow, Giyoo Hatano, Robert
A. LeVine, Hazel Markus, and Peggy Miller
In press The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind,
Many Mentalities. In Handbook of Child Psychology. 6th
edition. William Damon and Richard M. Lerner, eds. New
York: John Wiley and Sons.
260 American Anthropologist Vol. 109, No. 2 June 2007
Shweder, Richard A., and Robert A. LeVine
1975 Dream Concepts of Hausa Children: A Critique of the
Doctrine of Invariant Sequence in Cognitive Development.
Ethos 3(2):209230.
Shweder, Richard A., M. Mahapatra, and Joan G. Miller
1990 Culture and Moral Development. In Cultural Psychology:
Essays on Comparative Human Development. James Stigler,
Richard A. Shweder, and Gilbert Herdt, eds. Pp. 130204. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Smith, Mary F.
1954 Baba of Karo: A Woman of the Muslim Hausa. London:
Faber and Faber.
Spiro, Melford E.
1958 Children of the Kibbutz: A Study in Child Training and
Personality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Stafford, Charles
1995 The Roads of Chinese Childhood: Learning and Identi-
cation in Angang. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stephens, Sharon, ed.
1995 Children and the Politics of Culture. Princeton: Princeton
University Press.
Stocking, George S.
1968 Race, Culture, and Evolution: Essays in the History of
Anthropology. New York: Free Press.
1974 The Shaping of American Anthropology, 18831911: A
Franz Boas Reader. New York: Basic Books.
1992 Polarity and Plurality: Franz Boas as Psychological Anthro-
pologist. In New Directions in Psychological Anthropology.
Theodore Schwartz, Geoffrey M. White, and Catherine A.
Lutz, eds. Pp. 311323. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Suarez-Orozco, Carola, and Marcelo Suarez-Orozco
2001 Children of Immigration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Super, Charles M., and Sara Harkness
1985 Infants Niche in Rural Kenya and Metropolitan America.
In Cross-Cultural Research at Issue. L. L. Adler, ed. Pp. 247255.
New York: Academic Press.
1986 The Developmental Niche: A Conceptualization at the
Interface of Society and the Individual. International Journal
of Behavioral Development 9:545570.
Tobin, Joseph, David Wu, and Dana Davidson
1989 Preschool in Three Cultures. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Toren, Christina
1990 Making Sense of Hierarchy: Cognition and Social Process
in Fiji. LSE Monograph in Anthropology 61. London: Athlone
Press.
1993 Making History: The Signicance of Childhood Cogni-
tion for a Comparative Anthropology of Mind. Man (n.s.)
28:461478.
Tronick, Edward Z., Gilda Morelli, and P. K. Ivey
1992 The Efe Infant and Toddlers Pattern of Social Relation-
ships: Multiple and Simultaneous. Developmental Psychology
28:568577.
Tronick, Edward Z., Gilda A. Morelli, and Steve Winn
1987 Multiple Caretaking of Efe (Pygmy) Infants. American
Anthropologist 89(1):96106.
UN Population Division
2005 World Population Monitoring 2003: Population, Educa-
tion and Development. New York: UN Economic and Social
Council.
van Gennep, Arnold
1960[1908] The Rites of Passage. Michaela Vizedom and Mari
Caffee, trans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Vygotsky, Lev
1978 Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psy-
chological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Wedgwood, C. H.
1938 The Life of Children in Manam. Oceania 9:129.
Weisner, Thomas S.
1984 Ecocultural Niches of Middle Childhood: A Cross-Cultural
Perspective. In Development during Middle Childhood: The
Years from Six to Twelve. W. A. Collins, ed. Pp. 335369.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
1987 Socialization for Parenthood in Sibling Caretaking Soci-
eties. In Parenting across the Lifespan. Jane Lancaster, Alice
Rossi, Jeanne Altmann, and Lonnie Sherrod, eds. Pp. 237270.
New York: Aldine.
1989 Cultural and Universal Aspects of Social Support for Chil-
dren: Evidence among the Abaluyia of Kenya. In Childrens
Social Networks and Social Supports. Deborah Belle, ed. Pp.
7090. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Weisner, Thomas S., and Ronald Gallimore
1977 My Brothers Keeper: Child and Sibling Caretaking.
Current Anthropology 18:169190.
Whiting, Beatrice B.
1965 Sex Identity Conict and Physical Violence: A Compara-
tive Study. American Anthropologist 67:123140.
Whiting, Beatrice B., ed.
1963 Six Cultures: Studies of Child Rearing. New York: John
Wiley and Sons.
Whiting, Beatrice B., and Carolyn Edwards
1988 Children of Different Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Whiting, Beatrice B., and John W. M. Whiting
1975 Children of Six Cultures: A Psycho-Cultural Analysis.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Whiting, John W. M.
1941 Becoming a Kwoma. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
1954 The Cross-Cultural Method. In The Handbook of Social
Psychology. Gardner Lindzey, ed. Pp. 524531. Cambridge,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
1964 Effects of Climate on Certain Cultural Practices. In Explo-
rations in Cultural Anthropology. Ward Goodenough, ed. Pp.
611632. New York: McGraw-Hill.
1994 Fifty Years as a Behavioral Scientist: Autobiographical
Notes. In Culture and Human Development: The Selected
Papers of John Whiting. Eleanor Hollenberg Chasdi, ed. Pp.
1444. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Whiting, John W. M., and Irvin L. Child
1953 Child Training and Personality: A Cross-Cultural Study.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Whiting, John W. M., Irvin L. Child, and William W. Lambert
1966 Field Guide for a Comparative Study of Socialization. New
York: John Wiley and Sons.
Whiting, John W. M., R. Kluckhohn, and A. Anthony
1958 The Function of Male Initiation Ceremonies at Puberty.
In Readings in Social Psychology. 3rd edition. Eleanor E.
Maccoby, T. Newcomb, and E. Hartley, eds. Pp. 359370. New
York: Henry Holt.
Williams, Thomas R.
1969 A Borneo Childhood: Enculturation in Dusun Society.
New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Yarrow, Marian R., John D. Campbell, and Roger V. Burton
1968 Child Rearing: An Inquiry into Research and Methods.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

También podría gustarte