Está en la página 1de 4

Journal of Research in Personality 49 (2014) 21–24

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Research in Personality


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp

Brief Report

When to cooperate and when to compete: Emotional intelligence


in interpersonal decision-making
Pablo Fernández-Berrocal a,⇑, Natalio Extremera a, Paulo N. Lopes b, Desireé Ruiz-Aranda a
a
University of Málaga, Campus de Teatinos, s/n, 29071 Malaga, Spain
b
Catholic University of Portugal, Travessa Palma, 1649-023 Lisboa, Portugal

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study examined the relationship between emotional intelligence (EI), assessed with an ability test,
Available online 29 December 2013 and interpersonal decision-making using the Prisoner’s Dilemma Game (PDG). Previous research found
that individuals who self-report high EI tend to cooperate more than others in the Prisoner’s Dilemma.
Keywords: We relativize these findings by showing that individuals scoring high on an ability measure of EI choose
Emotional intelligence effective strategies to deal with three different PDG conditions during real interactions. This suggests that
Social interaction emotionally intelligent individuals are not rigidly predisposed to cooperate regardless of others’ behavior.
Social dilemmas
Instead, EI is associated with the capacity to respond flexibly to others’ strategies and to the interaction
Prisoner’s dilemma
Cooperative goals
context in order to maximize long-term gains – even when this means competing rather than
cooperating.
Ó 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction as generalized social phobia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia, who


score lower on the MSCEIT, show poorer interpersonal and social
Emotional intelligence (EI) theory proposes that the abilities to functioning than control group with higher MSCEIT scores (Aguir-
perceive, understand, use and manage emotions in oneself and re, Sergi, & Levy, 2008; Jacobs et al., 2008; Kee et al., 2009).
others are inter-related and constitute a distinct form of intelli- On the basis of existing theory and research, people with higher
gence (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). According to this theory, emotion- EI would be expected to anticipate others’ actions and therefore
ally intelligent individuals use these skills to navigate the social make better interpersonal decisions in the course of social interac-
world and manage interpersonal interaction and social dilemmas tions. This hypothesis can be tested using the Prisoner’s Dilemma
effectively. EI plays a role in the establishment and maintenance Game (PDG; Axelrod, 1984), one of the most widely used para-
of interpersonal relationships (Mayer, Roberts, & Barsade, 2008). digms for simulating the course of social decision-making. Of the
Previous research has found that individuals scoring highly on abil- different strategies analyzed in the literature on PDG (e.g., always
ity measures of EI (Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence cooperate, always defect, tit-for-tat), the most effective for achieving
Test, MSCEIT) tend to have more positive and less conflictive rela- cooperation based on reciprocity in the medium and long term is
tionships with others in a range of naturalistic interpersonal con- tit-for-tat (Axelrod, 1984). Studies using the PDG have shown that
texts (e.g., Brackett, Warner, & Bosco, 2005; Lopes, Salovey, & emotional states and individual differences related to emotional
Straus, 2003; Lopes et al., 2004). EI predicts emotional and social traits influence cooperation. For example, Nelissen, Dijker, and
competencies not only in a context of consolidated social relations, De Vries (2007) reported that fear reduces, while guilt increases,
but also in initial encounters with strangers (Brackett, Rivers, Shiff- cooperation in PDG; Hirsh and Peterson (2009) showed that higher
man, Lerner, & Salovey, 2006). Recent studies using neuroimaging scores on the withdrawal facet of neuroticism and the enthusiasm
techniques provide further evidence that persons with higher facet of extraversion independently predict greater likelihood of
MSCEIT scores solve social problems more quickly and accurately cooperation.
than their counterparts, while revealing less brain activity, even But how do people with high EI behave? Schutte et al. (2001;
after controlling for differences in performance in solving non- Study 4) found that participants scoring higher on a self-report
social problems (Reis et al., 2007). Studies with clinical samples measure of EI cooperated more in PDG. This raises a concern that
lead to similar conclusions: patients with psychopathologies such high-EI individuals might be inclined to cooperate in PDG even
when they should adopt a different strategy to defend their inter-
ests. Evolutionary game theory (Axelrod, 1984) has shown that cer-
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: University of Málaga, Campus de Teatinos, s/n, tain strategies, such as always cooperate, are not evolutionarily
29071 Malaga, Spain. Fax: +34 95 213 26 31.
stable, because in certain highly competitive contexts they do
E-mail address: berrocal@uma.es (P. Fernández-Berrocal).

0092-6566/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.12.005
22 P. Fernández-Berrocal et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 49 (2014) 21–24

not allow individuals to confront others who use the always defect be explained by other intelligence constructs. This control is espe-
strategy. In order to examine whether high EI would lead individ- cially important because previous research has shown that persons
uals to cooperate regardless of the other player’s behavior, or in- with higher cognitive ability tend to be more cooperative in both
stead allow them to respond differently to helpers and cheats, experimental and real-world Prisoner’s Dilemmas (Jones, 2008).
we decided to analyze the strategies used by emotionally intelli- The D-48 Intelligence Test reveals adequate internal consistency
gent individuals in different PDGs. We carried out our study using (.89) and test–retest reliability (.69), and no ethnicity or gender
an ability-based measure of EI (MSCEIT) in order to avoid reliance bias (Domino & Morales, 2000). In the present study, the mean
on self-report measures, which can introduce error due to limited score was 28.28 (SD = 5.33).
or inaccurate self-knowledge, self-enhancement bias and other Tendency to cooperate versus compete was measured one month
distortions (Mayer et al., 2008; Schlegel, Grandjean, & Scherer, before the real PDG by describing to participants the classic Pris-
2013). oner’s Dilemma task for two players and showing the payoff matrix
We investigated the relationship between participants’ EI and (Axelrod, 1984; see Appendix). Participants were asked ‘‘Imagine
their behavior in a PDG in face-to-face interaction with a confeder- you are playing; what would be your first move: cooperate or com-
ate. The confederate used a consistent and predetermined interac- pete?’’ (Komorita, Hilty, & Parks, 1991). Responses were coded 0
tion strategy (always cooperate, always defect or tit-for-tat), about for cooperate (61.2% of respondents) and 1 for compete. We in-
which participants were not forewarned. Statistical analyses cluded tendency to cooperate versus compete as a control variable
controlled for cognitive intelligence and tendency to cooperate. in our analyses, because previous research in social dilemmas has
Our hypothesis was that people with high EI would adapt their found that people with a tendency to cooperate attempt to maxi-
response to the other person’s behavior (always cooperate, always mize the joint outcome for both players (Fehr & Fischbacher,
defect or tit-for-tat), thereby maximizing gains across conditions. 2003; Nelissen et al., 2007).
Prisoner’s Dilemma Game. We used a two-player version of the
PDG (Axelrod, 1984). In any one round of this game, a participant
2. Method
scores 5 points in s/he competes and the opponent cooperates, 3
points if both cooperate, 1 point if both compete, and 0 points if
2.1. Participants
s/he cooperates and the opponent competes (see Appendix). Partic-
ipants were randomly assigned to three experimental conditions
The sample consisted of 232 university students (42 male and
(dilemma type): always cooperate, always defect or tit-for-tat. In
190 female) with a mean age of 22 years (SD = 4.19), who partici-
the always cooperate condition, the confederate always cooperated,
pated in the study in return for course credit.
regardless of the participant’s behavior, so that his/her first move
was to cooperate. In the always defect condition, the confederate al-
2.2. Procedures and measures ways defected, regardless of the participant’s behavior, so that his/
her first move was to compete. In the tit-for-tat condition, the con-
Data were collected in two phases. In the first phase, partici- federate used the tit -for- tat strategy, and his/her first move was to
pants completed measures of EI, cognitive intelligence, tendency cooperate.
to cooperate and socio-demographic information – during normal Before the PDG began, participants were provided basic instruc-
lecture hours and in the presence of a researcher. In the second tions and told that the aim of the game was to obtain as many
phase, one month later, individual participants played a PDG with points as possible. For each round, the players (participant and
a confederate in a laboratory room. confederate) presented their choices simultaneously. Participants
Emotional intelligence was measured using the Mayer, Salovey, did not know how many rounds they would play, and were told
Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT v.2.0; Mayer, Salovey, that the game would be ended at random. In reality, the game
& Caruso, 2002). This ability- and knowledge-based test of 141 was always stopped after 20 rounds. The outcome of interest was
items assesses the four dimensions of EI proposed by Mayer and the total number of points obtained by each participant in the
Salovey (1997): identifying, using, understanding and managing PDG. However, the range of possible scores varied across condi-
emotions. Tasks range from identifying emotion in human faces tions. If the confederate always competed, the minimum score a
to identifying strategies for managing emotions in social situations. participant could obtain was 0 and the maximum 20. In the always
The four dimensions are thought to contribute jointly to the quality cooperate condition, the minimum was 60 and the maximum 100.
of interpersonal interaction and effective decision-making and In order to obtain a meaningful common metric to analyze the ef-
therefore can be subsumed into a general factor of EI (Mayer fect of EI across conditions, we standardized PDG scores within
et al., 2002). Thus, for the sake of parsimony, we report results condition (M = .09; SD = 1.07).
based on total scores rather than sub-dimensions. Mayer et al.
(2002) reported good reliability values for total MSCEIT scores,
including internal consistency (split-half reliability, .91) and test– 3. Results
retest reliability (.86). We used scores based on consensus norms,
which map closely onto scores based on expert norms. In our study Intercorrelations between key variables, reported in Table 1,
the split-half reliability was .73, with M = 98.77 (SD = 15.55) for to- indicate that high-EI individuals tended to score higher on the
tal MSCEIT score. For additional information on the psychometric PDG than their low-EI counterparts. Women scored slightly higher
properties of the MSCEIT, see Mayer et al. (2002) and Mayer, Salo- than men on the MSCEIT. Other correlations were not significant.
vey, Caruso, and Sitarenios (2003). To test our hypothesis, we used a General Linear Model (GLM)
Cognitive intelligence. The D-48 Intelligence Test was used to as- approach for analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), focusing on the
sess fluid intelligence (Gough & Domino, 1963). This general intel- effect of EI, after controlling for cognitive intelligence, tendency
ligence test evaluates the capacity to conceptualize and apply to cooperate, sex, and age, as well as experimental condition.
systematic reasoning to new problems. It contains 48 sets of dom- Although ANCOVA showed a significant main effect of condition
inoes displayed in logical series. Each set includes a domino with taking into account these covariates (F(2, 222) = 10.77, p < .001,
one blank face that respondents must fill in. We included cognitive g2 = .09), we did not examine this effect further because overall
intelligence as a control variable in our analyses to show that the mean differences across conditions had already been removed by
effects observed are specific to emotional intelligence and cannot standardizing the DV within condition. In support of our
P. Fernández-Berrocal et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 49 (2014) 21–24 23

Table 1
Correlations between measured variables.

Variable Correlations
1 2 3 4 5
1. Gender
2. Age .10
3. Emotional intelligence .15* .03
4. Cognitive intelligence .07 .00 .10
5. Tendency to cooperate .06 .01 .03 .06
6. PDG score .04 .03 .19** .09 .04

Note. N = 232. Gender was coded 0 for male and 1 for female. Tendency to cooperate
was coded 0 for cooperate and 1 for compete. Emotional intelligence = Mayer–
Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test; cognitive intelligence = D-48 Intelli-
gence Test; PDG score = score on the Prisoner’s Dilemma Game (standardized
within experimental condition to yield a common metric).
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

Fig. 1. Interaction of dilemma type (AC, always cooperate; AD, always defect; TFT, tit-
for-tat) and emotional intelligence in predicting PDG scores.
hypothesis, we found a significant main effect of EI, indicating that
EI was positively related to PDG scores across conditions:
F(1, 222) = 5.75, p < .05, g2 = .02. No main effects were found for that EI, assessed through an ability- and knowledge-based test
cognitive intelligence, tendency to cooperate, sex or age. rather than by self-report, does not rigidly predispose people to-
An interaction effect was found for dilemma type and EI: ward cooperation regardless of others’ behavior (Côté, DeCelles,
F(2, 222) = 10.17, p < .001, g2 = .08. To examine this interaction fur- McCarthy, Van Kleef, & Hideg, 2011; Hein & Singer, 2008). Instead,
ther and identify the range of values for which the moderator has a EI appears to be associated with the capacity to flexibly adapt to
significant effect on the predictor, we applied the Johnson–Ney- the other’s strategy and to the interaction context in order to attain
man technique, following the procedure of Hayes and Matthes one’s goals (Tamir & Ford, 2012). This means competing when nec-
(2009). In the always defect condition, participants obtained higher essary to establish or improve one’s position.
PDG scores if they had EI scores above 111 (b = 0.20, p < .05), and Why are people with low EI reluctant to compete when the
lower PDG scores if they had EI scores below 95 (b = 0.18, other person systematically refuses to cooperate (see Fig. 1)? They
p < .05; Fig. 1).1 may find it difficult to identify and interpret their feelings in emo-
tionally challenging situations; to use their feelings in order to
guide effective decision making; and to manage their own and oth-
4. Discussion ers’ emotions. Dealing with someone who systematically refuses to
collaborate is likely to arouse negative emotions such as anger, fear
A significant main effect of emotional intelligence indicates or sadness (Tamir & Ford, 2012). An interesting avenue for future
that, overall, high-EI individuals did better on the PDG than their research would be to examine participants’ emotional states while
low-EI counterparts, after controlling for cognitive intelligence they play the game, as a possible mediator of the relationship be-
and tendency to cooperate. Moderation analysis indicates that this tween EI and PDG scores.
performance advantage occurred only in the condition where the Why are people with high EI not more likely to take advantage
confederate always competed. In this setting, PDG scores corre- of someone who is consistently cooperative? Perhaps they view
spond exactly to the number of times that a participant chose to building a relationship and preserving their reputation as a fair
compete (because participants earned 1 point if they competed negotiator as more important – a view that is likely to enhance
and 0 if they cooperated in this game). Thus, our results show that, benefits over the long term, at the expense of short-term gains.
in a highly competitive context, emotionally intelligent individuals Once again, future research could examine whether such goals ex-
were more willing to compete in order to attain their goals and ad- plain the relationship between EI and performance in dealing with
vance their interests than their low-EI counterparts. In other a cooperative partner. This admittedly speculative interpretation
words, high-EI individuals were less likely to be abused by does not preclude the possibility that individuals who are emo-
‘‘cheats’’. tional intelligent and Machiavellian might take advantage of coop-
In the condition where the confederate always cooperated, par- erative and credulous people (e.g., Côté et al., 2011).
ticipants could also earn more points by competing instead of In the condition where the confederate played tit-for-tat, PDG
cooperating. In this condition, however, high-EI participants did scores are not directly proportional to the number of times that
not score higher than their low-EI counterparts, indicating that participants chose to cooperate or compete. Here, the results of
they were not more likely to compete. In other words, they did the game depend on complex series of reactions and counter-reac-
not take advantage of someone who was consistently cooperative. tions. Overall, however, high-EI individuals tended to do well
Previous research has suggested a linear relationship between across experimental conditions, whereas low-EI individuals did
EI and cooperation: people with higher self-reported EI tend to poorly when faced with a fiercely competitive confederate. This
cooperate more (Schutte et al., 2001). However, cooperation is suggests that emotionally intelligent individuals can adapt to and
not always the best strategy. Adapting one’s behavior in response hold their ground in different contexts, adjusting their response
to others’ is arguably a better approach. The present findings show to the other person’s strategy. Interestingly, adaptability may de-
rive from the consistent use of a tit-for-tat strategy, in line with
1
We obtained similar results when we repeated the analysis without controlling the social norm of reciprocity – i.e., competing when others behave
for cognitive intelligence and tendency to cooperate. Significant main effects were competitively and cooperating when others behave cooperatively.
found for dilemma type [F(2, 224) = 10.64, p < .001, g2 = .09] and EI [F(1, 224) = 6.38,
p < .05, g2 = .03], as well as an interaction effect for dilemma type and EI
This response pattern could also be viewed as an adaptive facet of
[F(2, 224) = 9.95, p < .001, g2 = .08]. The pattern of interactions was similar to that personality, reflecting stable ‘‘if-then’’ situation-contingent re-
shown in Fig. 1. sponses (Mischel & Shoda, 1995).
24 P. Fernández-Berrocal et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 49 (2014) 21–24

Several limitations of our study should be noted. First, partici- Domino, G., & Morales, A. (2000). Reliability and validity of the D-48 with Mexican
American college students. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 22, 382–389.
pants were university students, so we must be cautious about gen-
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425,
eralizing the results to the general population. Second, we did not 785–791.
validate the D-48 test for our study population, given that we only Gough, H. G., & Domino, G. (1963). The D-48 test as a measure of general ability
collected the number of total correct responses for each partici- among grade school children. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 27, 344–349.
Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedure for probing interactions
pant. Nevertheless, the D-48 has been validated in university-age in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research
populations (Domino & Morales, 2000), so we consider it a reliable Methods, 41, 924–936.
assessment tool in this study. Third, while the PDG is one of the Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: The empathic brain
and its modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 153–158.
most frequently used paradigms for simulating social decision- Hirsh, J. B., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). Extraversion, neuroticism, and the prisoner’s
making, its ecological validity is limited insofar as it involves an dilemma. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 254–256.
artificial situation without serious personal consequences. Future Jacobs, M., Snow, J., Geraci, M., Vythilingam, M., Blair, R. J. R., Charney, D. S., et al.
(2008). Association between level of emotional intelligence and severity of
research should verify and explore our results in the context of anxiety in generalized social phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 1487–1495.
mixed-motive interpersonal negotiations with relevant conse- Jones, G. (2008). Are smarter groups more cooperative? Evidence from prisoner’s
quences, requiring both cooperation and competition. dilemma experiments, 1959–2003. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization,
68, 489–497.
Despite these limitations, the present study suggests that EI is Kee, K. S., Horan, W. P., Salovey, P., Kern, R. S., Sergi, M. J., Fiske, A. P., et al. (2009).
associated with the ability to both cooperate and compete, adjust- Emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 107, 61–68.
ing one’s response to different social contexts flexibly so as to max- Komorita, S. S., Hilty, J. A., & Parks, C. D. (1991). Reciprocity and cooperation in social
dilemmas. Journal of Conflict Management, 35, 494–518.
imize individual and joint gains. This also implicates EI in the
Lopes, P. N., Brackett, M. A., Nezlek, J., Schütz, A., Sellin, I., & Salovey, P. (2004).
ability to anticipate others’ decisions and emotions during deci- Emotional intelligence and social interaction. Personality and Social Psychology
sion-making in social contexts. Bulletin, 30, 1018–1034.
Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., & Straus, R. (2003). Emotional intelligence, personality and
the perceived quality of social relationships. Personality and Individual
Acknowledgments Differences, 35, 641–658.
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. G. (2008). Human abilities: Emotional
This research was supported in part by projects SEJ-07325 and intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507–536.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional
PSI2012-37490 (Spain). intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion, 3, 97–105.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional
Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) user’s manual. Toronto, ON: MHS.
Appendix A. Supplementary material
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey & D.
J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in implications (pp. 3–31). New York: Basic Books.
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality:
the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.12.005.
Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in
personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268.
References Nelissen, R. M. A., Dijker, A. J., & De Vries, N. H. (2007). How to turn a hawk into a
dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some
Aguirre, F., Sergi, M. J., & Levy, C. A. (2008). Emotional intelligence and social dilemma game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 280–286.
functioning in persons with schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, 104, 255–264. Reis, D. L., Brackett, M. A., Shamosh, N. A., Kiehl, K. A., Salovey, P., & Gray, J. R. (2007).
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books. Emotional intelligence predicts individual differences in social exchange
Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., & Salovey, P. (2006). Relating reasoning. NeuroImage, 35, 1385–1391.
emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and Schlegel, K., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2013). Constructs of social and
performance measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and emotional effectiveness: Different labels, same content? Journal of Research in
Social Psychology, 91, 780–795. Personality, 47, 249–253.
Brackett, M. A., Warner, R. M., & Bosco, J. (2005). Emotional intelligence and Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J., Bobik, C., Coston, T., Greeson, C., Jedlicka, C., et al. (2001).
relationship quality among couples. Personal Relationships, 12, 197–212. Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. Journal of Social Psychology,
Côté, S., DeCelles, K. A., McCarthy, J. M., Van Kleef, G. A., & Hideg, I. (2011). The Jekyll 141, 523–536.
and Hyde of emotional intelligence: Emotion-regulation knowledge facilitates Tamir, M., & Ford, B. Q. (2012). When feeling bad is expected to be good: Emotion
prosocial and interpersonally deviant behavior. Psychological Science, 22, regulation and outcome expectancies in social conflicts. Emotion, 12, 807–816.
1073–1080.

También podría gustarte